Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 31 October 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Restricted and repetitive behavior in children with autism during the first three years of life: A systematic review

  • 1Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 2Institute on Community Integration, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States

Restricted and repetitive behavior (RRB) is a core diagnostic feature of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Previous research shows that RRB is prevalent early in life and observed in neurotypical development as well. Less is known, however, about early RRB patterns, developmental trajectories, and the relation to outcomes for autistic children. The purpose of this systematic review was to synthesize findings from studies examining RRB in autistic children from birth through age 3. A detailed protocol was designed a priori based on PRISMA guidelines for systematic reviews. From the published literature, 41 peer reviewed journal articles were identified and included in this review. Our synthesis of the literature suggests that differences in RRB are evident prior to age 2 in children with or who go onto be diagnosed with autism. These differences were evident for both frequency and intensity of RRB across multiple topographies. There were mixed results regarding functional outcomes associated with early RRB, such as cognitive and adaptive behavior, though relations appeared to become stronger as children aged beyond toddlerhood. Notably, level of RRB appears unrelated to autism severity in young autistic children. A wide range of RRB have been reported to be elevated in autistic children during the first years of life, including repetitive motor behaviors, atypical sensory behaviors, insistence on sameness (IS), and self-injurious behaviors (SIBs). In contrast to studies of older children, RRB in very young autistic children do not appear to be associated with functional outcomes but may be valuable to include in early screening efforts.

Systematic review registration: https://osf.io/huzf3, unique identifier: doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/HUZF3.

Introduction

Although autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by both restricted repetitive behaviors (RRB) and social communication characteristics (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), considerably less research has focused on the domain of RRB (Richler et al., 2010; Troyb et al., 2016). What is known about RRB in very early childhood is particularly limited. To date, it is unclear whether and how RRB differentiates autistic children1 from neurotypically-developing children (TD) and children with developmental delays (DD). Furthermore, there is limited research tracking the developmental trajectory of RRB and its relations to child outcomes (Leekam et al., 2011). The heterogenous range of RRB topographies and the diverse terminology across research groups, in combination with a lack of universally utilized RRB measures within clinical and service delivery settings, make it difficult to piece together the current state of knowledge on RRB. Gaps in our understanding of RRB hinder the domain's utility to inform identification and intervention efforts in the early years of life when they have the highest potential to capitalize on maximal neural and behavioral plasticity. This review is a step toward filling these gaps by synthesizing the available literature on RRB in autistic children from birth through age 3.

Restricted repetitive behaviors are a class of behaviors characterized by high frequency, repetition, and an insistence or desire for sameness in the environment (Kanner, 1943). In general, RRBs are a continuum of behaviors that may include repetitive motor movements like hand flapping, preoccupation with parts, and circumscribed or restricted interests/routines. While taxonomies of RRB vary (e.g., Lewis and Bodfish, 1998), the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.) outlines four patterns of RRB that include (1) repetitive motor, use of objects or speech, or stereotyped behaviors; (2) rigidity including insistence on sameness (IS), ritualized or inflexible adherence to routines; (3) restricted behaviors including circumscribed interests and fixation; and (4) atypical sensory responses including hypo- and hyper-reactivity as well as sensory-seeking behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Of the patterns defined, atypical sensory responses and interests is relatively newer to the RRB domain.

The addition of atypical sensory responses to ASD diagnostic criteria has resulted in increased attention to the taxonomy of RRB. Researchers have conducted numerous factor analyses pertaining to RRB presentation within ASD, primarily using caregiver-reported measures. Turner (1999) subdivided the broad range of RRB into two factors: (1) “lower-order” behaviors characterized by repetitive movements including stereotyped motor movements, repetitive manipulation of objects, and repetitive forms of self-injurious behavior (SIB) and (2) “higher order” behaviors including object attachments, IS, repetitive language, and circumscribed interests. Whereas, lower-order repetitive behaviors are characterized by motor rigidity, higher-order repetitive behaviors are considered to be more complex and include cognitive rigidity. Relatedly, RRB topographies have been organized under repetitive sensory motor (RSM) and IS (Bishop et al., 2013; Uljarević et al., 2017; Hiruma et al., 2021). Additional factors, including SIB, have also been identified from validation studies (Lam and Aman, 2007; Mirenda et al., 2010; Bishop et al., 2013) of the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al., 2000), while a more recent study suggested an additional separate factor for stereotyped speech (Hiruma et al., 2021).

