Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 September 2022
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Community Series: Adaptation to Psychological Stress in Sport – Volume II View all 7 articles

“I must be perfect”: The role of irrational beliefs and perfectionism on the competitive anxiety of Hungarian athletes

  • 1School of Doctoral Studies, Hungarian University of Sports Science, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Faculty of Health and Education, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, United Kingdom
  • 3FTC Handball Academy, Budapest, Hungary
  • 4Department of Psychology and Sport Psychology, Hungarian University of Sports Science, Budapest, Hungary

In this study the influence of irrational beliefs and perfectionism on the emergence of competitive anxiety was investigated. While previous studies indicate that higher irrational beliefs predict greater competitive anxiety, in the present study it is hypothesized that this relationship is mediated by perfectionism. A serial atemporal multiple mediation analysis revealed that both adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism were significant partial mediators between irrational beliefs and competitive anxiety. The total score and all four subscale scores on irrational beliefs had both direct and indirect effects on cognitive competitive anxiety, the latter effects mediated by both forms of perfectionism. Depreciation beliefs had a direct effect, and demandingness and awfulizing had indirect effects, on somatic competitive anxiety when both forms of perfectionism were entered as mediators. These findings suggest that both irrational beliefs and perfectionism influence the emergence of competitive anxiety, therefore rational emotive behavioral therapy with a focus upon perfectionism may be an effective means of reducing competitive anxiety in athletes.

Introduction

Sport is a highly competitive field in which athletes are required to meet high expectations and be both physically and mentally fit to reach peak performance. More and more sport professionals recognize the importance of mental preparation in optimizing performance and maintaining mental health. For example, nineteen-time US gymnastics world champion Simone Biles withdrew from the finals of the 2020 Tokyo Olympic Games in order to focus on her wellbeing (Ingle, 2021). As three-time Olympic champion Hungarian fencer Áron Szilágyi noted shortly after winning his third Olympic title, “sport is a universal phenomenon, and I do not think there is a sport in the world where revolutionary innovations are yet to be discovered in terms of preparation, tactics or even technique. In short, they have maximized what they had, so the importance of mental preparation has increased dramatically because that is where there are still untapped advantages to be gained” (Molcsapat, 2022).

One of the most common targets in athletes’ mental screening and preparation is competitive anxiety, which is a negatively valenced emotional state that could lead to drastic performance reduction under pressure if not managed properly (Hardy, 1990), although whilst anxiety is often experienced as an unpleasant emotional state, it is not always maladaptive (Ellis and DiGiuseppe, 1993) or related to poor performance (Craft et al., 2003). Several theoretical models have emerged from studies of the relationship between sport performance and anxiety. The most popular theory is Martens et al. (1990) multidimensional anxiety model, which makes a distinction between three major factors of competitive anxiety: cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence. Cognitive anxiety involves concerns about, and negative expectations for performance, while somatic anxiety includes physical symptoms (e.g., wet palms, muscle tension, and increased heart rate). Both high and low levels of somatic anxiety may impair sport performance, since high somatic anxiety leads to the mentioned autonomic symptoms, while low somatic anxiety is associated with fatigue, boredom, lethargy, etc. While this model offers an explanation for the various symptoms of anxiety in athletes, it is still not fully clarified what psychological factors influence the emergence of competitive anxiety.

