Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 26 October 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Momentarily narcissistic? Development of a short, state version of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory applicable in momentary assessment

  • 1Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 3University College Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Background: Narcissism viewed as a personality process rather than a stable trait explains narcissistic functioning as a tool for maintaining a positive self-view. Studying narcissism therefore needs adequate momentary measures for collecting higher frequency longitudinal data in experience sampling method (ESM) studies. In this study, a shorter version of the Pathological Narcisissm Inventory is offered to measure vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic states, applicable in momentary assessment.

Methods: The measurement tool was tested in three samples. First, we assessed the factor structure and associations with other contemporary measures of narcissism in a cross-sectional design on one English speaking (n = 319) and one Hungarian sample (n = 236). Second, we conducted a five-day long experience sampling method study with a total of 15 measurement points (n = 123).

Results: Based on structural equation modelling and multilevel analyses, the results suggest that the measure has adequate psychometric properties in both the within and between subject levels as well as acceptable convergent and discriminant validity.

Conclusions: The Pathological Narcissism Inventory – State Version (PNI-S) can be a useful tool in momentary data collection enabling the examination of personality processes behind narcissistic functioning.

Introduction

In recent years the study of narcissism has expanded substantially, however some important questions are still left open (Miller et al., 2021). Researchers agree that narcissistic behaviours can be categorised into at least two broader subtypes, namely grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (Wink, 1996). Individuals with grandiose narcissistic traits are described as arrogant, exploitative, and entitled (Cain et al., 2008), and they often engage in self-aggrandisement and self-promotion while also striving for a sense of uniqueness (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2008; Miller et al., 2017). By contrast, vulnerable narcissism is characterised by defensive social withdrawal, self-inhibition, and substantial reliance on the approval of others for feelings of self-worth (Cain et al., 2008; Zeigler-Hill et al., 2008). At the same time, these individuals also hold grandiose expectations of oneself and others (Wink, 1996) and they tend to be overly sensitive to the feelings of embarrassment and shame (Edershile et al., 2019).

Recently, two hierarchical models were proposed to integrate the seemingly incoherent nature of narcissistic grandiosity and vulnerability (Wright and Edershile, 2018; Miller et al., 2021). Krizan and Herlache (2018) offered the Narcissism Spectrum Model (NSM) placing self-importance and entitlement on the core of narcissistic functioning. In their model narcissistic vulnerability would be a phenotypic manifestation of the reactive orientation system characterised by avoidance while narcissistic grandiosity is reflecting the approach-dominant orientation system (Gray and McNaughton, 2000). Miller et al. (2016) on the other hand applied the five-factor model of personality and argued that antagonism with features like exploitativeness, entitlement or reactive anger serve as the core of narcissistic functioning while vulnerability can be understood through neuroticism, and grandiosity through agentic traits of extraversion. These two hierarchical solutions are similar in nature highlighting general narcissistic tendencies, while also differentiating functionally divergent manifestations (Wright and Edershile, 2018).

Besides these hierarchical models another recent conceptualization of narcissism is aiming to separate two social strategies of how narcissistic individuals aim to maintain their grandiose self. In the Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Concept (NARC; Back et al., 2013; Back, 2018) the pathway of narcissistic admiration serves as an assertive self-enhancement strategy to approach positive social incentives (e.g., grandiose fantasies, striving for uniqueness), while narcissistic rivalry aims to prevent social failure by protecting the self in an antagonistic manner (e.g., devaluation of others, striving for supremacy), although these strategies can be considered as agentic or antagonistic behaviour patterns from the viewpoint of the five-factor model (Back, 2018). Similarly to the hierarchical conceptualizations, the NARC model also identifies a general, overarching goal behind distinct narcissistic behaviours, namely the maintenance of the grandiose view of the self.

Vulnerable and grandiose narcissism has substantially different nomological networks (Miller et al., 2011). Regarding personality traits, grandiosity is positively associated with extraversion (e.g., facets assertiveness and excitement seeking) while negatively with agreeableness (e.g., facets compliance and modesty). Vulnerability on the other hand is in strong positive association with neuroticism (e.g., facets depression and angry hostility) and a moderate negative correlation with agreeableness.

These differences also exist in other important aspects of personality functioning for example implicit and explicit self-esteem (Miller et al., 2011; Mota et al., 2019), or most indicators of psychological well-being. Grandiose narcissism is in a moderate positive association with subjective well-being (Egan et al., 2014; Czarna et al., 2018), life satisfaction (Sedikides et al., 2004) frequent experiences of positive emotions (Rhodewalt et al., 1998) or self-esteem (Miller et al., 2011). In contrast, vulnerable narcissism is negatively associated with factors of eudaimonic well-being (Kaufman et al., 2018) and positively to negative emotionality, depression and low self-esteem (Miller et al., 2011, 2017).

Measurement of narcissism was also subject to clarification recently, as several measures of grandiosity and vulnerability emerged over the years, and different underlying conceptualizations made the synthetisation of results difficult (Miller et al., 2021). Narcissism measures can therefore be categorized by measuring either vulnerable or grandiose narcissism or both. Currently the most widespread measure of grandiose narcissism is the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin and Terry, 1988; Cain et al., 2008), although several other psychometrically sound measures were introduced in recent years (e.g., the Narcissistic Grandiosity Scale NGS; Rosenthal et al., 2007). Separate measurement of vulnerable narcissism is also supported (e.g., the Maladaptive Covert Narcissism Scale; Cheek et al., 2013 or the Narcissistic Vulnerability Scale; NVS; Crowe et al., 2018), moreover several measures are assessing the two dimensions as facets of one instrument e.g., the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., 2009); or the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI; Glover et al., 2012).