It is currently unclear if and how RRB differentiates autistic children from those who do not have ASD in the first years of life. Restricted and repetitive behavior are not mutually exclusive nor phenomenologically unique to autism. For example, in a review, Leekam et al. (2011) found that all patterns of RRB were also present in children with other disorders (e.g., Tourette syndrome, fragile X, Rett's, and Down's syndromes) as well as in TD children. It is thus necessary to determine if there is sufficient evidence to suggest that aspects of RRB, such as frequency, severity, and developmental trajectory differ between autistic children and TD children and children with DD. The current lack of clarity limits the potential of including aspects of RRB in early screening. This is a critical limitation as accumulating research suggests that RRB may differentiate children who later develop ASD as early as 6–12 months of age (Baranek, 1999; Zwaigenbaum, 2005; Elison et al., 2014; Wolff et al., 2014; Purpura et al., 2017; Miller et al., 2021), perhaps preceding many social communication characteristics which are found to emerge later in toddlerhood (Ozonoff et al., 2010). Furthermore, studies indicate that RRB are reliable predictors of a stable ASD diagnosis between 2 and 9 years of age (Lord and Luyster, 2006; Lord et al., 2006). Closer examination into how RRB looks in autistic children in comparison to TD children and children at elevated likelihood for ASD such as those with DD is needed in order to evaluate the utility of RRB as early markers of ASD.

Another gap in our understanding of RRB is in how it changes across early development in autistic children, as well as how such change compares to children who are TD. There is evidence that RRB in autistic individuals is developmentally dynamic (Leekam et al., 2011; Berry et al., 2018). A key complication is that RRB are also common in TD children during early childhood and similarly undergo rapid change through early school age. Some topographies of RRB, such as motor stereotypies, may peak in the first year of life and gradually decrease thereafter (Thelen, 1979; MacLean et al., 1991; Sifre et al., 2021). Other RRB, such as IS and behavioral rigidity, are often observed from toddlerhood through preschool age, for example, when it is common for young children to request the same song, movie, or clothing repeatedly, to carry around a certain item, or to insist that a task be completed in a specific order (Evans et al., 1997; Zohar and Felz, 2001). An understanding of how RRB associated with ASD changes over early childhood, and in relation to TD, has the potential to inform developmental mechanisms as well as timing of intervention when warranted.

Relatedly, relations of RRB to other developmental outcomes such as social-communication, cognitive ability, and adaptive functioning are also unclear. This knowledge is critical in decisions to intervene for RRB (i.e., some forms of RRB may presage outcomes that warrant prevention, while others may be innocuous or even beneficial). In comparison to social communication characteristics, much more controversy surrounds the intervention of RRB, namely in regards to if and what specific RRB require intervention. On the one hand, RRB can scaffold the learning of more complex skills. For example, motor stereotypies may be foundational to the acquisition of goal-directed behavior, while IS or ritualistic behavior may provide stability and order to a preschooler's erratic and increasingly demanding environment. Relatedly, autistic adults report that RRB are functional and adaptive, enhancing their abilities to function, control stimulation, and cope with stress (Manor-Binyamini and Schreiber-Divon, 2019). Indiscriminant prevention or suppression of RRB has the potential for both short- and long-term adverse effects. Conversely, RRB also have the potential to interfere with learning and the acquisition of skills if they serve as barriers to opportunities and experiences necessary for healthy development. For example, socialization may become limited if a child is engaging in high or interfering levels of RRB (Nadig et al., 2010). Furthermore, parents of autistic children report that RRB is one of the most challenging features associated with ASD (South et al., 2005; Raulston and Machalicek, 2018) and indicate high levels of stress associated with RRB (Hayes and Watson, 2013). Given these complex, yet critical perspectives, it is important to clarify what is known about early RRB in relation to key functional outcomes.

In summary, limited knowledge of RRB in very early childhood hinders the utility of RRB as a core feature of ASD in efforts to further refine early identification and intervention. With increasing prevalence estimates of ASD, recent clinical advancements in diagnosing ASD at a younger age, and thus ability to provide intervention services earlier, it is critical to clarify what is currently known of the distinctiveness of RRB in young children with ASD. Thus, the purpose of this review was to synthesize findings from studies examining RRB in autistic children from birth through age 3. Our review addresses three questions: (1) What is known about the types of RRB that differentiate autistic children from TD children and children with DD?; (2) How do the frequency, the duration, and the severity of RRB in early child development change over time in children with ASD?; and (3) What is known about RRB in relation to child outcomes for autistic children birth through age 3?

Materials and methods

Our study protocol was drafted using an earlier version of Van den Akker et al. (2020), a product that was a result of the Society for Improving Psychological Science. The protocol is registered with the Open Science Framework on May 22nd, 2022 (https://osf.io/huzf3).

