Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 April 2023
Sec. Perception Science

Literacy acquisition facilitates inversion effects for faces with full-, low-, and high-spatial frequency: evidence from illiterate and literate adults

\r\nQi Yang,*&#x;Qi Yang1,2*†Lina Zhang&#x;Lina Zhang2†Changming ChenChangming Chen3Xiaohua Cao,,*Xiaohua Cao2,4,5*
  • 1School of Humanities, Tongji University, Shanghai, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China
  • 3School of Educational Sciences, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
  • 4Key Laboratory of Intelligent Education Technology and Application of Zhejiang Province, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China
  • 5Zhejiang Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory for the Mental Health and Crisis Intervention of Children and Adolescents, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China

Previous studies have found that literacy acquisition modulates configural face processing (i.e., holistic and second-order configural processing). However, it remains unclear how literacy acquisition impacts the configural processing indexed by the inversion effect of normal or filtered faces. We asked Chinese illiterate and literate adults to judge whether two sequentially-presented stimuli, including faces, houses (experiment 1), and high- or low-pass filtered faces (experiment 2) were identical. Literate adults outperformed illiterate controls in the upright face and house conditions (experiment 1) and the upright high- and low-pass filtered conditions (experiment 2) but not in the inverted conditions. Notably, the size of an inversion effect (i.e., subtracting inverted accuracy from upright accuracy) was greater among literate adults than that among illiterate adults in both experiments. These findings support that literacy acquisition promotes configural face processing.

1. Introduction

Learning to read is a critical milestone in human development. The plasticity of the human brain lays a foundation for which learning to read reshapes the structure and function of the brain organization in processing linguistic materials (such as words) and non-linguistic materials (such as faces) (Dehaene et al., 2015). This study centers on the impact of literacy acquisition on configural face processing, which refers to any phenomenon involving perceiving relations among facial features (Farah et al., 1998).

A recent neural recycling hypothesis claims that learning to read is a great invention over evolutionary history. However, this history is too short for neural areas to have evolved specifically for word processing. To this end, literacy acquisition needs to “invade” other neural areas, typically ones that subserve face and object processing (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007; Dehaene et al., 2010). In line with the theoretical framework, a growing body of empirical studies has supported the association between literacy and face processing (Dehaene et al., 2015; Feng et al., 2022). For example, the left visual word form area (VWFA), an occipitotemporal temporal region preferentially responsive to visual words, is less responsive to faces for literate adults as opposed to illiterate adults (Dehaene et al., 2010). Neural activation of faces in VWFA decreases gradually as the knowledge of letters increases among young children (Cantlon et al., 2010). Early exposure to reading also delays face lateralization indexed by electrophysiological signals in the occipitotemporal areas roughly corresponding to the VWFA in Chinese (Li et al., 2013) and American children (Dundas et al., 2013, 2014). In addition, dyslexic readers show poor performance in both word and face processing (Jozranjbar et al., 2021) and reduce hemispheric lateralization to words and faces compared to normal readers (Gabay et al., 2017). Face recognition impairments are found to be more severe in the bilateral than the unilateral temporo-occipital cortex (Gainotti and Marra, 2011). A left occipital arteriovenous malformation results in pure alexia and prosopagnosia (Liu et al., 2011). These findings suggest that both distinct and overlapping neurocognitive computations subserve the processing of faces and words; and there are both cooperative and competitive interactions regarding the development of visual representations for these two categories (Plaut and Behrmann, 2011).

Despite that prior studies have demonstrated a link between literacy acquisition and face processing, few studies have investigated how literate and illiterate populations may undergo different configural face processing, which is a specific aspect of face processing (Ventura et al., 2013; Cao et al., 2019; Rossion and Lochy, 2021; Yang et al., 2021). The configural face processing is usually measured by different experimental paradigms, such as the face inversion paradigm, the composite face paradigm, and the spatial configuration paradigm (Maurer et al., 2002). Furthermore, despite the limited literature that did explore the relationship between literacy acquisition and configural face processing with the composite face paradigm and spatial configuration paradigm, different experimental paradigms tell different stories regarding cognitive mechanisms underlying configural face processing (Maurer et al., 2002; Richler et al., 2012). Therefore, varying paradigms may be necessarily adopted to understand how literacy affects configural face processing. We explain the differences among the aforementioned paradigms for configural face processing in detail below. The face inversion paradigm taps the first-order configural processing. The features of upright faces are arranged with two eyes above a nose, which is above a mouth (the first-order relationship). The inverted presentation disrupts the first-order configural information (Yin, 1969). The composite paradigm taps the holistic processing, gluing the features into a gestalt, and reflects the failure of selective attention to facial parts (Gauthier and Bukach, 2007). The spatial configuration paradigm instructs the observers to perceive the spatial distances among facial features, which measures the sensitivity to the second-order configural relations (Maurer et al., 2002; Cao et al., 2019).