Recently, increasing attention has been focused on the benefits of cognitive behavior therapies (CBTs) in sport psychology (e.g., Turner et al., 2020a), including Ellis (1999) rational emotive behavioral therapy (REBT). The adaptation of REBT for sport-related objectives is a relatively new research area (Turner, 2016) but research and professional practice literature us growing (Jordana et al., 2020). The theoretical model undergirding REBT primarily focuses on rational (flexible, non-extreme, and logical) and irrational (rigid, extreme, illogical) beliefs. Four forms of irrational beliefs are distinguished. Dogmatic demandingness (so-called primary irrational beliefs) includes maladaptive demands on one’s own performance, illustrated by statements such as I want to be successful, and that means I have to be successful. The model makes a distinction between three types of secondary irrational beliefs such as awfulizing (If I don’t succeed, it will be terrible), low frustration tolerance (failure is unbearable to me), and depreciation (If I fail, I am a complete failure; Dryden, 1995). These Irrational beliefs have been found to be associated with trait, state, social, and performance anxiety in both clinical and non-clinical samples (Himle et al., 1982; Deffenbacher et al., 1986; Wood et al., 2018), and in specific athlete samples (e.g., Chadha et al., 2019; Turner et al., 2019a, 2022; Mansell, 2021). Browne et al. (2010) enumerated a number of unhealthy factors associated with irrational beliefs including anger, guilt and shame, and psychopathological constructs such as depression, anxiety and suicidal thoughts. A more recent meta-analysis revealed relatively high correlations between irrational beliefs and general distress, depression, anxiety, anger, and guilt (Visla et al., 2016). Also, various applied studies that utilize REBT have indicated that as irrational beliefs reduce, so too do forms of anxiety (e.g., Turner and Barker, 2013; Turner et al., 2020b; Bowman and Turner, 2022), intimating a connection between irrational beliefs and anxiety.

In REBT theory and practice, greater irrational beliefs have been linked to greater perfectionism (Flett et al., 1991; Ellis, 2002). Perfectionism involves excessively high expectations of oneself followed by critical self-evaluation of performance (Frost et al., 1990). There is irrationality nested or implied within the notion of perfectionism, because the idea that something (especially a human being, or the outcomes driven by human beings) could be perfect is an extreme and illogical idea. As fallible and ever-changing human beings, we are not capable of perfection. However, outside of REBT, perfectionism is considered to be a multidimensional personality characteristic that can be both adaptive and maladaptive (Gotwals et al., 2012). Several dimensions of perfectionism have been identified in sport contexts, which are currently divided into the two broad categories of adaptive vs. maladaptive perfectionism (strivings vs. concerns; Gotwals et al., 2012). Perfectionist strivings include one’s internal standards and drives toward perfection. Perfectionist concerns (expectations) are associated with fear of failure and negative reactions to imperfection (Stoeber and Otto, 2006).

Perfectionism has long been considered as a factor that can be detrimental to sport performance, since perfectionist expectations are potentially associated with negative cognitive, emotional and behavioral consequences (Limburg et al., 2017). However, this does not equally apply for both forms of perfectionism. Perfectionist strivings in sport have been found to be associated with positive emotions (Sagar and Stoeber, 2009), stable self-confidence, and low cognitive and somatic competitive anxiety (Stoeber et al., 2007). However, Gotwals and Tamminen (2022) could not consistently distinguish between adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism in various situational contexts, since the relevance of the distinction also depended on other psychological factors such as athletes’ susceptibility to irrational beliefs, which was associated with increased distress after negative events (failure). These findings are in line with the alternative distinction between standards and expectations (Rice and Ashby, 2007), and with the finding that adaptive perfectionism potentially enhances athletic performance, while maladaptive perfectionism is negatively associated with mental health (Hill et al., 2018). Nevertheless, the previous findings have not clarified whether perfectionism has a positive or negative influence on athletes’ mental health. As Hill et al. (2020) point out, any form of perfectionism, despite its positive effects, can pose a threat to one’s wellbeing. If an athlete is perfectionistic and holds irrational beliefs, it is surely likely that they will suffer psychological distress, including chronic or acute anxiety.

The present study explores the associations between irrational beliefs, perfectionism, and competitive anxiety in a sample of athletes. While previous findings have shown that irrational beliefs influence the emergence of competitive anxiety, studies that have examined the relationship between perfectionism (primarily strivings) and competitive anxiety have not revealed a consistently positive or negative association between the two constructs. Separately, the influence of irrational beliefs and perfectionism on competitive anxiety has been demonstrated, but the relationship between irrational beliefs and perfectionism, and their combined effect upon anxiety, has not been investigated in athletes. Hence, in the present study we explore the associations between these three variables utilizing atemporal mediational models. It is hypothesized that irrational beliefs would have significant indirect effects on cognitive and somatic competitive anxiety, mediated by adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism (standards and discrepancy, respectively). In other words, we anticipate that it is through perfectionism that irrational beliefs exert the most impact upon anxiety.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

The sample consisted of 219 Hungarian athletes (123 males, 96 females, M = 26.39), who were invited e-mail via various sports clubs to complete an online form. The sample included both recreational (N = 140) and professional athletes (N = 79) pursuing diverse individual (N = 82) and team sports (N = 137), including boxing, speed skating, soccer, ice hockey, handball, and table tennis. The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Hungarian University of Sport Science Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided.