Study selection

This review included peer-reviewed, quantitative studies that examined RRB in autistic children birth through age 3 (0 to < 48 months). Studies that included children older than 48 months were eligible for inclusion if the mean age of autistic participants was under 48 months or if data on autistic participants under 48 months could be extracted. Studies that did not include autistic children were excluded. Qualitative, intervention, non-human animal studies, gray literature, book chapters, reviews, and studies written in a language other than English were also excluded. Studies that fit our inclusion criteria but did not contain data contributing to at least one research question were also excluded (e.g., Ramsey et al., 2018; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2018; Stephenson et al., 2021).

Eligible studies were located through a search of PubMed, PsycINFO, Academic Search Premier, Education Source, and Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) using the following keywords and roots: (autis* or asd) AND (restricted repetitive behavior* OR rrb) AND (early develop* OR earlychild* OR birth to three). This search procedure was originally conducted in September of 2019 and identified 357 studies. After duplicates were removed, 288 studies were screened by title and abstract following the inclusion criteria above, identifying 51 studies for full text review. Twenty-two studies met eligibility criteria. A hand search of the references of these studies identified an additional 12 studies for a total of 34 studies. A second search using the same procedure was conducted in April of 2021, identifying 7 additional studies for a total of 41 studies included in this review (see Figure 1). Studies identified through this process represent literature published between 1999 and 2020. Study selection was independently conducted by the first two authors on Rayyan, a free web application that allows for blind coding of studies for systematic reviews (Ouzzani et al., 2016). All inter-rater discrepancies were resolved by consensus.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart.

Data collection

A coding rubric was jointly developed by the three authors and was iteratively updated throughout the data collection process. Extracted data included (a) study descriptives (i.e., authors, date of publication); (b) study methodology [i.e., study purpose, RRB measures used (direct or indirect)]; (c) sample characteristics [i.e., age, sex, race/ethnicity, diagnosis, higher-familial-likelihood sample (infant siblings of an autistic child), prospective sample, group matching]; and (d) findings pertaining to research questions. To enable uniform reporting of sample characteristics across studies, the research team calculated descriptive statistics (i.e., sample size, age mean, and percentages of female/male/racial/ethnic groups) as needed based on the information provided.

The first author coded all 41 studies while the second author independently coded 37% (n = 15) of the studies. Periodic consensus meetings were held throughout data collection to discuss inter-rater discrepancies. Interrater agreement was calculated by [agreements/(agreements + disagreements)] * 100. Mean IRA was 94% (range 80–100%).

Data analysis

RQ1 was addressed using studies that compared RRB in autistic children to TD children and children with DD (n = 26). RQ2 was addressed using studies with longitudinal or repeated measures analyses that examined changes in dimension(s) of RRB over a minimum of two time points (n = 24). Lastly, RQ3 was addressed using studies that examined RRB in relation to concurrent and/or distal child outcomes (n = 24).

RRB categories

Given the heterogeneity of RRB topographies, a lack of uniformity in the terminology and measures used across research groups, we synthesized research using five broad categories of RRB:

Overall-RRB: Findings pertaining to global RRB measures and scores that reflect topographies from different patterns of RRB.

Repetitive sensory-motor behavior (RSM): Findings pertaining to stereotyped movements and preoccupations with a specific interest, activity, or object.

Insistence on sameness (IS): Includes findings pertaining to compulsive, ritualistic behaviors, and resistance to changes in routines.

Self-injurious behavior (SIB): Includes findings pertaining to repetitive behaviors that cause or have the potential to cause bodily injury ranging from minor to severe.

Atypical sensory behaviors: Includes findings pertaining to hyper- or hypo-activity to sensory input and sensory-seeking behaviors. Findings here are limited to studies that used a sensory-specific measure.

Table 1 presents the measures reflected in the literature. We organized findings according to these RRB categories for several reasons. Repetitive sensory-motor behavior and Insistence on sameness are included as previous studies have indicated that the heterogeneous range of RRB topographies load most consistently into these two factors (Bishop et al., 2013; Uljarević et al., 2017; Hiruma et al., 2021). Self-injurious behavior is also included as factor analyses of the RBS-R—a parent-report measure frequently used in our included studies—report this as a third and separate factor from RSM and IS (Lam and Aman, 2007; Mirenda et al., 2010; Bishop et al., 2013). Finally, sensory behaviors is included as this topography has been added to the RRB domain as part of the diagnostic criteria for ASD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). We reasoned that organizing findings according to this categorization would facilitate cross-study synthesis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Measures used for defining RRB categories and outcomes.

Group definitions

To address RQ1, we defined ASD, TD, and DD groups as the following:

ASD Group: Children who are identified as having ASD, autism, autistic disorder, Aperger' syndrome, PDD-NOS, and higher-familial-likelihood children with ASD (HL-ASD). If a study differentiated between these diagnoses (e.g., compared autistic children to children with PDD-NOS), these groups were combined to create one ASD group with aggregated results.