Recent empirical findings seemed to support the hypothesized link between literacy and configural face processing. For example, with the composite face paradigm, Ventura et al. (2013) found that illiterate adults processed faces and houses more holistically than their literate counterparts. Recently, combining the spatial configuration and the inverted face paradigms, Cao et al. (2019) found that the illiterate were less sensitive to changes in the spatial arrangement among facial features in upright conditions than the literate, but showed comparable spatial sensitivity in inverted conditions to the literate. These findings suggested that literacy acquisition could reshape configural face processing.

However, unlike the first two experimental paradigms, the face inversion paradigm has not been widely used to investigate the literacy effect on configural face processing. Moreover, Cao et al. (2019) concentrated on the spatial configuration of faces and presented two faces or houses simultaneously. Many studies have demonstrated that a stimulus’s presentation type (i.e., simultaneous vs. sequential) could generate distinct cognitive processes or processing strategies (Hole, 1994; Richler et al., 2009; Li and Cao, 2017). For instance, the simultaneous presentation may pose less memory load for observers than the sequential presentation and encourage participants to utilize part-based, feature-matching strategies or image-matching strategies (Hole, 1994; Richler et al., 2009), while sequential presentation may promote participants to deal with faces holistically, as well as reduce the participants’ tendency to compare the local features of faces (Yang and Schwaninger, 2010). Thus, the first goal of the present research is to investigate whether the literacy effect on face inversion effect could still be observed when paired faces are sequentially presented.

Furthermore, previous literature has investigated the literacy effect on configural face processing with full-spatial-frequency faces. Spatial frequency refers to several cycles per degree of visual angle or several cycles per image (Morrison and Schyns, 2001), which is an essential determinant of visual processing. High spatial frequency is generally linked with featural and local information, whereas low spatial frequency is with coarse and global information (Sergent, 1986; Boutet et al., 2003; Cheung et al., 2008). Previous studies debated whether only low-spatial frequencies or both low- and high-spatial frequencies subserve configural face processing (Goffaux and Rossion, 2006; Cheung et al., 2008). For instance, Goffaux and Rossion (2006) found that the holistic face processing was driven by the low spatial frequency not by the high spatial frequency in the partial composite face paradigm, while Cheung et al. (2008) showed that holistic face processing was supported by both low spatial frequency and high spatial frequency in the complete composite face paradigm. In the composite face paradigm, participants are required to attend the top (or bottom) half of a composite face (relevant part) while ignoring the bottom (or top) half (irrelevant part) and judge whether the relevant half of the study face is identical to that of the test face. In the partial composite paradigm, the irrelevant parts of both study and test faces are always different, while in the complete paradigm, the irrelevant parts of both study and test faces are either same or different. Moreover, a previous study demonstrated that, relative to poor readers, good readers showed enhanced sensitivity to low-spatial frequencies ranging from 2 to 6 cyc/deg, and no differences were observed in sensitivity to high-spatial-frequencies among poor and good readers ranging from 64 to 128 cyc/deg (Patching and Jordan, 2005). Converging these findings, we thus want to ask whether literacy acquisition could drive individuals to adopt certain spatial frequencies to support configural face processing. To the best of our knowledge, it remains unexplored how spatial frequency modulates the literacy effect on configural face processing. Therefore, the second purpose of our research is to investigate whether the literacy effect on face inversion processing could also be observed in those faces with a specific range of spatial frequencies preserved.

To sum up, we ran two experiments to further the understanding of the literacy effect on configural face processing. We compared face and house processing in the first experiment between literate and illiterate adults. According to the neural recycling hypothesis, one main prediction is that the literate and illiterate should demonstrate differential patterns in the face inversion effect. In particular, based on a previous study (Cao et al., 2019), we predict that literate adults would show a more significant recognition performance than illiterate adults in upright rather than inverted faces and even houses when these stimuli are presented sequentially. In the second experiment, we want to further explore whether literacy acquisition improved face inversion processing in distinct spatial frequencies. If in line with the findings from Cheung et al. (2008), the present study should hypothesize that literacy acquisition enhances the configural processing of faces in both spatial frequencies. If in line with the findings from Goffaux and Rossion’s and Patching and Jordan’s study (Patching and Jordan, 2005; Goffaux and Rossion, 2006), the present study should expect that the literacy effect could only be found in the low-spatial-frequency condition. More importantly, a larger face inversion effect size, indexed by subtracting inverted accuracy from upright accuracy or by subtracting upright response time from inverted response time, is expected for literates than for illiterates in both experiments.