With recent hierarchical models conceptualizing vulnerability and grandiosity as personality moderated expressions of the same core (Wright and Edershile, 2018), a three-dimensional approach has also been applied in evaluating different measurement methods. The role of differentiating entitlement, grandiosity and vulnerability is mainly to enable a more unified approach in measuring these constructs with several different tools (Wright and Edershile, 2018). According to this concept, different underlying factors are captured more precisely by different measurement tools. For example, the NGS (Rosenthal et al., 2007) is rather measuring exhibitionism than entitlement, while the grandiosity and vulnerability factors of the PNI (Pincus et al., 2009) both capture the “entitlement core” of narcissism (for more details see Wright and Edershile, 2018). Although the focus of this scale is closer to vulnerability (Krizan and Herlache, 2018; Crowe et al., 2019) and maladaptive manifestations of narcissism (Pincus et al., 2009), central elements of grandiosity are also captured sufficiently especially when applying a multivariate approach (Edershile et al., 2019). In this study we used the PNI for developing a state level measure of narcissistic functioning mostly because it captures not only exhibitionism and vulnerability but also entitlement and the antagonistic core of narcissism in both factors. The dominant use of the above scales reflects the similarly dominant view that narcissism is a stable trait (for a review, see Campbell and Miller, 2011), however how these descriptive behaviours converge at the individual level and in real-time is less well understood (Wright and Edershile, 2018; Edershile and Wright, 2021). Trait level measurement on the other hand is also subject to several biases coming from aggregation of experiences or the lack of reliable processes of memory recollection (for a review, see Hektner et al., 2007). Moreover, from a methodological point of view narcissism is mainly studied in between-subject settings, which offers insights into structural and dispositional differences between people, however internal personality processes can be examined rather on the within-subject level (Bolger et al., 2003) and with intensive longitudinal data (Wright and Edershile, 2018).

Zooming in on the narcissistic process (which we refer to as including actions, feelings, and thoughts) that characterise narcissistic traits can help in understanding why and when this process operates and in which circumstances does it result in positive versus negative subjective experiences. Recent models of narcissism are beginning to conceptualise narcissism as a dynamic self-regulatory process (Giacomin, 2016) in which the narcissistic process is used to maintain positive self-views (Morf and Rhodewalt, 2001; for a review, see Giacomin, 2016). This growing focus on the understanding of within-individual processes underlies the need to measure narcissistic actions, feelings, and thoughts from moment to moment enabling us to observe individual trajectories of momentarily narcissistic tendencies and how the stability of this process emerges over time (i.e., the stability that characterises traits). Furthermore, focusing on these trajectories of behaviours would allow us to understand how vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic tendencies are temporally associated (Edershile et al., 2019). This is crucial because vulnerable and grandiose behaviours correlate weakly on the trait level (Miller et al., 2010), they have different nomological networks (Miller et al., 2011), although both of them can occur within the same individual present at different times or situations according to clinical theories and observations (Wink, 1996; Edershile et al., 2019). Furthermore, a recent study by Jauk and his colleagues (2021) emphasized a nonlinear association between grandiose and vulnerable traits indicating, that at higher levels of grandiosity vulnerability increases, indirectly reflecting the possible effects of state changes.

With a growing interest in the processes that underlie narcissistic functioning (Geukes et al., 2017; Edershile et al., 2019), there are different approaches for measuring grandiose and vulnerable narcissism as momentary assessments (i.e., states). Giacomin (2016), for example, mainly used trait measures adapted to capture states by changing the instructions such that participants were asked to reflect on their current states. This, however, might not be ideal because most items refer to general or aggregated personal qualities. Nevertheless, this step toward a state measure revealed moderate fluctuations across contexts.

In other studies, the adjective-based Narcissistic Grandiosity Scale (NGS) and the Narcissistic Vulnerability Scale (NVS) were used as state measurement, (e.g., Edershile et al., 2019; Edershile and Wright, 2021) where participants needed to decide how they feel about themselves at the moment. Originally, the NGS consists of 16 adjectives (e.g., glorious, prestigious) while the NVS offers 11 (e.g., underappreciated, insecure), although in most studies using intensive longitudinal data a shorter version of the scales with 4–4 items demonstrated good psychometric properties (Crowe et al., 2016, 2018; Edershile et al., 2019).

Narcissism viewed from the dynamic self-regulatory perspective (Morf and Rhodewalt, 2001) can be understood as a set of feelings, actions and behaviours that the individual tends to use to maintain positive self-views. Therefore, limiting the scope of study to the self-related feelings, might limit our ability to evaluate every aspect connected to narcissistic states when they are not accompanied by stronger feelings.

The state-like measures of narcissism demonstrate an important step toward capturing the dynamics of narcissistic behaviours. However, to collect higher-frequency longitudinal data in experience sampling method studies (Hektner et al., 2007), it is crucial that researchers use the shortest assessment possible while ensuring that validity is protected. Furthermore, researchers need to minimise the chance, that a time-consuming assessment might interfere with the process they wish to study.

In this study we aimed to develop a tool for assessing narcissistic states, which is necessary to study how and when the narcissistic process emerges. We assessed the psychometric properties of a state measure of narcissism focusing on momentary narcissistic behaviours. It consists of seven items, making it shorter than the previous measures used (NGS and NVS; Edershile et al., 2019), assessing both vulnerable (four items) and grandiose (three items) narcissistic functioning. On two different samples we assessed the convergent and discriminant validity of the measure with the currently used trait measurements of narcissism and self-esteem. On the third sample, we tested its within-and between-subject level associations using structural equation modelling and multilevel analyses, based on a five-days long experience sampling method setting. We hypothesized that state-level vulnerable narcissism would be in positive relationship with other current measures of vulnerable narcissism and in negative relationship with self-esteem and psychological well-being (Miller et al., 2018). We also expected that grandiose narcissistic states would be in positive correlation with other contemporary measures of grandiosity, and in positive correlation with self-esteem and psychological well-being (Miller et al., 2011; Hyatt et al., 2018). In this research we selected the criterion variables based on their consistent and well-established associations with grandiose and vulnerable narcissism in previous studies (e.g., Miller et al., 2011; Aghababaei and Błachnio, 2015; Kaufman et al., 2018).