Measures

Competitive state anxiety was measured with the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2; Martens et al., 1990; Hungarian adaptation by Sipos et al., 1999). The questionnaire contains 27-items divided into three scales assessing cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence. The Hungarian version demonstrated high internal consistency (Cronbach’s αs ranged from 0.75 to 0.85) (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, reliability, and correlation of variables (iPBI, SAPS, and CSAI-2-H).

Irrational beliefs were measured with the Irrational Performance Beliefs Inventory (iPBI; Turner and Allen, 2018). The iPBI contains 20-items composing the four subscales of demandingness (DEM), awfulizing (AWF), low frustration tolerance (LFT), and depreciation (DEP). The original instrument showed adequate validity and test-retest reliability (Turner and Allen, 2018). In the absence of a previous Hungarian adaptation, a Hungarian translation of the iPBI (e.g., Chotpitayasunondh and Turner, 2019; Nejati et al., 2022) was back-translated into English by an independent native English-speaking colleague, and the authors of the original instrument found the back-translation acceptable, thus construct equivalence was ensured. As the first table shows all subscales of the Hungarian version showed adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s αs ranged from 0.70 to 0.89).

Trait-level adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism (i.e., standards and discrepancy, respectively) were measured with the eight-item revised short Almost Perfect Scale (SAPS; Rice et al., 2014; Hungarian adaptation by Reinhardt et al., 2018). The standards subscale (four items) assesses factors related to high performance expectations, while the discrepancy subscale (four items) assesses the perceived distance from self-set standards and related concerns. The Hungarian version showed adequate reliability and validity, and no significant correlation was found between the two subscales (Reinhardt et al., 2018, 2019). The reliability of the questionnaire is also appropriate for the current sample (Cronbach’s αs ranged from 0.81 to 0.83) (see Table 1).

Statistical data analyses

All measures were first checked for normality, and all showed normal distribution. The descriptive statistics and Pearson intercorrelations of the measures were then calculated using the IBM SPSS program. The direct and indirect effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety with perfectionism competitive anxiety as a mediator were tested with serial multiple mediation (SAMM) analyses using the PROCESS macro v. 4.10 integrated into the IBM SPSS program. We ran Hayes (2022) model 6, 10 times, since we had two mediators (standards, discrepancy) in each case (see Figure 1). The independent variables (X) were the iPBI total score (irrational beliefs) and its four subscale scores, while the dependent variables (Y) were cognitive and somatic anxiety (as measured with the CSAI-2-H) and the mediators was the two form of perfectionism (adaptive and maladaptive) standards (M1) and discrepancy (M2) which measured by SAPS.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Serial atemporal multiple mediation model with two mediators. X = independent variable; Y = dependent variable; M1, M2 = mediators; a1, a2, b1, b2, d21, c, c′ = regression coefficients.

Results

Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations

The descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of the target variables are presented in Table 1. All measures of irrational beliefs correlated positively with both cognitive and somatic anxiety and with both adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Furthermore, both forms of perfectionism correlated positively with both cognitive and somatic anxiety.

Serial atemporal multiple mediation analyses

A total of 10 serial atemporal multiple mediation (SAMM) analyses were conducted to test the direct and indirect effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety (cognitive and somatic) through perfectionism (adaptive and maladaptive). The results are presented in Table 2. The total effect of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety was significant in each of the 10 mediation models.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Serial atemporal multiple mediation analysis results.