2. Experiment 1

2.1. Participants

We recruited 15 illiterate (14 female) and fifteen literate adults (14 female) from the same rural village in central China. Illiterate adults had an average age of 64.40 years old (range: 53−77 years; SD = 6.50 years), and the average age of literate adults was 61.00 years old (range: 56−68 years; SD = 3.35 years). The G-Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007) was used to assess the required sample sizes based on a prior study (Cao et al., 2019), in which the effect size (ηp2 = 0.21) of interaction between Literacy and Orientation was applied to the present experiment. A power analysis showed that a sample size of 5 in one group would be required at the 0.05 alpha level with 90% power. Thus, we had enough sample size to detect this effect size. The illiterate adults were identified by the Grade one literacy questionnaire (Wang and Tao, 1993), the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, Wu et al., 2002), and the Word-color Stroop task. These illiterates were those who could not recognize Chinese characters in the one, two, and three lists or less than five Chinese characters in succession in the Grade One literacy questionnaire (Wang and Tao, 1993; Cao et al., 2019), and could pass the test of MMSE (the score is above 14). We used the scores of MMSE to ensure that none of the participants experienced cognitive impairment, and used the scores of the Word-color Stroop task to identify that the illiterate participants indeed could not recognize common Chinese characters. These critical cognitive abilities measured in these tasks are considered core consequences of literacy acquisition, which has been used to distinguish between illiterate and literate individuals (Cao et al., 2019). All of them were orally reported as right-handed, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the participants has a history of mental illness or cognitive impairment. All participants and their relatives received verbal or written informed consent, and the ethical committee at Zhejiang Normal University approved the study.

2.2. Stimuli

The original materials were twenty Chinese adults’ upright face pictures with neutral expressions (half of them were male faces) (Cao et al., 2016, 2019; Zhang et al., 2021). Contour features, such as hair and ears, were cropped out using Photoshop (PS), and the isolated face pictures were put against a neutral gray background. Twenty upright houses were used as control materials, taken from the local village where participants lived (Cao et al., 2019). The production process of the upside house pictures was the same as that of the face pictures. The lightness and brightness of the pictures were generally identical. We used the software Adobe Photoshop CS5 (San Jose, CA) to adjust the lightness and brightness of visual stimuli and kept two types of visual stimuli similar in lightness and brightness (houses: 124 ± 19.58 cd/m2; full-spatial frequencies faces: 147 ± 1.84 cd/m2). We rotated the upright face/house pictures 180 degrees to make the inverted face/house pictures (see Figure 1). The visual angle of each picture is 4.0° × 6.2°. The experiment was carried out on Lenovo’s 14 inches G470 laptop with a screen resolution of 1024 × 768 (Zhang et al., 2018). The experiment was run based on the E-prime 1 software1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Panels (A,B) are examples of the upright and inverted face and house stimuli in Exp. 1.

2.3. Procedure

To begin with, a 500 ms black fixation cross appeared against a white background at the center of the screen, followed by a study stimulus for 600 ms. Then, a blank gray screen was displayed for 1,000 ms, followed by a test stimulus, which was displayed in the same orientation as the study stimulus. The test stimulus would disappear as soon as the participant initiated the response (see Figure 2). The task of the participants is to judge whether the study stimulus and the test stimulus were identical as quickly and accurately as possible. Half of the participants pressed “A” for “same” or “L” for “different”; for the other half, the key response was counterbalanced. The inter-trial interval was 1,000 ms. Participants completed 16 practice trials before the formal experiment. Only when the accuracy of the practice procedure was higher than 75% could they be allowed to start the formal experiment. Otherwise, they needed to complete the practice procedure again and again. The formal experiment consisted of four blocks with a total of one hundred and sixty trials, and each block had twenty trials for each condition (i.e., upright and inverted conditions).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The example of the upright-face conditions for one trial. Participants are required to judge whether the study face and the following test face are identical.

2.4. Data analysis

We used SPSS 22.0 software2 to analyze the accuracy and response time with a 2 (Literacy: illiterate vs. literate) × 2 (Stimulus Category: face vs. house) × 2 (Orientation: upright vs. inverted) mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA). In this model, we took Stimulus Category and Orientation as within-subject factors and Literacy as the between-subjects factor. We also performed independent sample t-tests to examine the difference in the size of the face inversion effect between literate and illiterate groups when the interaction of Literacy and Orientation was significant. We deleted 1.2% of trials accounting for all correct trials whose response times were less than 300 ms or longer than 3,000 ms (Cao et al., 2019).

2.5. Results

2.5.1. Basic information

The ages were matched, t(28) = −1.57, p = 0.140, d = 0.59, but the years of education differed significantly between both groups, t(14.57) = 12.17, p < 0.001, d = 4.60 (Mliterate = 8.67 ± 0.69 years vs. Milliterate = 0.21 ± 0.10 years).

2.5.2. Analysis of accuracy

A repeated measures ANOVA showed the main effects of Stimulus Category with higher accuracy for houses (0.94) than faces (0.77), F(1, 28) = 122.34, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.81, Orientation with higher accuracy for upright (0.89) than inverted condition (0.82), F(1, 28) = 41.11, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.60, and Literacy with higher accuracy for literates (0.88) than illiterates (0.83), F(1, 28) = 6.21, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.18.

The analysis also found that Literacy interacted with Orientation, F(1, 28) = 4.35, p = 0.046, ηp2 = 0.13. The simple effect analysis revealed that accuracy was higher for literate (0.92) than illiterate adults (0.86) in upright condition, t(28) = 3.61, p = 0.001, d = 1.36, and that accuracy was comparable between illiterates (0.81) and literates (0.83) in inverted condition, t(28) = −0.92, p = 0.365, d = 0.35. Another direction of this interaction showed that accuracy was higher in upright than inverted conditions for literates, t(14) = 4.61, p < 0.001, d = 2.46, and illiterates, t(14) = 5.58, p < 0.001, d = 2.98, separately (see Figure 3). Independent-sample t-test demonstrated a larger size of inversion effect in literate adults than illiterate controls, t(28) = 2.09, p = 0.046, d = 0.79.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Bar plot with mean accuracy and standard errors as a function of Literacy and Orientation in experiment 1 (left) panel and in experiment 2 (right) panel. Error bars indicated standard errors. * Indicates p < 0.05, and *** indicates p < 001.