Lastly, we expected weak but positive relationship between grandiose and vulnerable states on the between-subject level representing the entitlement core of the distinct manifestations. In contrast, on the within-subject level we expected negative relationship between grandiose and vulnerable states as with context-dependent fluctuations grandiosity and vulnerability were not expected to be present at the same time (Wink, 1996; Edershile et al., 2019).

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

In this study three different samples were administered in different languages and with different settings.

In Sample 1 (S1), participants were recruited from a pool of fluent speaking first-year international students from a large Dutch university who participated in exchange for course credits. The measures were administered in English. Participants registered for the study through a secure online portal from which they were redirected to the online surveys. A total of 319 participants (73% female; mean age = 20.18; SD = 2.31) filled out the measures. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Psychology, University of Groningen (registration number: 18102-S).

In Sample 2 (S2) participants were recruited from a pool of students from a large Hungarian university who participated in exchange for course credits. The measures were administered in Hungarian. Participants registered for the study through a secure online portal from which they were redirected to the online surveys. A total of 236 participants (75% female; mean age = 22.10; SD = 3.91) filled out the measures.

In Sample 3 (S3), participants were recruited from a large university in Hungary who participated in exchange for course credits. The measures were administered in Hungarian. A total of 128 participants completed the first wave of the study in which they filled out trait measures and provided their mobile phone numbers. A total of 123 participants completed the second part of the study with at least 80% fill-out rate (66.4% female; mean age = 21.84; SD = 3.53) in which participants had to fill out three measurements per day for five consecutive days. The five-day period started on a Tuesday and lasted until Saturday. Participants were sent a short message with the current questionnaire’s link, which could be easily filled out on an Android or iOS smartphone. Three momentary questionnaires were sent out at a random time within three separate time frames: from 8:00 to 11:00, from 12:00 to 15:00, and from 16:00 to 19:00. Questionnaires were distributed at least 2 h after the previous measurement. The study containing Sample 2 and Sample 3 was approved by the Ethical Committee of Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary (registration number: 2018/229).

Measures

Narcissistic personality inventory (NPI-40)

The Likert version of the original English version of the NPI-40 (Raskin and Terry, 1988) was administered in Sample 1, which presents only the original 40 narcissistic items. Participants stated how much each item described them on a scale from one to five. This response format of the NPI is recently gaining popularity (Wetzel et al., 2016; Miller et al., 2018; Engyel et al., 2022). The NPI was administered in Samples 1 and Sample 3.

Pathological narcissism inventory (PNI)

The PNI (Pincus et al., 2009) assesses seven factors of both vulnerable and grandiose narcissism. A factor structure was proposed of two higher-order dimensions (Wright et al., 2010): Exploitativeness, Self-Sacrificing, Self-Enhancement, and Grandiose Fantasies together form the grandiosity factor while Contingent Self-Esteem, Hiding the Self, Devaluing, and Entitlement Rage form the vulnerability factor. Items are rated from “Not at all like me” as zero to “Very much like me” as five. The PNI was administered in Sample 1 and Sample 3.

Narcissistic grandiosity scale (NGS)

The NGS (Rosenthal et al., 2007; Crowe et al., 2016) is an adjective scale that contains 16 items. Participants are presented with the 16 adjectives (e.g., Glorious, Prestigious) and asked to rate how much these adjectives describe themselves. The scale has been recently demonstrated to have good psychometric properties (Crowe et al., 2016). The NGS was administered in Sample 1.

Narcissistic vulnerability scale (NVS)

The NVS (Crowe et al., 2018) is also an adjective-based measure similar to the NGS with 11 items such as Underappreciated, Insecure, and Fragile. Participants are asked to state how much these adjectives describe them on a scale from one to seven. The NVS was administered in Sample 1.

Maladaptive covert narcissism scale (MCNS)

The MCNS (Cheek et al., 2013) is a Likert-type measure assessing hypersensitive or vulnerable narcissism. It consists of 23 items with answer options ranging from one to five. This scale is considered as a significantly improved version (Cheek et al., 2013) of the original Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (Hendin and Cheek, 1997). The MCNS was administered in Sample 1 and Sample 3.

Self-esteem

Self-esteem was assessed using the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965), a widely used 10-item scale capturing global self-esteem. Participants respond to questions (e.g., “I am able to do things as well as most other people”) on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from zero – “strongly disagree” to three – “strongly agree.” The measure consists of five reversed items. The RSES was administered in Sample 1 and Sample 3.

State self-esteem

State self-esteem was assessed in Sample 3 as a momentary assessment. We used one positive and one negative statement from the Rosenberg (1965) self-esteem scale (RSES), changing the framing to capture momentary states. The two items were “Right now, I feel that I cannot be proud of anything” and “Right now, I feel that I have a number of good qualities.”

Eudaimonic well-being

To measure eudaimonic well-being the Hungarian version of the Ryff and Keyes (1995) Well-Being Scale was used which assesses Ryff’s (1989) original six factors of eudaimonic well-being with three items for each factor: purpose in life, autonomy, personal growth, self-acceptance, positive relations and environmental mastery. The measure offered good internal consistency in our sample (α = 0.86). This scale was administered in Sample 3.

Pathological narcissism inventory – state version (PNI-S)

We aimed to assess both vulnerable and grandiose narcissism as states. Based on our preliminary analysis, using a combination of data-driven psychometric and judgmental content-related considerations (for a review, see Kruyen et al., 2013) we used one item from each of the seven subfactors of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., 2009). Items were considered appropriate if they represented the latent factors well based on the factor loadings in the original study of Pincus et al. (2009). First, items with factor loadings higher than 0.7 were selected in each subfactor of the PNI, following the recommendation of Tabachnick et al. (2007). Second, items were selected if they could be understood as a current state of mind (e.g., “I often fantasize about having a huge impact on the world around me.”) rather than just aggregated personal qualities. Selected items were judged by the members of our research team regarding their ability to capture the content of the PNI subscale well. Last, the highest rated items were tailored to measure the momentary experience of the participant (e.g., “Right now, I am having fantasies of having a huge impact on the world around me.”). The phrase “Right now,” was included in all items to avoid possible effects of skipping the measure instructions and to help participants focusing on their current state of mind. The vulnerable narcissistic state was therefore assessed with four items while the grandiose state was captured with three items. Items of the PNI-S are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Items of the PNI-S with the related facets from the original PNI and an exploratory factor analysis of the PNI-S in the international and Hungarian samples.