Relationships between irrational beliefs, perfectionism, and cognitive anxiety

Significant positive direct effects were obtained for demandingness (β = 0.27, 95% CI = 0.001–0.20), awfulizing (β = 0.14, 95% CI = 0.05–0.22), low frustration tolerance (β = 0.15, 95% CI = 0.04–0.26), depreciation (β = 0.23, 95% CI = 0.14–0.32), and total irrational beliefs (β = 0.27, 95% CI = 0.15–0.39) with cognitive anxiety as the outcome variable and both standards and discrepancy entered as mediators (see Table 2). Importantly, adaptive perfectionism (standards) alone did not significantly predict cognitive anxiety in any of the SAMM models (see Figure 2). The indirect effects of demandingness (β = 0.04, 95% CI = 0.1–0.07), awfulizing (β = 0.06, 95% CI = 0.03–0.10), low frustration tolerance (β = 0.07, 95% CI = 0.02–0.03), depreciation (β = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.01–0.05), and total irrational beliefs (β = 0.06, 95% CI = 0.03–0.09) on cognitive anxiety were significant only when both mediators (standards and discrepancy) were entered (see Table 3). Significant indirect effects were obtained for low frustration tolerance (β = 0.11, 95% CI = 0.03–0.05), depreciation (β = 0.11, 95% CI = 0.04–0.17), and total irrational beliefs (β = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.03–0.09) with cognitive anxiety as the outcome variable and discrepancy as the mediator.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Regression weights for the SAMM models with irrational beliefs (iPBI; DEM, AWF, LFT, DEP, IBS), perfectionism (SAPS; STD, DCY) and cognitive anxiety (CSAI-2; CAN). *p < 0.05. DEM, demandingness; LIT, low-frustration tolerance; AWF, awfulizing; DEP, depreciation; IBS, total of irrational beliefs; STD, standards (adaptive perfectionism); DCY, discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism); CAN, cognitive anxiety.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Indirect effects of serial atemporal multiple mediations (SAMM).

Serial atemporal multiple mediation among irrational beliefs, perfectionism, and somatic anxiety

Significant positive direct effects were obtained for depreciation (β = 0.19, 95% CI = 0.11–0.27), and total irrational beliefs (β = 0.18, 95% CI = 0.07–0.30) with somatic anxiety as the outcome variable and both standards and discrepancy entered as mediators (see Figure 3). Adaptive perfectionism (standards) did not significantly predict somatic anxiety in any of the tested models. Demandingness (β = 0.02 95% CI = 0.01–0.05) and awfulizing (β = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.00–0.06) had significant indirect effects on somatic anxiety when both mediators (standards and discrepancy) were entered.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regression weights for the SAMM models with irrational beliefs (iPBI), perfectionism (SAPS) and somatic anxiety (CSAI-2). *p < 0.05. DEM, demandingness, LFT, low-frustration tolerance; AWF, awfulizing; DEP, depreciation; IBS, total of irrational beliefs; STD, standards (adaptive perfectionism); DCY, discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism); SAN, somatic anxiety.

Discussion

In the present study we explored the effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety through adaptive (standards) and maladaptive (discrepancy) perfectionism. To the authors’ knowledge, this study is the first to explore the effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety among Hungarian athletes, and it is the first to test adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism as mediators of the mentioned effects. It also adds to and extends the limited research that has linked irrational beliefs to perfectionism.

Effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety through perfectionism

Both primary and secondary irrational beliefs were positively associated with both cognitive and somatic competitive anxiety in athletes, which corroborates previous related findings (e.g., Chadha et al., 2019). Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism were also positively associated with both forms of competitive anxiety, in line with the findings of Hill et al. (2020), who emphasize that either form of perfectionism can pose a threat to one’s wellbeing. However, the positive correlations between adaptive perfectionism and competitive anxiety were low in magnitude, thus they are not inconsistent with the finding that perfectionist standards in sport may in some cases be associated with positive emotions and low cognitive and somatic competitive anxiety (Stoeber et al., 2007; Sagar and Stoeber, 2009). These results suggest that there is a relationship between athletes’ irrational beliefs and trait-level perfectionism and they raise the possibility that perfectionism mediates the effects of irrational beliefs on perfectionism. As such, it appears that athletes who hold both strong irrational beliefs and strong perfectionistic beliefs could be a greater risk of experiencing higher competitive anxiety, but more technically, it is through perfectionistic beliefs that irrational beliefs might be more problematic.