The interaction of Stimulus Category by Literacy was also significant, F(1, 28) = 8.50, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.23. Separate analyses for the two stimuli revealed that as for faces, accuracy was lower for illiterates (0.73) than for literates (0.81), t(28) = −3.15, p = 0.004, d = 1.19, but as for houses, there was similar performance between illiterates (0.94) and literates (0.94), t(28) = 0.05, p = 0.961, d = 0.02.

The analysis also yielded an interaction between Stimuli Category and Orientation, F(1, 28) = 38.17, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.58, with higher accuracy of upright (0.83) than inverted faces (0.71), t(29) = 7.14, p < 0.001, d = 2.65), and with similar performances between upright (0.94 ± 0.04) and inverted houses (0.93), t(29) = 1.19, p = 0.244, d = 0.44. We found an insignificant three-way interaction, F(1, 28) = 0.28, p = 0.601, ηp2 = 0.01.

2.5.3. Analysis of response time

A repeated measures ANOVA showed the main effects of Stimulus Category with an overall faster response of houses (778 ms) than faces (956 ms), F(1, 28) = 42.03, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.60, and Orientation with the quicker response speed of upright (849 ms) than inverted stimuli (885 ms), F(1, 28) = 14.16, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34.

The main effect of Literacy was not significant, F(1, 28) = 2.64, p = 0.115, ηp2 = 0.09. In addition, neither two-way interactions (Stimulus Category by Orientation, F(1, 28) = 0.95, p = 0.338, ηp2 = 0.03; Stimulus Category by Literacy, F(1, 28) = 3.41, p = 0.075, ηp2 = 0.11; Orientation by Literacy, F(1, 28) = 0.02, p = 0.891, ηp2 < 0.01) nor three-way interaction, F(1, 28) = 0.30, p = 0.590, ηp2 = 0.01, was found.

The present experiment revealed that recognition accuracy was higher for literates than for illiterates in upright rather than inverted faces, consistent with the previous study (Cao et al., 2019, experiment 1). The effect also extends to house processing, similar to previous findings (Cao et al., 2019, experiment 2). Moreover, the present study also revealed that literate adults showed a larger inversion effect size than illiterate adults. In sum, the experiment provided insights into the close link between literacy and non-word processing. To gain a reliable literacy effect on configural face processing, we tried to extend the effect to these faces filtered with distinct spatial frequencies in experiment 2.

3. Experiment 2

3.1. Participants

We recruited eleven illiterate (ten female) and eleven literate adults (11 female) from a rural area of central China. Note that eight illiterate adults (one male) and all literate adults participated in Experiment 1 because illiterate adults were not readily available in the region where the study was conducted. Illiterate adults had an average age of 63.27 years old (range: 53−69 years; SD = 4.96 years), and the average age of literate adults was 61.00 years old (range: 56−68 years; SD = 3.90 years). The G-Power 3.1 was used to assess the required sample sizes in experiment 2. In this analysis, the mean effect size (ηp2 = 0.17) of Cao et al. and experiment 1 in the present study was adopted to calculate the sample size, which demonstrated that a sample size of six in one group would be required at 0.05 alpha level with 90% power. Thus, the sample size used in the present experiment is necessary to detect this effect size. All of them were right-handed native Chinese with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants and relatives received verbal or written informed consent, and the ethical committee at Zhejiang Normal University approved the study. The screening of subjects was the same as in experiment 1.

3.2. Stimuli

The face stimuli of experiment 2 are all from experiment 1. MATLAB software3 was used to filter all the face materials with low-spatial frequency (2−8 cycles per face) and high-spatial frequency (32−128 cycles per face) (Goffaux and Rossion, 2006; Cheung et al., 2008). Figure 4 shows an exemplar stimulus for each condition.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Panel (A) is an example of a pair of upright and inverted faces in high spatial frequency conditions. Panel (B) shows the example of a pair of upright and inverted faces in low spatial frequency conditions.

3.3. Procedure

The experiment procedure is the same as experiment 1.

3.4. Data analysis

We used SPSS 22.0 software (see text footnote 2) to analyze the accuracy and response time with a 2 (Literacy: literate vs. illiterate) × 2 (Spatial Frequency: high vs. low) × 2 (Orientation: upright vs. inverted) ANOVA. In this model, we took Spatial Frequency and Orientation as within-subjects factors and Literacy as the between-subjects factor. In addition, we performed independent sample t-tests to examine differences in the size of the face inversion effect between literate and illiterate groups when the interaction of Literacy and Orientation was significant. We deleted 5.1% of trials accounting for all correct trials whose response times were less than 300 ms or longer than 3,000 ms.