The Hungarian translation of the items was carried out following the guidelines of the standard test-adaptation procedure: first, two members of the research group translated the items separately, then a back-translation was applied. The internal consistency of the subscales is presented in both the English and Hungarian measures in Table 1. The subscale measuring vulnerable narcissistic states offers acceptable reliability in both samples, however the α for grandiose states remains moderate, due to the limited number of items, and due to the fact, that two items out of the three had substantially higher factor loadings in the scale. Moreover, coefficient alpha can be underestimated when tests are short (Cronbach, 1951; Hoekstra et al., 2019) and ranges of acceptable alpha is also lower when measures have fewer than 10 items (Pallant, 2011).

Statistical analysis plan

Descriptive statistics and intraclass correlation coefficients

To compare the nomological network of PNI-S grandiosity and vulnerability factors with other contemporary measures of narcissism and other correlates intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) were calculated. ICC values above 0.8 are regarded as signs of good reliability (Koo and Li, 2016; Liljequist et al., 2019). First, PNI-S factors were compared to the original PNI grandiosity and vulnerability factors. Second, other grandiosity measures, vulnerability measures and external correlates were used excluding the PNI due to the considerable overlap between the state and trait constructs.

Exploratory factor analysis

First, we applied an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using SPSS version 25 on the data to explore the factor structure of the 7-item long measure. We applied the Maximum Likelihood method for factor extraction, with a direct oblimin rotation on both the English version (S1), both the Hungarian version (S2) of the measure. Factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 were extracted.

Confirmatory factor analysis with covariates

Second, we performed confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) using Mplus 8.3 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). In a CFA, a satisfactory degree of fit requires the comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) to be close to 0.95, and the model should be rejected when these indices are less than 0.90 (Brown, 2006). The next fit index was the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). RMSEA below 0.05 indicates excellent fit, a value around 0.08 indicates adequate fit, and a value above 0.10 indicates poor fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999).

We applied a CFA on S1 to assess the associations of the two-factor measurement model of grandiose and vulnerable states with other measures of narcissism and personality functioning using a CFA with covariates analysis.

Furthermore, we conducted a multilevel CFA on the data from Sample 3 to differentiate the within-subject and the between-subject level in our analysis (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015).

Within-subject level analysis

We also tested the intra-individual associations of the state measures in an experience sampling method setting on Sample 3. We used 15 measurement points, and therefore the standard methods for evaluating the association could not be used without violating the assumption of independence of observations.

Therefore, the present study applied the dynamic structural equation modelling framework (DSEM) using Mplus (McNeish and Hamaker, 2019), which enables the integration of both SEM models and time-series analysis (Asparouhov et al., 2018). Our data contained information on two levels, 15 measurement points were nested within a person. Therefore, using multilevel modelling, the within-subject level dynamics of narcissistic states can be separated from the associations captured in the between-subject level.

Our model is presented in Figure 1. As we had two outcome variables collected at each measurement, besides the autoregression (when a variable from the previous time point predicts the same variable in a following time point) we were also interested in the cross-lagged associations between them (meaning the effect of the first variable collected in a time point on the second variable in the preceding time point), we used a multilevel cross-lagged vector autoregressive model [multilevel VAR (1)]. These autoregressive relationships are also good measures for examining construct stability (Hamaker et al., 2015), which was one of our main aims in this study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The stability of state constructs: Within level analysis of the multilevel cross-lagged autoregressive model. Note: All estimates are standardized, bold estimates are significant on p<0.001. CI: 95% credibility interval.

To capture easily comparable results, we used standardized estimates in our analysis. Following the recommendations of Schuurman et al. (2016) we used within-subject standardization. The process was the following: first we standardized the regression coefficients for each person separately based on their within-subject variances. On the between-person level, standardization was based on the between person variances (McNeish and Hamaker, 2019). As our data collection technique was based on unequally spaced measurements the lagged effects of the model had to be corrected due to the time elapsed between the evening and morning data collection. Therefore, following the recommendation of Asparouhov et al. (2018) we used a 6 h interval as a baseline in our analyses.

Associations on the between-subject level

Regarding the associations of the PNI-S with other criterion variables in the momentary assessment, first we used repeated-measures correlation (Bakdash and Marusich, 2017) with the R package “Rmcorr” (R Core Team, 2017) to evaluate the association between vulnerable and grandiose states of narcissism and state self-esteem. Repeated-measures correlation also eliminates the problem of ergodicity between the different levels of analysis (Molenaar, 2004).

Lastly, we averaged all the momentary measurement points of narcissistic states and tested the between-subject level associations of the averaged scores with other contemporary measures of narcissism and criterion variables on Sample 3 using multiple regression. Grandiose and vulnerable states were used simultaneously as predictors to account for the unique variance each one explains.

Results

Descriptive statistics and intraclass correlation coefficients

Descriptive statistics and a correlation table on all study variables is reported in Table 2. The nomological networks of PNI-S grandiosity and vulnerability factors were compared using intraclass correlation coefficients on Sample 1. The following measures were used in comparison: Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin and Terry, 1988), Narcissistic Grandiosity Scale (NGS; Crowe et al., 2016), Maladaptive Covert Narcissism Scale (MCNS; Cheek et al., 2013), Narcissistic Vulnerability Scale (NVS; Crowe et al., 2018), Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and state self-esteem. Our results suggest that both the grandiosity (ICC ranges from 0.835 to 0.844) both the vulnerability factor (ICC ranges from 0.968 to 0.978) have similar associations with other measures of narcissism and external correlates. Furthermore, PNI-S factors were also compared to the original factors of the PNI. The ICC was high between vulnerability factors (ICC = 0.965) while moderate between grandiosity factors (ICC = 0.518). The most substantial difference in the associations of the PNI-S and PNI grandiosity factors were with measures of narcissistic vulnerability (NVS and MCNS) indicating, that the original PNI grandiosity factor also measures aspects of vulnerability, while the PNI-S grandiosity factor does not.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics, internal consistency, and associations of all study variables (Sample 1).