Overall, the findings of the present paper indicate that athletes who hold stronger rigid, extreme and illogical thinking (irrational beliefs) are more concerned about their performance and possible negative consequences (cognitive anxiety), which is in large part due to their excessive expectations for, and highly critical attitude toward their own performance. Both irrational beliefs and perfectionistic beliefs reflect illogical and extreme ways of thinking, and as such, it is perhaps unsurprising that both are related to higher anxiety. Specifically, depreciation and low frustration tolerance had significant indirect effects on athletes’ cognitive anxiety when maladaptive perfectionism was the only mediator. Irrational beliefs also influenced the athletes’ somatic anxiety. Full mediation was found for the effects of demandingness and awfulizing with both forms of perfectionism entered as mediators (i.e., the indirect effects but not the direct effects were significant). This suggests that athletes with dogmatic self-set standards and fear of poor performance show higher somatic anxiety due to their perfectionist traits. Overall irrational beliefs, low frustration tolerance, and depreciation had significant direct effects but no indirect effect on somatic anxiety. That is, athletes who have difficulty tolerating adversity (LFT) and whose global sense of worth is highly vulnerable to, and in part defined by, momentary poor performance (DEP) are likely to experience higher levels of somatic anxiety irrespective of whether or not they have perfectionist standards and concerns. Importantly, adaptive perfectionism (standards) alone did not significantly mediate the effects of irrational beliefs on competitive anxiety in any of the tested models, which is consistent with the findings reported by Hill et al. (2018). These findings suggest that adaptive perfectionism may not have an antagonistic effect over above irrational beliefs, unlike maladaptive perfectionism alone, which did mediate between irrational beliefs and anxiety.

In sum, the obtained findings support the hypothesis that irrational beliefs positively predict competitive anxiety, and especially cognitive anxiety, through perfectionism, especially maladaptive perfectionism. Perfectionism also mediated the effects of two types of irrational beliefs (DEM and AWF) on somatic anxiety, a possible explanation for which is that these two types of irrational beliefs may be conceptually similar to perfectionistic beliefs. Overall, athletes who set dogmatic standards for themselves are not able to manage frustration and tend to awfulizing and disparagement themselves and others experience greater competitive anxiety. The following process influenced by perfectionist traits such as overly high expectations and a disproportionate focus on mistakes.

Practical implications

The obtained findings have important practical applications for both researchers and practitioners. We believe that it is very important that researchers use adequate statistical procedures in addition to adequate instruments in order to obtain meaningful results. The statistical procedure (SAMM) used in the present study allows for testing complex models elaborating on empirically supported previous theories, such as those concerning the predictive relationships between irrational beliefs, perfectionism, and competitive anxiety. Practitioners may also benefit from the results of the present study. While REBT is a relatively new technique in sport (most research emerging post-2013; Jordana et al., 2020), the present study demonstrates its relevance to competitive anxiety. The results suggest that enabling athletes to gain insight into their irrational beliefs may help them better manage their perfectionism and competitive anxiety. A promising means to this end is the REBT procedure adapted for athletes (Turner and Bennett, 2018), which may be used in both individual and group settings. Practitioners should also be mindful that anxiety it is not always maladaptive (Ellis and DiGiuseppe, 1993) or related to poor performance (Craft et al., 2003), and thus, when working with athletes who report high anxiety, we need to understand the extent to which this anxiety is indeed maladaptive for the athlete. The distinction between adaptive and maladaptive emotion is a core consideration of REBT (e.g., Turner et al., 2018), which advocates the diminishment of maladaptive emotion and not adaptive emotion.