3.5. Results

3.5.1. Basic information

The average ages were matched, t(20) = −1.50, p = 0.166, d = 0.90, but the average years of education differed between both groups, t(10) = 10.06, p < 0.001, d = 4.60 (Mliterate = 8.45 ± 2.69 years vs. Milliterate = 0.18 ± 0.41 years).

3.5.2. Analysis of accuracy

A repeated measures ANOVA revealed the main effects of Spatial Frequency with higher accuracy for low-spatial-frequency condition (0.74) than high-spatial-frequency condition (0.66), F(1, 20) = 21.50, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.52, Orientation with higher accuracy for upright (0.73) than inverted conditions (0.66), F(1, 20) = 34.96, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64, and Literacy with higher accuracy for literates (0.73) than illiterates (0.67), F(1, 20) = 10.19, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.34.

There was only an interaction between Literacy and Orientation, F(1, 20) = 6.68, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.25. Separate analyses for the two orientations revealed that there was a significant distinction between illiterates (0.69) and literates (0.78) for upright faces, t(20) = -4.00, p = 0.001, d = 1.79, but not between illiterates (0.65) and literates (0.68) for inverted faces, t(20) = −1.40, p = 0.177, d = 0.63. Another direction of this interaction showed that the accuracy was higher for the upright condition than for the inverted condition for both groups (literates: t(10) = 6.04, p < 0.001, d = 3.82; illiterates: t(10) = 2.34, p = 0.041, d = 1.48) (see Figure 3). The independent sample t-test showed that the inversion effect size was larger in literate adults than in illiterate controls, t(20) = 2.59, p = 0.018, d = 1.16.

We found neither the interaction between Spatial Frequency and Orientation, F(1, 20) = 2.54, p = 0.127, ηp2 = 0.11, nor the interplay between Literacy and Spatial Frequency, F(1, 20) = 0.02, p = 0.877, ηp2 = 0.001. Also, there was no significant triple interaction among Literacy, Orientation, and Spatial Frequency, F(1, 20) = 0.12, p = 0.737, ηp2 = 0.01.

3.5.3. Analysis of response time

The analysis revealed the main effect of Spatial Frequency with a quicker response speed of low-spatial-frequency faces (964 ms) than high-spatial-frequency faces (1,161 ms), F(1, 20) = 12.27, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.38. Other main effects were not significant.

There was only one interaction between Literacy and Spatial Frequency, F(1, 20) = 6.46, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.24. Separate analyses for the two kinds of Spatial Frequency revealed that the difference between illiterates (1,027 ms) and literates (1,295 ms) was marginally significant for high-spatial-frequency faces, t(20) = −2.02, p = 0.057, d = 0.90, but significant distinction between illiterates (973 ms) and literates (956 ms) for low-spatial-frequency faces was not reached, t(20) = 0.177, p = 0.861, d = 0.08.

We revealed neither the interaction between Spatial Frequency and Orientation, F(1, 20) = 0.46, p = 0.507, ηp2 = 0.02, nor the interplay of Literacy and Orientation, F(1, 20) = 0.97, p = 0.336, ηp2 = 0.05. The three-way interaction of Literacy by Orientation by Spatial Frequency was not significant, F(1, 20) < 0.01, p = 0.957, ηp2 < 0.01.

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to examine the literacy effect on the first-order configural face processing and conducted two experiments to compare the face recognition performance and the size of the inversion effect between literate and illiterate adults when these stimuli were presented consecutively. In experiment 1, we found that illiterate adults showed lower accuracy than literate controls for upright faces but comparable accuracy for inverted faces. This literacy effect also extended to house processing. In experiment 2, we also observed a two-way interaction between Literacy and Orientation, similar to experiment 1. However, high-and low-spatial frequencies did not modulate the interaction between Literacy and Orientation in face processing. In addition, the illiterate responded faster to faces with high spatial frequency, rather than faces with low spatial frequency, as compared with the literate. Notably, both experiments showed that the inversion effect size was more prominent in literate than illiterate adults. These findings further highlighted the link between literacy acquisition and configural face processing.

4.1. Literacy acquisition promotes the first-order configural processing

Consistent with the neural recycling hypothesis (Dehaene and Cohen, 2007; Dehaene et al., 2010), which claims that literacy acquisition “invades” these cortexes typically for processing faces or other objects, the present study showed a larger inversion effect for faces and houses in literate adults compared with illiterate control. Moreover, the literate performed better in recognizing upright faces and houses presented sequentially than the illiterate. The present study, combined with a prior study in which paired faces were presented simultaneously (Cao et al., 2019), suggested that the way of stimuli presentation (simultaneous vs. sequential) seems not to change the outcome of literacy facilitation effect on face inversion effect. However, these two presentations may tax different cognitive loads (Menon et al., 2015) and require distinct processing strategies (Hole, 1994; Richler et al., 2009). The previous literature demonstrated that simultaneous presentation required less cognitive effort compared with sequential presentation; simultaneous presentation facilitated participants to process faces holistically, and even decreased the preference for the use of the local features (Yang and Schwaninger, 2010), while participants in the sequential presentation may employ memory-based-implicating strategies or the outcome-of-an-attention strategies. In sum, the findings suggested that the literacy effect on the inversion effect for faces was stable.