TD Group: Children who are identified as typically-developing, children with no known disabilities or concerns, and lower-familial-likelihood without ASD (LL-Neg) and higher-familial-likelihood children without ASD (HL-Neg).

DD Group: Children who are identified with a global developmental, speech/language, or motor delay/difficulties or children with a non-spectrum condition such as a learning or intellectual disability.

Dimensions of RRB change

To address RQ2, we examined changes over time in the following RRB dimensions:

Severity: Changes in overall/composite RRB scores.

Frequency: Changes in the number of times a participant/group engages in a specific RRB topography/category.

Duration: Changes in the length of time a participant/group engages in a specific RRB topography/category.

Early child outcomes

To address RQ3, we presented findings on relations between the five RRB categories described above and concurrent or distal measures of the following child outcomes: ASD severity, RRB severity, social-communication/interaction (including expressive and receptive communication), cognitive ability, adaptive function, motor, and self-regulation. Table 1 presents the measures that examined each outcome. Restricted and repetitive behavior was included as an outcome to evaluate associations between categories of RRB both concurrently and over time (e.g., whether early RRB predicts later RRB). Outcomes were not specified a priori; all relations between RRB categories and outcomes that emerged from the reviewed studies are reported.

Results

Study characteristics

The description of studies included in this review are presented in Table 2. Hereafter, studies are referred to as Sn, where n refers to the order a study appears in Table 2. Altogether, the studies in our sample included a total of 5,816 participants. Sample sizes ranged from N = 20 (S26) to N = 760 (S22). Male participants made up the majority of study samples (i.e., ranging from 50.0% in S11, S14, and S20 to 92.3% in S5). Twenty-six (63.4%) studies reported some racial/ethnic information, indicating majority-White samples (i.e., ranging from 39.7% in S14 to 92.5% in S8). Nineteen (46.3%) studies used prospective ASD samples and seven (17.1%) studies used higher-familial-likelihood samples. Among the 30 studies that compared ASD samples to TD and/or DD samples, seven (23.3%) studies matched participants by chronological age and six (20.0%) studies matched participants by maturational age. Twenty-two (53.7%) studies used direct measures of RRB, 12 (29.3%) studies used indirect measures, while seven (17.1%) studies used a combination of both.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study characteristics.

RQ1: Comparisons of RRB across groups

Complete findings addressing RQ1 are presented in Supplementary Table 1 and are summarized below. Overall, far more studies examined RRB in autistic children in comparison to TD children (n = 24) than to children with DD (n = 13). The most frequently examined variables were RSM (n = 16), overall-RRB (n = 12), and sensory behaviors (n = 10), with comparisons of IS (n = 3) and SIB (n = 2) less common. Group differences in RSM, overall-RRB, and sensory behaviors were examined across the birth through age 3 developmental period, while group differences in IS and SIB were primarily focused on birth through age 2. In studies that address RQ1, SIB has only been measured indirectly; however, all other RRB categories have been measured using a combination of direct and indirect measures. Due to the low number of studies comparing SIB, these results are presented in Supplementary Table 1 but are not further interpreted in this section.

During the birth through age 3 developmental period, overall-RRB and RSM were significantly higher in autistic children than in TD children and children with DD. Comparisons of IS and sensory behaviors predominantly were mixed, indicating that autistic children either have similar (S1; S9; S31; S39; S41) or significantly higher (S3; S11; S18; S31; S33; S40; S41) IS or sensory behaviors relative to TD children and children with DD. One study (S1) found that children with ASD had significantly lower RSM (specifically object usage) than children with DD. However, no studies indicated that autistic children have less IS or sensory impairments than TD children or children with DD. These findings remained consistent across all ages and measurement approaches (i.e., direct or indirect).

RQ2: Changes in RRB over time

Developmental changes in RRB by severity, frequency, and duration patterns over time were only reported in a subset of the longitudinal and repeated measures studies (n = 12). There were notable increases in severity, frequency, and duration for ASD groups. Increases in circumscribed interests, unusual preoccupations, compulsions, and rituals (IS), as well as hand and finger mannerisms and repetitive use of objects (RSM) and sensory behaviors were reported in four studies (S15; S36; S26; S40). Conversely, decreasing (S16; S19) or stable (S5; S18) levels of overall-RRB were reported in three studies, while two studies reported decreasing or stable trajectories in specific topographies [i.e., motor stereotypies (S20), SIB (S36), and sensory seeking behavior (S41)]. One study reported relatively stable RSM and increasing IS (S32). One study reported increases in overall duration of RRB over time (S17).