Exploratory factor analysis

An EFA was applied on the 7-item long measure both using the English (S1) and the Hungarian version (S2) of the test. Results indicate that the seven items form two distinct factors in both samples, factor loadings ranging from 0.31 to 0.81 (for the details, see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparing the nomological networks of the PNI-S factors with intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) in Sample 1.

Confirmatory factor analysis with covariates

In order to perform CFA with covariates analysis, we estimated the model fit of the two-factor measurement model of PNI-S on Sample 1. The results supported that the two-factor solution offers acceptable fit indices according to current traditions (RMSEA = 0.058; CFI = 0.961; TLI = 0.937; χ2 = 374.6; df = 21; p < 0.001). Standardised factor loadings ranged from 0.53 to 0.79 for the vulnerability factor and 0.23 to 0.81 for the grandiosity factor. Grandiosity and vulnerability factors were moderately positively associated (0.28). The factor loadings are presented in the Supplementary Material.

To examine the convergent and discriminant validity of the PNI-S, we applied a series of confirmatory factor analyses with covariates. The results are presented in Table 4. The grandiosity factor had a medium-strong positive relationship with the other standard measures of grandiose narcissism, but it was mostly unrelated to vulnerability measures. The vulnerability factor, on the other hand, was positively related to other measures of narcissistic vulnerability. Self-esteem and state self-esteem both had medium negative associations with the vulnerability factor while the grandiosity factor was not significantly related to any measures of self-esteem.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Associations of the PNI-S in Sample 1: confirmatory factor analysis with covariates.

Multilevel confirmatory factor analysis

The multilevel CFA used 1741 observations and offered acceptable fit indices according to current traditions (RMSEA = 0.035; CFI = 0.938; TLI = 0.900; χ2 = 960.2; df = 42; p < 0.001; SRMRwithin-subject = 0.035; SRMRbetween-subject = 0.093). Standardized factor loadings and intraclass correlations are presented on Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardised factor loadings and intraclass correlations of the multilevel CFA.

On the within-subject level the association between the grandiosity and vulnerability factors is weakly negative (r = −0.32). On the between subject level however, the association between the two factors is weakly positive (r = 0.26).

Measurement invariance

Measurement invariance was tested between the original English version of the measure and the Hungarian translation. The configural and the metric model were significantly different from each other (χ2 = 14.23, df = 5; p = 0.014), while the differences in the fit indices (configural model: χ2 = 49.44, CFI = 0.958, RMSEA = 0.062, CI [0.036–0.088]; metric model: χ2 = 71.65, CFI = 0.928, RMSEA = 0.069, CI [0.048–0.090]) were mixed. Following the recommendations of Chen (2007) whether sample sizes are adequate (total N > 300) measurement invariance between the models can be assumed if changes in the CFI measure is less than –0.01 supplemented by a change in the RMSEA measure less than 0.01. Between the configural and metric model CFI diminished with 0.03 while RMSEA increased by 0.012 indicating noninvariance. The metric and scalar model showed nonivariance (CFI measure dropped by 0.07, RMSEA increased with 0.02). These results suggest that direct cultural comparison with the PNI-S should be applied with caution and further research is needed to test the invariance of the construct in these settings.

Within-subject level analysis

In the multilevel VAR (1) model we used Bayesian estimation based on Markov Chain Monte Carlo chains in Mplus 8.3 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). We estimated statistical significance by 95% credibility intervals (CI), meaning that each parameter has a 95% chance of falling into this range. If this CI does not contain zero, we can conclude, that the estimate is different from zero (McNeish and Hamaker, 2019).

Our model (DIC = 39129.6; estimated number of parameters: 1207.3) estimates are presented in Figure 1.

Based on the model estimates both the grandiose, both the vulnerable narcissistic state produced significant autoregressive effect on the within subject level. The within-level explained variance (R2) was 0.04 for the grandiose narcissistic state while 0.14 for the vulnerable narcissistic state.

Associations on the between-subject level (momentary assessment, Sample 3)

First, to test the association between the PNI-S and state self-esteem, we used repeated-measures correlation (Bakdash and Marusich, 2017). State vulnerable narcissism was in moderate negative association with state self-esteem (r = −0.40; df = 1,621; p < 0.001; CI [−0.44 to −0.36]) while state grandiose narcissism was in a moderate positive association with it (r = 0.35; df = 1,621; p < 0.001; CI [0.31–0.40]). The two state measures of narcissism were in weak negative association (r = −0.18; df = 1,621; p < 0.001; CI [−0.23–0.12]).

Lastly, we tested the associations of the averaged scores of all measurement points of the narcissistic states with other contemporary measures of narcissism and some criterion variables with a set of multiple regression analyses. Grandiose and vulnerable states were used simultaneously as predictors to account for the unique variance each one explains. The results were in line with previous research (see Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Associations of the PNI-S in Sample 3: Regression based on averaged scores from the momentary assessments.

State vulnerable narcissism had a medium positive association with other measures of narcissistic vulnerability while state grandiose narcissism had a medium positive association with grandiosity measures. Self-esteem on the trait level and state self-esteem are both negatively associated with state vulnerable narcissism while grandiose narcissism shows positive associations with them.

Discussion

The present study was designed to develop a state-level measurement tool for grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. As the personality process behind narcissism is gaining more interest, there is a growing need for appropriate measurement tools for capturing momentary fluctuations.

In this study, the Pathological Narcissism Inventory – State Version (PNI-S) was first administered in two samples of undergraduates in a cross-sectional design to test the factor structure and between-subject level associations with contemporary measures of narcissism (S1, S2). The two-factor solution yielded an appropriate fit to our data. Vulnerable narcissistic states captured with four items offered higher internal consistency on our first sample, while the factor loadings and the internal consistency of the three-item long state-level grandiose narcissism subscale remained moderate. In S3, we tested the measurement tool in a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis, which provided appropriate fit and factor loadings on both the within-subject, both the between-subject level. These results are in line with our intention, to create a measurement tool as short as possible, not to interfere with the process we study while keeping the reliability and validity of the measure.