In addition, the literacy effect on face processing also extended to non-facial stimuli (i.e., house) processing, which suggested that the literacy effect might be domain-general, supporting the neural recycling hypothesis. More precisely, these original cortexes that learning to read “invades” may subserve domain-general cognitive ability.

4.2. Literacy acquisition may attenuate response speed to high spatial frequency

In the present study, we also found partial evidence that the illiterate participants reacted faster to faces with high spatial frequency than the literate participants, while a comparable response speed on faces with low spatial frequency was observed between illiterate and literate adults. The findings seem to suggest that literacy acquisition attenuated the processing speed to the facial information conveyed by the high spatial frequency. In other words, the illiterate seem to be inclined to employ the featural information to process faces as compared with the literate, given that the high spatial frequency transmits more features or local information. These findings were not inconsistent with the view that literacy acquisition improved configural face processing (such as Cao et al., 2019). Additionally, Patching and Jordan (2005) found that good readers showed higher sensitivity to low spatial frequency as compared with poor readers. Although poor readers and illiterate adults had similarities in terms of reading, they were essentially different groups. To sum up, combined with these studies mentioned above, the present study provided insights into the complex relationship between literacy and the sensitivity to distinct spatial frequencies.

Finally, we found that the literacy effect on face processing was not modulated by spatial frequency. This is consistent with previous research, which shows that low and high spatial frequency evoked holistic face processing in a complete composite face paradigm (Cheung et al., 2008). These studies suggested that literacy acquisition seemed not to drive different sensitivities to spatial frequency to support configural face processing.

4.3. Two possibilities concerning the literacy effect on configural face processing

The two experiments in the present study both showed the facilitated face inversion effect of literacy, (partially) consistent with the findings of Cao et al. (2019) and Ventura et al. (2013). Cao et al. (2019) used the spatial configuration task and showed that illiterate adults were less sensitive to the relationships between facial features than literate controls in upright rather than inverted faces. However, Ventura et al. (2013) found that literacy acquisition decreased the holistic processing indexed by the composite face effects. Two possibilities could account for the inconsistent result patterns between Ventura et al. (2013) and the present study. One obvious possibility may be associated with the differences in these experimental paradigms (composite face paradigm and face inversion paradigm). The composite face paradigm reflects the failure of selective attention to facial parts, whereas the face inversion paradigm demonstrates the disruption of the typical arrangement of facial features. Different experimental paradigms could tap into the different dimensions of configural face processing (Maurer et al., 2002; Richler et al., 2012), given the complex relationships among different paradigms (Teunisse and De Gelder, 2003; Wang et al., 2012; DeGutis et al., 2013; Rezlescu et al., 2017). Therefore. future studies are encouraged to perform a face inversion task among western literate and illiterate adults or to conduct a composite face task in Chinese illiterates and literates.

An alternative possibility is an attention-shaped-by-language hypothesis (Yang et al., 2021), which suggests that language shapes how its user deploys the attentional resources to the visual processing of words, given the extensive and longstanding experience with the visual features of words. In the current case, literates acquired Portuguese scripts in Ventura et al.’s (2013) study, whereas literate adults acquired Chinese scripts in the present study and Cao et al.’s (2019)study. Chinese characters may be special cases as they possess a highly non-linear visual complexity of shape (Huang et al., 2004), while Portuguese words are composed of basic letters and are linear. Long-term and extensive exposure to the differences in visual features between Chinese and Portuguese scripts may induce distinct processing demands for Chinese compared to Portuguese scripts and further transfer to the processing of configural faces. Recently, Ma et al. (2022) showed that N170 amplitudes elicited by naturalistic and Mooney faces were larger for Chinese children than for German children, which suggested superior holistic processing in Chinese children. Using the Framed-Line Test in Portugal and Thailand, Ventura et al. (2008) found that Portuguese literates showed higher accuracy in the absolute task (reflecting the analytic processing) compared to the relative task (reflecting the holistic processing). However, Portuguese illiterates and ex-illiterates showed higher accuracy in the relative task than in the absolute task. However, all Thai groups showed consistently higher accuracy on the relative task than on the absolute task. The attention-shaped-by-language hypothesis seems to account for the domain-general mechanism underlying the literacy effect on object inversion processing (such as faces and houses). In a word, these findings demonstrated that schooling/literacy imposed different impacts on the participants with distinct script systems. Future studies may find it worthwhile to explore new experimental designs to examine the attention-shaped-by-language hypothesis.

4.4. Implications and limitations

The current data adds ingredients to the theoretical development of the relations between literacy and face processing by examining the first-order configural processing (i.e., face inversion effect) between Chinese literate and illiterate adults. Following the neural recycling hypothesis, previous studies mainly centered on the correlations between face processing and word processing, or between face processing and the amount of literacy. Recently, few studies (including the present study) with different paradigms of configural face processing shed light on how literacy acquisition affects configural face processing, particularly in different script systems (Ventura et al., 2013; Cao et al., 2019). Combined with the existing findings, the present study also proposed that the attention-shaped-by-language hypothesis could account for the cognitive mechanisms underlying literacy acquisition on configural face processing. Moreover, the present study also has some implications for further studies, which should be conducted by recruiting those literate and illiterate adults trained with different script systems.