RQ3: Relations between RRB and child outcomes

Findings addressing RQ3 are presented in Table 3. The RRB categories most frequently examined in relation to child outcomes were RSM (n = 13) and overall-RRB (n = 12), followed by IS (n = 5), sensory behaviors (n = 5), and SIB (n = 4). The child outcomes most frequently examined were cognitive ability (n = 19), social-communication/interaction (n = 18), and RRB severity (n = 14), followed by adaptive function (n = 10), ASD severity (n = 6), motor ability (n = 3), and self-regulation (n = 2). In studies that address RQ3, all RRB categories were measured using a mix of direct and indirect measures and outcome measures were both concurrent and distal. Due to the low number of studies examining motor ability and self-regulation, their relations with RRB categories are presented in Table 3 but are not further interpreted in this section.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Relations between RRB and early childhood outcomes.

RRB categories in relation to ASD severity

Three studies did offer some evidence that, in general, more severe RRB was associated with higher ASD severity: S19 found that ADOS RRB scores were positively correlated with ADOS total scores for both male and female participants; S35 found that repetitive movements with objects (based on the RMRIS of CSBS) at age 2 was positively correlated with age 3 ADOS total scores; and S36 found that presence of sensory interests at age 3–5 was positively correlated with CARS total scores from ages 8 through 10.

RRB categories in relation to RRB severity

Studies revealed mixed findings, indicating either no relations or more commonly positive relations (i.e., in general greater frequency or severity of RRB was associated with more severe concurrent or distal RRB). No studies indicated negative relations wherein greater RRB was associated with less frequent or severe concurrent or distal RRB.

RRB categories in relation to social-communication/interaction

Study findings were mixed, indicating either no or negative relations (i.e., in general greater frequency or severity of RRB was associated with lower social-communication/interaction outcomes).

RRB categories to in relation cognitive ability

The studies revealed mixed findings regarding the relations of overall-RRB to cognitive ability and RSM to cognitive ability, indicating either no or less commonly negative relations (i.e., higher overall-RRB/RSM were associated with lower cognitive ability). Restricted and repetitive behaviors by age 3 were significantly negatively correlated with non-verbal cognitive skills (S30) and also significantly negatively associated with stereotyped behavior in particular (S34). Cognitive ability was generally shown to be unrelated to IS, SIB, and sensory behaviors. Only one study (S25) found a positive relation between higher-order RRB (as measured by the ADI-R) and cognitive ability.

RRB categories and adaptive function

Study findings were mixed, indicating either no or negative relations between RRB and adaptive function. In general, greater frequency or severity of RRB was associated with lower adaptive function.

Findings of relations between each of the five RRB categories and child outcomes remained consistent whether direct or indirect measures of RRB were used and with participant samples spanning the birth through age 3 developmental period. With the exception of relations between RRB categories and ASD severity, which were predominantly examined distally, relations between RRB categories and all other child outcomes were examined both concurrently and distally.

Discussion

We conducted a systematic review of studies examining RRB in autistic children from birth through age 3 to clarify what is currently known of the distinctiveness of RRB in young autistic children, the developmental trajectory of RRB in early childhood, and the relations between RRB and child outcomes. We identified 41 studies meeting inclusion criteria for review, including 12 studies containing longitudinal data. We found that existing studies suggest that RRB frequency and severity during the period of birth through age 3 are significantly higher in autistic children in comparison to TD children and children with DD, though differences for the latter comparison were less consistent. While the studies we reviewed relied on a range of measurement approaches, there was in general agreement across indirect (e.g., parent report) and direct observational measures. Of note, there was converging evidence from multiple studies that increased RRB associated with ASD is evident during early toddlerhood and prior to age 2. While there were no studies identified in our sample examining RRB prior to 12 months of age, one paper published since our search closed reported elevated RSM in children later diagnosed with ASD at age 9 months (Miller et al., 2021). This contributes additional evidence suggesting that elevated RRB may be an early behavioral marker of ASD.

We further found that most topographies of RRB (i.e., RSM, IS, SIB, sensory) were largely unrelated to autism severity during the first years of life. This suggests that many varieties of repetitive behavior may be seen in young children who have or are presymptomatic/prodromal for ASD irrespective of degree of clinical severity. This is in contrast to findings from studies of older children, where modest relations between multiple forms of RRB and ASD severity have been reported (e.g. Uljarević et al., 2022). Interestingly, we did see some evidence that RRB was negatively associated with language skills (e.g., S15; S16; S30), though relatively few studies examined these relations. Similar to autism severity, we found that IS, SIB, and sensory behaviors were largely unrelated to cognitive ability. This pattern was observed across multiple studies and stands in contrast to what has been reported in studies of older children and adults (e.g., Gabriels et al., 2005; Esbensen et al., 2009; Gotham et al., 2013). In those studies, RRB is reported to be negatively associated with cognitive function (Duerden et al., 2012). However, consistent with findings from older children (e.g. Lam et al., 2008), six studies reported associations between RSM and cognitive ability (S14; S27; S32; S34; S35, S38), though three others reported no such association (S23; S36; S40). Findings related to adaptive function were particularly mixed with some studies reporting no association and others reporting a small to moderate negative association, though no study reported a positive association between adaptive function and RRB.