State level narcissistic vulnerability and grandiosity were positively associated on the between-subject level, which is in line with previous research using the PNI (Pincus et al., 2009), and it may reflect the entitlement core of narcissism (Wright and Edershile, 2018). The two factors of the PNI-S provided the expected associations with the contemporary measures of trait-level grandiose and vulnerable narcissism and gender. Previous research with the PNI argued that either the grandiosity factor might not be represented sufficiently (Crowe et al., 2019) or vulnerability should be partialed out from the grandiosity construct (Edershile et al., 2019), our results suggest that the PNI-S grandiosity factor is in strong association with other contemporary grandiosity measures (e.g., the NPI, NGS) and weakly or non-significantly associated with measures of vulnerability (NVS, MCNS/HSNS) in both cross-sectional, both ESM designs (for more details, see Tables 13, 6). Furthermore, according to our results the nomological network of the PNI-S grandiosity factor is different from the original PNI grandiosity factor especially in associations with narcissistic vulnerability (see Table 3), suggesting that the limited number of state-based items can be effective in partialing out narcissistic vulnerability from the grandiosity factor (Edershile et al., 2019).

Moreover, in accordance with our previous expectations (based on Edershile and Wright, 2021), on the within-subject level the association between vulnerable and grandiose states were negative suggesting that these states are not likely to be present at the same time in individuals. Based on these associations we argue that the PNI-S can sufficiently capture both grandiose both vulnerable narcissistic states and can also differentiate between them.

We also tested the psychometric properties and associations of vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic states on the within-and between person level during a five-day long ESM study. This differentiation is of great importance, because between-subject relations are a good source of information from dispositional, structural variables, that differentiate people from each other, while within-subject associations are offering insights into the internal dynamic process between variables and their dependence on situational factors (Bolger et al., 2003). This measurement tool was specifically designed to enable researchers gathering momentary data in multiple data-collection points.

First, we conducted a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis to examine the fit of the two-factor solution in both the within-and the between-person level. Our results suggest that the scale performs well in both settings, the association between the grandiose and vulnerable states is negative on the within-subject level, while positive on the between-subject level. According to our explanation, vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic states can be associated as overall narcissistic tendencies or traits when we compare individuals on the between-person level (similar to the narcissistic core by Krizan and Herlache, 2018), especially at higher levels of trait grandiose narcissism (Jauk et al., 2022). On the other hand, narcissism – viewed as a self-regulatory function in maintaining a grandiose self (Morf and Rhodewalt, 2001; Back, 2018) – can manifest in different within-person processes (Edershile and Wright, 2021) reflecting on the individual’s specific evaluation of specific situations. This evaluation might result in the unfolding of either grandiose or vulnerable states, and those states are not likely to be present at the same time, although possible shifts between them are suggested by clinical theories (Wink, 1996; Edershile and Wright, 2021). The results from the repeated measures correlation show a similar, somewhat weaker association, where grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic states are negatively associated. It is also worth mentioning, that this rather small effect could not be reproduced in the multilevel Var (1) model in which cross-lagged and autoregressive effects are controlled for. These findings highlight the importance of understanding the narcissistic process itself, not limited to the trait level of narcissism, as everyday functioning might be strongly affected by the internal personality process. This weak negative association should although be investigated further by future research.

Second, the within-person contemporaneous, autoregressive and cross-lagged effects of both vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic states were tested on a multilevel Var (1) model (see Figure 1). Our results suggest that the autoregressive effect (meaning the effect of the preceding narcissistic state on the currently measured one) for both grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic states is meaningful, underlining the stability aspect of these constructs, and their reliability over time (Hamaker et al., 2015). On the other hand, we did not find significant associations between the grandiosity and vulnerability constructs neither on a contemporaneous nor a crossed lagged setting. These results imply that prior states of vulnerability did not predict currently measured grandiosity and vice versa, while current states of grandiosity and vulnerability were also not significantly associated.

The relationship between narcissistic states and self-esteem also served as an important aspect of the validation process of the PNI-S. State level vulnerable narcissism was in medium negative association with both state and trait level self-esteem in all of our analyses which is in line with previous research (e.g., Miller et al., 2010, 2016, 2018). Grandiose narcissism on the other hand has shown non-significant association with self-esteem on a rather trait-like measurement (S1), but moderate positive association on our longitudinal study (S3). On the trait level this weak association is in line with the results of Hyatt et al. (2018) who used a meta-analytic approach and found that the strength of this relationship was between r = 0.10 and r = 0.43 across different samples. The stronger association on a longitudinal setting on the other hand might highlight the value of capturing the dynamic fluctuations of self-esteem in relation to narcissistic states. At the most general level, these temporal associations suggest that studying narcissism as a fluctuating process consisting of state iterations might enable a deeper understanding of the underlying personality processes of narcissism (Giacomin, 2016; Edershile et al., 2018; Edershile and Wright, 2021).

Limitations

Despite the strengths of the present study including the different languages of administration, samples, data collection techniques and statistical methods used there are also limitations worth mentioning. The measures used were all self-report measures, and they were administered in student samples overrepresented by women. While this is common practice in current narcissism research, future studies would benefit from collecting informant reports or observing the process of narcissistic personality functioning in laboratory settings and from doing so with clinically diagnosed samples. Furthermore, the administration of other currently available state measures (e.g., the NVS and the NGS) in S3 would have offered more opportunities to study convergent validity, however we aimed to limit the number of items presented to the participants in order to avoid interfering with the internal personality processes we wished to study. Further research with more diverse samples and more balanced gender distributions should also aim to study measurement invariance across genders, cultural settings and age ranges, particularly with the grandiose narcissistic states factor, in which internal consistency remained moderate.