Some limitations may be considered in the present study. First, socialization may reduce the credibility of making causal inferences. Participants in both groups were matched as closely as possible on age and gender, but they may differ in the amount of their exposure to faces. For instance, the literate may experience more distinct faces than the illiterate when attending regular school (Cao et al., 2019). They have more access to a broader range of people after acquiring literacy. The experience of faces may lead to differential effects on configural face processing (Balas and Saville, 2015).

Second, previous studies demonstrated that females performed better face perception and cognition than males (Sommer et al., 2013). However, in the present study, almost illiterate adults were female (only one male) because males had more opportunities to attend school in China in the 20th century. Therefore, we shall be cautious to generalize the current findings to male illiterate adults, or other populations.

5. Conclusion

The present study concluded that the literate promoted face recognition when upright faces were presented. The effect also extends to different stimuli (i.e., houses) and faces with distinct spatial frequencies. These findings shed insight on the close association between literacy acquisition and configural face processing.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Zhejiang Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

QY, CC, and XC: conceptualization. QY and LZ: data curation, formal analysis, and visualization. XC: funding acquisition and project administration. QY: investigation. QY and XC: methodology. QY, LZ, and CC: roles/writing—original draft. All authors: writing—review and editing.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 21FYYB051).

Acknowledgments

We thank to all participants for participating in the present study and Haidan Duan for assistance with data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1061232/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://pstnet.com/
  2. ^ https://www.ibm.com/
  3. ^ https://www.mathworks.com/

References

Balas, B., and Saville, A. (2015). N170 face specificity and face memory depend on hometown size. Neuropsychologia 69, 211–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutet, I., Collin, C., and Faubert, J. (2003). Configural face encoding and spatial frequency information. Percept. Psychophys. 65, 1078–1093. doi: 10.3758/BF03194835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantlon, J. F., Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., and Pelphrey, K. A. (2010). Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. Cereb. Cortex 21, 191–199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., Yang, Q., and Hu, F. (2016). Eyeglasses elicit effects similar to face-like perceptual expertise: Evidence from the N170 response. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 883–891. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4525-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., Yang, Q., Zhong, P., and Chen, C. (2019). The characteristics of face configural effect in illiterates and literates. Acta Psychol. 201:102951. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, O. S., Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., and Gauthier, I. (2008). Revisiting the role of spatial frequencies in the holistic processing of faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1327–1336. doi: 10.1037/a0011752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeGutis, J., Wilmer, J., Mercado, R. J., and Cohan, S. (2013). Using regression to measure holistic face processing reveals a strong link with face recognition ability. Cognition 126, 87–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56, 384–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., and Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 234–244. doi: 10.1038/nrn3924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., et al. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330, 1359–1364. doi: 10.1126/science.1194140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dundas, E. M., Plaut, D. C., and Behrmann, M. (2013). The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 348–358. doi: 10.1037/a0029503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dundas, E. M., Plaut, D. C., and Behrmann, M. (2014). An ERP investigation of the co-development of hemispheric lateralization of face and word recognition. Neuropsychologia 61, 315–323. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., and Tanaka, J. N. (1998). What is “special” about face perception. Psychol. Rev. 105:482. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, X., Monzalvo, K., Dehaene, S., and Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2022). Evolution of reading and face circuits during the first three years of reading acquisition. Neuroimage 259:119394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabay, Y., Dundas, E., Plaut, D., and Behrmann, M. (2017). Atypical perceptual processing of faces in developmental dyslexia. Brain Lang. 173, 41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainotti, G., and Marra, C. (2011). Differential contribution of right and left temporo-occipital and anterior temporal lesions to face recognition disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, I., and Bukach, C. (2007). Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? Cognition 103, 322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffaux, V., and Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are “spatial”–holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32:1023. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.1023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hole, G. J. (1994). Configurational factors in the perception of unfamiliar faces. Perception 23, 65–74. doi: 10.1068/p230065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, K., Itoh, K., Suwazono, S., and Nakada, T. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of grapheme-phoneme conversion. Neurosci. Lett. 366, 254–258. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.05.099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jozranjbar, B., Kristjánsson, Á, and Sigurdardottir, H. M. (2021). Featural and configural processing of faces and houses in matched dyslexic and typical readers. Neuropsychologia 162:108059. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C., and Cao, X. (2017). The presentation location of the reference stimuli affects the left-side bias in the processing of faces and Chinese characters. Front. Psychol. 8:1673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., Kang, L., Zhao, J., Yang, Z., He, S., and Weng, X. (2013). Neural competition as a developmental process: Early hemispheric specialization for word processing delays specialization for face processing. Neuropsychologia 51, 950–959. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y. C., Wang, A. G., and Yen, M. Y. (2011). “Seeing but not identifying”: Pure alexia coincident with prosopagnosia in occipital arteriovenous malformation. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 249, 1087–1089. doi: 10.1007/s00417-010-1586-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., Kang, J., Li, X., Maurer, U., Cao, X., and Sommer, W. (2022). Does learning different script systems affect configural visual processing? ERP evidence from early readers of Chinese and German. Psychophysiology 59:e14006. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., and Mondloch, C. J. (2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01903-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, N., White, D., and Kemp, R. I. (2015). Variation in photos of the same face drives improvements in identity verification. Perception 44, 1332–1341. doi: 10.1177/0301006615599902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, D. J., and Schyns, P. G. (2001). Usage of spatial scales for the categorization of faces, objects, and scenes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 454–469. doi: 10.3758/BF03196180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patching, G. R., and Jordan, T. R. (2005). Spatial frequency sensitivity differences between adults of good and poor reading ability. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46, 2219–2224. doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-1247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaut, D. C., and Behrmann, M. (2011). Complementary neural representations for faces and words: A computational exploration. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 28, 251–275. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.609812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezlescu, C., Susilo, T., Wilmer, J. B., and Caramazza, A. (2017). The inversion, part-whole, and composite effects reflect distinct perceptual mechanisms with varied relationships to face recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 43, 1961–1973. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richler, J. J., Mack, M. L., Gauthier, I., and Palmeri, T. J. (2009). Holistic processing of faces happens at a glance. Vis. Res. 49, 2856–2861. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., and Gauthier, I. (2012). Meanings, mechanisms, and measures of holistic processing. Front. Psychol. 3:553. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Lochy, A. (2021). Is human face recognition lateralized to the right hemisphere due to neural competition with left-lateralized visual word recognition? A critical review. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 599–629. doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02370-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergent, J. (1986). “Microgenesis of face perception,” in Aspects of face processing, eds H. D. Ellis, M. A. Jeeves, F. Newcombe, and A. Young (Dordrecht: Springer), 17–33. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-4420-6_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommer, W., Hildebrandt, A., Kunina-Habenicht, O., Schacht, A., and Wilhelm, O. (2013). Sex differences in face cognition. Acta Psychol. 142, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teunisse, J. P., and De Gelder, B. (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects. Brain Cogn. 52, 285–294. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00042-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, P., Fernandes, T., Cohen, L., Morais, J., Kolinsky, R., and Dehaene, S. (2013). Literacy acquisition reduces the influence of automatic holistic processing of faces and houses. Neurosci. Lett. 554, 105–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.08.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, P., Pattamadilok, C., Fernandes, T., Klein, O., Morais, J., and Kolinsky, R. (2008). Schooling in western culture promotes context-free processing. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 100, 79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, R., Li, J., Fang, H., Tian, M., and Liu, J. (2012). Individual differences in holistic processing predict face recognition ability. Psychol. Sci. 23, 169–177. doi: 10.1177/0956797611420575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. L., and Tao, B. (1993). Xiaoxeusheng shizi ceshi tiku ji pingjia liangbiao [The written vocabulary test for primary school students and norm]. Shanghai: The Shanghai Education Press.