It is notable that numerous studies included in our review consistently reported no association between RRB and either cognitive and adaptive function. We posit that the apparent inconsistency concerning relations of RRB to symptom severity, cognitive, and adaptive function likely reflects developmental effects whereby the range of RRB displayed by young children may be much broader and less reflective of other clinical features (Esbensen et al., 2009). Such associations likely emerge later as symptoms consolidate and as normative aspects of RRB lessen over early childhood. During toddlerhood, RRB associated with autism may occur at higher rates than in other children but may nonetheless serve similar adaptive functions. We did identify some evidence to this effect in the reviewed literature, wherein relations with RRB either emerged at later ages within a longitudinal sample (e.g., S36, S40) or were present in older toddlers/preschoolers but not in younger toddlers (e.g., S18).

There were several methodological factors that may also contribute to mixed findings. First, the studies we reviewed varied in sample characteristics (e.g., age, sex ratio, prospective vs. higher-familial-likelihood sample) and design (e.g., prospective, retrospective, longitudinal, cross sectional). Relatedly, a variety of direct and indirect RRB measures were used between and within studies. For example, S34 found that overall-RRB, as measured by the RBS-R, was negatively correlated with VABS ABC scores, while S24 found that overall-RRB, as measured by the ADI-R, was not related to VABS ABC scores. S35 looked at relations between RSM (based on RSMS of CSBS video samples vs. home video samples) and MSEL NVDQ and VDQ. They found that RSM, as measured by the RSMS on CSBS video samples, was negatively correlated with MSEL NVDQ, while RSM, as measured by the RSMS on home video samples, was not correlated with either MSEL outcomes. This suggests that both measurement and context may be important sources of variability. As such, it may be advisable to operationalize RRB constructs by combining measurement approaches.

A wide range of measures were utilized across the studies we reviewed, and it is clear there is no single standard measure of RRB. Variability in measurement approaches somewhat hinders attempts to synthesize the RRB literature around specific subtypes given that different research groups define and measure constructs differently. This was particularly true for RSM, IS, and sensory behavior subcategories. For example, studies focused on RSM examined topographies spanning items such as “repetitive movements with objects,” “repetitive movements of body,” “excessive interest,” “clutching objects,” “sticky attention,” “presence of preoccupations with abnormal intensity/focus,” “preoccupation with parts of objects,” “unusual posturing,” “repetitive leg and arm movements,” “twiddling/waving-,” “banging/tapping-,” “mouthing-,” “staring at/fixating-,” and “rubbing objects.” While these may all represent the construct of RSM, they are discrete topographies that most likely vary as a function of age and developmental level. Given this diversity of item-level topographies, coupled with differences in their definitions and measurements across research teams, nuanced findings regarding how RSM, IS, and sensory behaviors compare across ASD, TD, and DD groups and their relations to child outcomes are not surprising.

While existing findings suggest that RRB may differentiate autistic children from TD children by late infancy at the group level, the magnitude of such differences appears relatively modest. Indeed, most of the studies we reviewed, and in particular the longitudinal studies, suggest differences related to RRB become increasingly evident over toddlerhood. For example, S40 found that differences in RSM, IS, and SIB between autistic children and TD children became more pronounced at 24 relative to 12 months of age. They also found that RBS-R total inventory scores and subscales comprising the RSM and IS categories were associated with VABS ABC- and socialization scores at 24 months of age but that this association was not evident at 12 months of age. S36 found that presence of sensory interests at age 3–5 years, but not at age 1–2 years, were positively associated with CARS total scores and negatively associated with non-verbal reasoning at age 8–10 years. Likewise, adherence to routines/rituals at age 3–5 years, but not at age 1–2 years, were positively associated with RBS-R total scores at age 8–10 years. In summary, it is plausible that RRB in very early childhood inconsistently distinguish autistic children and are not yet predictive of child outcomes. Although there appears to be rationale for including RRB items in conjunction with others for the purpose of early screening, it would appear based on the literature to date that RRB alone are not sufficiently indicative of later outcomes in infants as currently measured. One issue for further study is whether alternative measurement approaches to RRB might offer greater sensitivity and predictive validity.