Limitations and future directions

The current study has several limitations. It is a cross-sectional study exclusively involving Hungarian athletes, which does not allow for the exploration of true causality, nor does it allow for assessing the importance of cultural factors, while most related previous findings were obtained for North American and British samples. Turner et al. (2019a) found that elite athletes reported smaller depreciation beliefs, this result suggesting that the level of athlete may influence the emergence of irrational beliefs but not the relationship between irrational beliefs and the outcome variables. Based on the previous studies further research is needed to fully understand how athletes level influence irrational beliefs and it could be a further development of the current study to compare athletes at various levels in terms of irrational beliefs, perfectionism and competitive anxiety. Finally, in addition to perfectionism, other psychological constructs (e.g., negative automatic thoughts, fear of failure, early maladaptive schemas, self-determined motivation, stress mindset, and cognitive appraisal) may also influence the emergence of anxiety alongside irrational beliefs (e.g., Buschmann et al., 2018; Chadha et al., 2019; Turner et al., 2019b, 2022; Mansell, 2021), the exploration of which requires further study. Another important objective of future studies should be a longitudinal assessment of the effectiveness of psychological interventions (e.g., REBT) in regulating athletes’ competitive anxiety by restructuring irrational beliefs and perfectionism. The placement of perfectionism in REBT is yet to be fully conceptualized, and intervention research may highlight how practitioners can effectively challenge perfectionism within an REBT framework.

Conclusion

The present study found that all types of irrational beliefs had an impact on competitive anxiety. Furthermore, the effects of several irrational beliefs on cognitive anxiety were partially mediated by adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism combined or by maladaptive perfectionism alone. These findings could suggest that an REBT intervention may help athletes effectively manage their maladaptive perfectionism and competitive anxiety.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Hungarian University of Sport Science Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

RT: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, writing—original draft, and visualization. MT: conceptualization, resources, writing—review and editing, and supervision. TK: investigation and supervision. LT: conceptualization, investigation, and supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bowman, A., and Turner, M. J. (2022). When time is of the essence: The use of rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT) informed single-session therapy (SST) to alleviate social and golf-specific anxiety, and improve wellbeing and performance, in amateur golfers. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 60:102167. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, C. M., Dowd, E. T., and Freeman, A. (2010). “Rational and irrational beliefs and psychopathology,” in The rational and irrational beliefs in human functioning and disturbances: Implications for research, theory, and practice, eds D. David, S. J. Lynn, and A. Ellis (New York: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Buschmann, T., Horn, R. A., Blankenship, V. R., Garcia, Y. E., and Bohan, K. B. (2018). The relationship between automatic thoughts and irrational beliefs predicting anxiety and depression. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 36, 137–162. doi: 10.1007/s10942-017-0278-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadha, N. J., Turner, M. J., and Slater, M. J. (2019). Investigating irrational beliefs, cognitive appraisals, challenge and threat, and affective states in golfers approaching competitive situations. Front. Psychol. 10:2295. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chotpitayasunondh, V., and Turner, M. J. (2019). The development and validation of the Thai-translated irrational performance beliefs inventory (T-iPBI). J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 37, 202–221. doi: 10.1007/s10942-018-0306-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craft, L. L., Magyar, T. M., Becker, B. J., and Feltz, D. L. (2003). The relationship between the competitive state anxiety inventory-2 and sport performance: A meta-analysis. J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 25, 44–65. doi: 10.1080/0264041031000101809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deffenbacher, J. L., Zwemer, W. A., Whisman, M. A., Hill, R. A., and Sloan, R. D. (1986). Irrational beliefs and anxiety. Cognit. Ther. Res. 10, 281–291. doi: 10.1007/bf01173466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dryden, W. (1995). Brief rational emotive behaviour therapy. London: Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Ellis, A. (1999). Why rational-emotive therapy to rational emotive behavior therapy? Psychol. Psychother. 36, 154–159. doi: 10.1037/h0087680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, A. (2002). “The role of irrational beliefs in perfectionism,” in The perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment, eds G. L. Flett and P. L. Hewitt (Washington: APA), 217–229.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ellis, A., and DiGiuseppe, R. (1993). Are inappropriate or dysfunctional feelings in rational-emotive therapy qualitative or quantitative? Cognit. Ther. Res. 5, 471–477.

Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., and Koledin, S. (1991). Dimensions of perfectionism and irrational thinking. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 9, 185–201. doi: 10.1007/BF01061229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., and Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognit. Ther. Res. 14, 449–468. doi: 10.1007/BF01172967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotwals, J. K., Stoeber, J., Dunn, J. G., and Stoll, O. (2012). Are perfectionistic strivings in sport adaptive? A systematic review of confirmatory, contradictory, and mixed evidence. Can. Psychol. 53, 263–279. doi: 10.1037/a0030288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotwals, K. J., and Tamminen, A. K. (2022). Intercollegiate perfectionistic athletes’ perspectives on success and failure in sport. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 34, 25–46. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2020.1740826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, L. (1990). “A catastrophe model of anxiety and performance,” in The stress and performance in sport, eds J. G. Jones and L. Hardy (Chichester: Wiley), 81–106.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional process analysis, 3rd. Edn. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hill, A. P., Madigan, D. J., and Jowett, G. E. (2020). “Perfectionism in sport, dance and exercise. An extended review and reanalysis,” in The handbook of sport psychology, eds G. Tenebaum and R. C. Eklund (New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley and Sons), 121–157.

Google Scholar

Hill, A. P., Mallinson-Howard, S. H., and Jowett, G. E. (2018). Multidimensional perfectionism in sport: A meta-analytical review. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 7, 235–270. doi: 10.1037/spy0000125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Himle, D. P., Thyer, B. A., and Papsdorf, J. D. (1982). Relationships between rational beliefs and anxiety. Cognit. Ther. Res. 6, 219–223. doi: 10.1007/bf01183895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingle, S. (2021). Simone Biles exits women’s Olympic teamgymnastics final over mental health concern. London: The Guardian.

Google Scholar

Jordana, A., Turner, M. J., Ramis, Y., and Torregrossa, M. (2020). A systematic mapping review on the use of rational emotive behavior therapy (rebt) with athletes. Int. Rev. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2020.1836673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Limburg, K., Watson, H. J., Hagger, M. S., and Egan, S. J. (2017). The relationship between perfectionism and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 73, 1301–1326. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansell, P. C. (2021). Stress mindset in athletes: Investigating the relationships between beliefs, challenge and threat with psychological wellbeing. Psychol. Sport. Exerc. 57:102020. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2021.102020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, R., Vealey, R. S., and Burton, D. (1990). Competitive anxiety in sport. Champaogn, Illinois: Human Kinetics Books.

Google Scholar

Nejati, M., Farsi, A., Moteshareie, E., and Turner, M. J. (2022). The Persian irrational performance beliefs inventory (iPBI-Persian): Translation, confirmatory factor analysis, and test–retest reliability, in Iranian athletes. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 40, 191–205. doi: 10.1007/s10942-021-00395-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinhardt, M., Rice, K. G., and Tóth, L. (2019). Latent profiles of perfectionism, mental health and stress in American and Hungarian undergraduates. Palma de Mallorca, Spain: 40th STAR Conference of the Stress and Anxiety Research Society.

Google Scholar

Reinhardt, M., Tóth, L., and Rice, K. G. (2018). Perfectionism reflected in emotion regulation and mental health among young elite athletes and controls. Lublin, Poland: 39th STAR Conference of the Stress and Anxiety Research Society.

Google Scholar

Rice, K. G., and Ashby, J. S. (2007). An efficient method for classifying perfectionists. J. Couns. Psychol. 54, 72–85. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.1.72

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, K. G., Richardson, C. M. E., and Tueller, S. (2014). The Short Form of the Revised Almost Perfect Scale. J. Pers. Assess. 96, 368–379. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.838172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagar, S. S., and Stoeber, J. (2009). Perfectionism, fear of failure, and affective responses to success and failure: The central role of fear of experiencing shame and embarrassment. J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 31, 602–627. doi: 10.1123/jsep.31.5.602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sipos, K., Kudar, K., Bejek, K., and Tóth, L. (1999). “Standardisation and validation of the hungarian competitive state anxiety inventory-2 (CSAI-2) of martens et al. (1990)” in Proceedings of the 20th international conference of STAR, Cracow.

Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., and Otto, K. (2006). Positive concepcions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 295–319. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., Otto, K., Pescheck, E., Becker, C., and Stoll, O. (2007). Perfectionism and competitive anxiety in athletes: Differentiating striving for perfection and negative reactions to imperfection. Pers. Individ. Differ. 42, 959–969. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molcsapat (2022). Szilágyi Áron pszichológusként beszél a kézi-eb-rõl. Available online at: https://molcsapat.hu/kezilabda/szilagyi-aron-pszichologuskent-beszel-a-kezi-eb-rol-2681 (accessed February 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

Turner, M. J. (2016). Rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT), irrational and rational beliefs, and the mental health of athletes. Front. Psychol. 7:1423. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., and Allen, M. (2018). Confirmatory factor analysis of the irrational performance beliefs inventory (iPBI) in a sample of amateur and semi-professional athletes. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 35, 126–130. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.11.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Aspin, G., Didymus, F., Mack, R., Olusoga, P., Wood, A. G., et al. (2020a). One case, five approaches: The application of psychotherapeutic approaches in sport psychology. Sport. Psychol. 34, 71–83. doi: 10.1123/tsp.2019-0079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Carrington, S., and Miller, A. (2019a). Psychological distress across sport participation groups: The mediating effects of secondary irrational beliefs on the relationship between primary irrational beliefs and symptoms of anxiety, anger, and depression. J. Clin. Sport Psychol. 13, 17–40. doi: 10.1123/jcsp.2017-0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Aspin, G., and Gillman, J. (2019b). Maladaptive schema as a potential mechanism through which irrational beliefs relate to psychological distress in athletes. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 44, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.04.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., and Barker, J. B. (2013). Examining the efficacy of rational-emotive behavior therapy (REBT) on irrational beliefs and anxiety in elite youth cricketers. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 25, 131–147. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.574311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., and Bennett, R. (2018). Rational emotive behavior therapy in sport and excercise. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Ewen, D., and Barker, J. B. (2020b). An idiographic single-case study examining the use of rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT) with three amateur golfers to alleviate sport performance phobias. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 32, 186–204. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2018.1496186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Jones, J., and Wood, A. G. (2018). Applying the REBT cognitive disputation technique to the binary theory of emotional distress. Eff. Sci.-Pract. 1, 46–64.

Google Scholar

Turner, M. J., Miller, A., Youngs, H., Barber, N., Brick, N. E., Chadha, N. J., et al. (2022). “I must do this!”: A latent profile analysis approach to understanding the role of irrational beliefs and motivation regulation in mental and physical health. J. Sports Sci. 40, 934–949. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2022.2042124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visla, A., Flückiger, C., Grosse Holtforth, M., and David, D. (2016). Irrational beliefs and psychological distress: A meta-analysis. Psychother. Psychosom 85, 8–15. doi: 10.1159/000441231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. G., Barker, J. B., Turner, M. J., and Sheffield, D. (2018). Examining the effects of rational emotive behavior therapy on performance outcomes in elite paralympic athletes. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 28, 329–339. doi: 10.1111/sms.12926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: irrational beliefs, competitive anxiety, rational emotive behavioral therapy, adaptive perfectionism, maladaptive perfectionism

Citation: Tóth R, Turner MJ, Kökény T and Tóth L (2022) “I must be perfect”: The role of irrational beliefs and perfectionism on the competitive anxiety of Hungarian athletes. Front. Psychol. 13:994126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.994126

Received: 14 July 2022; Accepted: 18 August 2022;
Published: 20 September 2022.

Edited by:

Antonio Hernández-Mendo, University of Malaga, Spain

Reviewed by:

Antonio Matas-Terron, University of Malaga, Spain
M a Auxiliadora Franquelo Egea, Universidad de Málaga, Spain

Copyright © 2022 Tóth, Turner, Kökény and Tóth. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Renátó Tóth, dG90aHJlbmF0bzI5QGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.