Conclusion

The present study demonstrated the usefulness of the seven items long Pathological Narcissism Inventory – State Version (PNI-S). This measure can perform better than original trait measures of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (e.g., the NPI or the HSNS) in momentary data collection research where short and current state-related items are crucial in capturing internal states of personality processes. Compared to other currently used momentary measures the PNI-S can be applicable if the entitlement-related core of narcissism is also in focus besides vulnerability and exhibitionism/grandiosity (e.g., the NVS or the NGS) and if vulnerable aspects of narcissistic functioning is equally important in measurement compared to the process-oriented Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire (Leckelt et al., 2018). Furthermore, our results also highlighted the differences between the within-and between person associations, enabling us to take a closer look into the personality processes behind narcissistic functioning.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of Psychology, University of Groningen and the Ethical Committee of Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ME and NR planned and designed the study, collected the data, and wrote the manuscript. ME and RU analysed the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; CFI, comparative fit index; DSEM, dynamic structural equation modelling; TLI, Tucker-Lewis Index; EFA, exploratory factor analysis; ESM, Experience sampling method; MCNS, Maladaptice Covert Narcissism Scale; Multilevel VAR (1), multilevel vector autoregressive model; NPI, Narcissistic Personality Inventory; NGS, Narcissistic Grandiosity Scale; NVS, Narcissistic Vulnerability Scale; PNI, Pathological Narcissism Inventory; PNI-S, Pathological Narcissism Inventory – State Version; SEM, structural equation modelling; S1, S2, S3, Sample 1, Sample 2, Sample 3.

References

Aghababaei, N., and Błachnio, A. (2015). Well-being and the Dark Triad. Pers. Individ. Differ. 86, 365–368.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., Hamaker, E. L., and Muthén, B. (2018). Dynamic structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 25, 359–388. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1406803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Back, M. D. (2018). “The narcissistic admiration and rivalry concept,” in Handbook of Trait Narcissism. eds. A. Hermann, A. Brunell, and J. Foster (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C., Dufner, M., Gerlach, T. M., Rauthmann, J. F., and Denissen, J. J. (2013). Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: disentangling the bright and dark sides of narcissism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 1013–1037. doi: 10.1037/a0034431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakdash, J. Z., and Marusich, L. R. (2017). Repeated measures correlation. Front. Psychol. 8:456. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolger, N., Davis, A., and Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 579–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., and Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 638–656. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, W. K., and Miller, J. D. (2011). The Handbook of Narcissism and Narcissistic Personality Disorder. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, doi: 10.1002/9781118093108.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheek, J. M., Hendin, H. M., and Wink, P. M. (2013). An expanded version of the hypersensitive narcissism scale: The maladaptive covert narcissism scale. In Meeting of the Association for Research in Personality, Charlotte, NC.

Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16, 297–334. doi: 10.1007/BF02310555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, M., Carter, N. T., Campbell, W. K., and Miller, J. D. (2016). Validation of the narcissistic grandiosity scale and creation of reduced item variants. Psychol. Assess. 28, 1550–1560. doi: 10.1037/pas0000281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, M. L., Edershile, E. A., Wright, A. G., Campbell, W. K., Lynam, D. R., and Miller, J. D. (2018). Development and validation of the narcissistic vulnerability scale: an adjective rating scale. Psychol. Assess. 30, 978–983. doi: 10.1037/pas0000578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, M. L., Lynam, D. R., Campbell, W. K., and Miller, J. D. (2019). Exploring the structure of narcissism: Toward an integrated solution. J. Pers. 87, 1151–1169.

Google Scholar

Czarna, A. Z., Zajenkowski, M., and Dufner, M. (2018). “How does it feel to be a narcissist? Narcissism and emotions,” in Handbook of Trait Narcissism. Cham: Springer, 255–263.

Google Scholar

Edershile, E. A., Simms, L. J., and Wright, A. G. (2019). A multivariate analysis of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory’s nomological network. Assessment. 26, 619–629.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Edershile, E. A., and Wright, A. G. (2021). Fluctuations in grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic states: a momentary perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 120, 1386–1414. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/8gkpm

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egan, V., Chan, S., and Shorter, G. W. (2014). The Dark Triad, happiness and subjective well-being. Pers. Individ. Differ. 67, 17–22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Engyel, M., Urbán, R., Bandi, S., and Nagy, L. (2022). Dimensionality of narcissism: A bifactorial model of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory using single-stimulus response formats. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 41, 4677–4691. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00971-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Hutteman, R., Dufner, M., Küfner, A. C., Egloff, B., et al. (2017). Puffed-up but shaky selves: state self-esteem level and variability in narcissists. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 112, 769–786. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacomin, M. (2016). Investigating grandiose narcissism as a personality process. (Doctoral dissertation). Available at: https://scholars.wlu.ca/etd/1864/ (Accessed September 16, 2022).

Google Scholar

Glover, N., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Crego, C., and Widiger, T. A. (2012). The five-factor narcissism inventory: a five-factor measure of narcissistic personality traits. J. Pers. Assess. 94, 500–512. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.670680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A., and McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Hamaker, E. L., Ceulemans, E., Grasman, R. P. P. P., and Tuerlinckx, F. (2015). Modeling affect dynamics: state of the art and future challenges. Emot. Rev. 7, 316–322. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience Sampling Method: Measuring the Quality of Everyday Life. New York, NY: Sage.

Google Scholar

Hendin, H. M., and Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: a reexamination of Murray's Narcism scale. J. Res. Pers. 31, 588–599. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1997.2204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoekstra, R., Vugteveen, J., Warrens, M. J., and Kruyen, P. M. (2019). An empirical analysis of alleged misunderstandings of coefficient alpha. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 22, 351–364. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2018.1547523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyatt, C. S., Sleep, C. E., Lamkin, J., Maples-Keller, J. L., Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., et al. (2018). Narcissism and self-esteem: a nomological network analysis. PloS One 13:e0201088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauk, E., Ulbrich, L., Jorschick, P., Höfler, M., Kaufman, S. B., and Kanske, P. (2022). The nonlinear association between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: An individual data meta‐analysis. J. Pers. 90, 703–726.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kaufman, S. B., Weiss, B., Miller, J. D., and Campbell, W. K. (2018). Clinical correlates of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism: a personality perspective. J. Pers. Disord. 34, 107–130.