Limitations

There are several limitations that merit consideration. First, this was not an exhaustive review of all studies that included data on RRB in autistic children from birth through age 3. Rather, we limited our sample to studies that provided data addressing our research questions. Therefore, the comparison groups and measurement strategies utilized varied across studies. Second, our review of research on some categories of RRB were limited to only a handful of published reports and as such, should be considered in aggregate with caution. For example, we did not interpret findings related to SIB given limited evidence. The handful of existing studies that examined SIB found that these behaviors do not differentiate those with ASD or DD in infancy; however, most measurement tools are not specific to early childhood. For example, self-directed motor behavior or proto-SIB (e.g., hitting head with hand in a manner unlikely to result in injury) may not rise to the level of a “problem” as measured by common instruments (e.g., ADI-R, RBS-R). Relatedly, there are potential sources of differences in findings related to control group type that we were unable to detect. Specifically, several studies utilized mental age matching to compose their comparison groups. Given the limited number of these studies and the heterogeneity among them, we were unable to conclude whether this factor was related to the strength and/or direction of study findings. Finally, there are aspects of our review criteria that may introduce bias. These include (1) restricting articles under consideration to English only, which over-represents predominantly White, Western populations and (2) searching databases of published articles, which introduces the potential for publication bias related to positive findings. Such sources of bias are not unique to this review but nonetheless warrant consideration.

Conclusion and future directions

To better understand the utility of early RRB in informing early identification and intervention efforts, we synthesized the available literature on RRB in autistic children ages birth through three. We found that the frequency and severity of early RRB are significantly higher in autistic children in comparison to TD children and children with DD. Furthermore, there are marked differences in the developmental trajectory of RRB in autistic children in comparison to TD children during the period of birth through age 3. These findings suggest that both absolute level of current RRB as well as change in RRB over time may be useful in the early screening for ASD. Early RRB, however, may be less informative for predicting functional outcomes. We found no consistent relations between RRB categories and ASD severity and mixed relations between RRB categories and social communication/interactions, cognitive ability, and adaptive function, with several studies suggesting that these relations likely become stronger as children reach preschool age and beyond. Altogether, these findings suggest that RRB present in children birth through age 3 are not, by themselves, indicative of later functional outcomes, but may be informative in identifying autism regardless of level of ASD severity.

It is plausible that discrepancies in definitions and measures of RRB used across the literature may contribute to obscuring relations between this behavioral domain and other functional outcomes. Given the heterogeneity of behaviors included under the RRB domain in addition to the number of RRB definitions and measures used by research groups, it may be beneficial for researchers and practitioners to carefully consider measurement approaches used to capture early RRB, particularly as it relates to the many different behaviors that comprise the domain. To date, more research has examined RSM topographies than any other categories of RRB from birth to three, though the literature suggests other topographies may be important targets during this period of development. Finally, continuing to expand on the early RRB literature is important to attempt to address the variability of the results across studies. For instance, more precision around both measurement and aggregation of study samples is needed to interrogate contradictory findings in the future. Additional research is needed to further solidify our understanding of how atypical sensory behaviors, IS, and SIB develop during the first years of life in autistic children and the extent to which these may or may not impact quality of life.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article and Supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

PC: conceptualization, data curation, investigation, methodology, validation, and writing—original draft preparation (lead). AD: conceptualization, data curation, investigation, methodology, validation, and writing—original draft preparation. JW: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, supervision, and writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health under award R01MH116961.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.986876/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^We use identity-first language (e.g., autistic children) as opposed to person-first language (e.g., children with autism) in accordance with the American Psychological Association's (2021) inclusive language guidelines, which recommend usage of the language that an identity group prefers. Autistic individuals who have been able to have indicated a preference for identity-first language (e.g., Brown, 2011; Botha et al., 2021).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

American Psychological Association (2021). Inclusive Language Guidelines. Available online at: https://www.apa.org/about/equity-diversity-inclusion/language-guidelines.pdf (accessed March 15, 2022).

Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age 12. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 213–224. doi: 10.1023/A:1023080005650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baranek, G. T., Boyd, B. A., Poe, M. D., David, F. J., and Watson, L. R. (2007). Hyperresponsive sensory patterns in young children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 112, 233–245. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[233:HSPIYC]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., and Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory Experiences Questionnaire: discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 591–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, A. B., Wetherby, A. M., and Chambers, N. W. (2012). Brief report: Repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorder and developmentally similar peers: a follow up to Watt et al. (2008). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2006–2012. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1434-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Itzchak, E., and Zachor, D. A. (2020). Toddlers to teenagers: long-term follow-up study of outcomes in autism spectrum disorder. Autism 24, 41–50. doi: 10.1177/1362361319840226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, K., Russell, K., and Frost, K. (2018). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorder: a review of associated features and presentation across clinical populations. Cur. Dev. Disord. Rep. 5, 108–115. doi: 10.1007/s40474-018-0139-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. L., Hus, V., Duncan, A., Huerta, M., Gotham, K., Pickles, A., et al. (2013). Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1287–1297. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1671-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., and Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 237–243. doi: 10.1023/A:1005596502855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botha, M., Hanlon, J., and Williams, G. L. (2021). Does language matter? Identity-first versus person-first language use in autism research: a response to Vivanti. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04858-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. (2011). The significant of semantics: person-first language: why it matters. Autistic Hoya. Available online at: https://www.autistichoya.com/2011/08/significance-of-semantics-person-first.html (accessed March 15, 2022).