Google Scholar

Wu, C., Zhou, D., Peter, C., Fan, J. H., and Qiao, Y. L. (2002). Neuropsychological and functional study in screening Alzheimer disease in Henan province. Chin. Ment. Health J. 16, 587–589. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2022.104108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., and Schwaninger, A. (2010). Face inversion effect emerges under critical configural discrepancy. Swiss J. Psychol. 69, 161–167. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Q., Cao, X., and Jiang, X. (2021). “The role of multilingual script systems in face processing,” in Multilingualism-interdisciplinary topics, ed. X. Jiang (London: IntechOpen). doi: 10.5772/intechopen.101539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. J. Exp. Psychol. 81, 141–145. doi: 10.1037/h0027474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, G. L., Li, A. S., Miao, C. G., He, X., Zhang, M., and Zhang, Y. (2018). A consumer-grade LCD monitor for precise visual stimulation. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 1496–1502. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1018-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Yang, Q., Sommer, W., Guo, G., and Cao, X. H. (2021). The composite face effect between young and older Chinese adults remains stable. Front. Psychol. 12:743056. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.743056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: literacy acquisition, configural face processing, spatial frequency, face inversion paradigm, script system

Citation: Yang Q, Zhang L, Chen C and Cao X (2023) Literacy acquisition facilitates inversion effects for faces with full-, low-, and high-spatial frequency: evidence from illiterate and literate adults. Front. Psychol. 14:1061232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1061232

Received: 04 October 2022; Accepted: 14 March 2023;
Published: 24 April 2023.

Edited by:

Yang Zhang, Soochow University, China

Reviewed by:

Valentinos Zachariou, University of Kentucky, United States
Guomei Zhou, Sun Yat-sen University, China
Jing Zhao, Hangzhou Normal University, China

Copyright © 2023 Yang, Zhang, Chen and Cao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qi Yang, yangqpsy@163.com; Xiaohua Cao, caoxh@zjnu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.