Google Scholar

Koo, T. K., and Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J. Chiropr. Med. 15, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krizan, Z., and Herlache, A. D. (2018). The narcissism spectrum model: a synthetic view of narcissistic personality. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 22, 3–31. doi: 10.1177/1088868316685018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruyen, P. M., Emons, W. H., and Sijtsma, K. (2013). On the shortcomings of shortened tests: a literature review. Int. J. Test. 13, 223–248. doi: 10.1080/15305058.2012.703734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leckelt, M., Wetzel, E., Gerlach, T. M., Ackerman, R. A., Miller, J. D., Chopik, W. J., et al. (2018). Validation of the narcissistic admiration and rivalry questionnaire short scale (NARQ-S) in convenience and representative samples. Psychol. Assess. 30, 86–96. doi: 10.1037/pas0000433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liljequist, D., Elfving, B., and Skavberg Roaldsen, K. (2019). Intraclass correlation–a discussion and demonstration of basic features. PLoS One 14:e0219854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeish, D., and Hamaker, E. L. (2019). A primer on two-level dynamic structural equation models for intensive longitudinal data in Mplus. Psychol. Methods. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/met0000250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Back, M. D., Lynam, D. R., and Wright, A. G. (2021). Narcissism today: what we know and what we need to learn. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 30, 519–525. doi: 10.1177/09637214211044109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Gentile, B., Carter, N. T., Crowe, M., Hoffman, B. J., and Campbell, W. K. (2018). A comparison of the nomological networks associated with forced-choice and likert formats of the narcissistic personality inventory. J. Pers. Assess. 100, 259–267. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2017.1310731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Gaughan, E. T., Gentile, B., Maples, J., and Keith Campbell, W. (2011). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. J. Pers. 79, 1013–1042.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Hyatt, C. S., and Campbell, W. K. (2017). Controversies in narcissism. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 13, 291–315. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., McCain, J. L., Few, L. R., Crego, C., Widiger, T. A., et al. (2016). Thinking structurally about narcissism: an examination of the five-factor narcissism inventory and its components. J. Personal. Disord. 30, 1–18. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Vize, C., Crowe, M., Sleep, C., Maples-Keller, J. L., et al. (2018). Vulnerable narcissism is (mostly) a disorder of neuroticism. J. Pers. 86, 186–199. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Widiger, T. A., and Campbell, W. K. (2010). Narcissistic personality disorder and the DSM-V. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 640–649. doi: 10.1037/a0019529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molenaar, P. C. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement 2, 201–218. doi: 10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morf, C. C., and Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: a dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol. Inq. 12, 177–196. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mota, S., Leckelt, M., Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Humberg, M., Schröder-Abé, M., et al. (2019). A comprehensive examination of narcissists’ self-perceived and actual socioemotional cognition ability. Collabra Psycho. 5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998–2015). Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Pallant, J. (2011). Survival manual. A step by step Guide to Data Analysis Using SPSS, 4th ed.. Allen & Unwin, Berkshire, UK.

Google Scholar

Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G., and Levy, K. N. (2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory. Psychol. Assess. 21, 365–379. doi: 10.1037/a0016530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/ (Accessed September 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Raskin, R., and Terry, H. (1988). A principle-components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 890–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodewalt, F., Madrian, J. C., and Cheney, S. (1998). Narcissism, self-knowledge organization, and emotional reactivity: The effect of daily experiences on self-esteem and affect. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 75–87.

Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, doi: 10.1515/9781400876136.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, S. A., Hooley, J. M., and Steshenko, Y. (2007). Distinguishing grandiosity from self-esteem: development of the narcissistic grandiosity scale. Manuscript in preparation.

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., and Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuurman, N. K., Ferrer, E., de Boer-Sonnenschein, M., and Hamaker, E. L. (2016). How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychol. Methods 21, 206–221. doi: 10.1037/met0000062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., and Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy?: self-esteem matters. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87:400.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., and Ullman, J. B. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Wetzel, E., Roberts, B. W., Fraley, R. C., and Brown, A. (2016). Equivalence of narcissistic personality inventory constructs and correlates across scoring approaches and response formats. J. Res. Pers. 61, 87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wink, P. (1996). “Narcissism,” in Personality characteristics of the personality disorders. ed. G. C. Costello (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons), 146–172.

Google Scholar

Wright, A. G., and Edershile, E. A. (2018). Issues resolved and unresolved in pathological narcissism. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 21, 74–79. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. G., Lukowitsky, M. R., Pincus, A. L., and Conroy, D. E. (2010). The higher order factor structure and gender invariance of the pathological narcissism inventory. Assessment 17, 467–483. doi: 10.1177/1073191110373227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeigler-Hill, V., Clark, C. B., and Pickard, J. D. (2008). Narcissistic subtypes and contingent self-esteem: do all narcissists base their self-esteem on the same domains? J. Pers. 76, 753–774. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00503.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: narcissism, grandiosity, vulnerability, momentary measurement, narcissistic states, confirmatory factor analysis, multilevel analysis, experience sampling method

Citation: Engyel M, de Ruiter NMP and Urbán R (2022) Momentarily narcissistic? Development of a short, state version of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory applicable in momentary assessment. Front. Psychol. 13:992271. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.992271

Received: 12 July 2022; Accepted: 13 September 2022;
Published: 26 October 2022.

Edited by:

Erin M. Buchanan, Harrisburg University of Science and Technology, United States

Reviewed by:

Emanuel Jauk, Medical University of Graz, Austria
Elizabeth A. Krusemark, Millsaps College, United States
Christopher Breeden, Wingate University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Engyel, de Ruiter and Urbán. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Márton Engyel, ZW5neWVsLm1hcnRvbkBwcGsuZWx0ZS5odQ==.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.