Bruckner, C. T., and Yoder, P. (2007). Restricted object use in young children with autism: definition and construct validity. Autism 11, 161–171. doi: 10.1177/1362361307075709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge Academic.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40, 719–732. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damiano, C. R., Nahmias, A., Hogan-Brown, A. L., and Stone, W. L. (2013). What do repetitive and stereotyped movements mean for infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1326–1335. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1681-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dow, D., Day, T. N., Kutta, T. J., Nottke, C., and Wetherby, A. M. (2019). Screening for autism spectrum disorder in a naturalistic home setting using the Systematic Observation of Red Flags (SORF) at 18–24 months. Autism Res. 13, 122–133. doi: 10.1002/aur.2226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duerden, E. G., Oatley, H. K., Mak-Fan, K. M., McGath, P. A., Taylor, M. J., Szatmari, P., et al. (2012). Risk factors associated with self-injurious behaviors in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2460–2470. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1497-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elison, J. T., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Botteron, K. N., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., et al. (2014). Repetitive behavior in 12-month-olds later classified with autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolescent Psychiatry 53, 1216–1224. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esbensen, A. J., Seltzer, M. M., Lam, K. S. L., and Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Age-related differences in restricted repetitive behaviors inautism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 57–66. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0599-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, D. W., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, D., King, R. A., et al. (1997). Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev. 68, 58–68. doi: 10.2307/1131925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriels, R. L., Cuccaro, M. L., Hill, D. E., Ivers, B. J., and Goldson, E. (2005). Repetitive behaviors in autism: relationships with associated clinical features. Res. Devel. Disab. 26, 169–181. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Germani, T., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Smith, I., Roberts, W., et al. (2014). Brief report: assessment of early sensory processing in infants at high-risk of autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 3264–3270. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2175-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotham, K., Bishop, S. L., Hus, V., Huerta, M., Lund, S., Buja, A., et al. (2013). Exploring the relationship between anxiety and insistence on sameness in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 6, 33–41. doi: 10.1002/aur.1263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grzadzinski, R., Donovan, K., Truong, K., Nowell, S., Lee, H., Sideris, J., et al. (2020). Sensory reactivity at 1 and 2 years old is associated with ASD severity during the preschool years. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 3895–3904. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04432-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guthrie, W., Swineford, L. B., Nottke, C., and Wetherby, A. M. (2013). Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation: stability of early diagnosis and symptoms in ASD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 582–590. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrop, C., Gulsrud, A., and Kasari, C. (2015). Does gender moderate core deficits in ASD? An investigation into restricted and repetitive behaviors in girls and boys with ASD. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 3644–3655. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2511-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrop, C., McConachie, H., Emsley, R., Leadbitter, K., and Green, J. (2014). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders and typical development: cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1207–1219. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1986-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. A., and Watson, S. L. (2013). The impact of parenting stress: a meta-analysis of studies comparing the experience of parenting stress in parents of children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 629–642. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1604-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiruma, L., Pretzel, R. E., Tapia, A. L., Bodfish, J. W., Bradley, C., Wiggins, L., et al. (2021). A distinct three-factor structure of restricted and repetitive behaviors in an epidemiologically sound sample of preschool-age children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 51, 3456–3468. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04776-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honey, E., McConachie, H., Randle, V., Shearer, H., and Le Couteur, A. S. (2008). One-year change in repetitive beavhiours in young children with communication disorders including autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1439–1450. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0191-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacques, C., Courchesne, V., Meilleur, A. S., Mineau, S., Ferguson, S., Cousineau, D., et al. (2018). What interests young autistic children? An exploratory study of object exploration and repetitive behavior. PLoS ONE 13, e0209251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2, 217–250.

Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., and Lord, C. (2010). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in toddlers and preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders based on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Autism Res. 3, 162–173. doi: 10.1002/aur.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. S. L., and Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behavior scale-revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 855–866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. S. L., Bodfish, J. W., and Piven, J. (2008). Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. J.Child Psychol.Psychiatry 49, 1193–1200. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01944.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawson, L. P., Joshi, R., Barbaro, J., and Dissanayake, C. (2018). Gender differences during toddlerhood in autism spectrum disorder: a prospective community-based longitudinal follow-up study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 2619–2628. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3516-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., and Uljarević, M. (2011). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: a review of research in the last decade. Psychol. Bull. 137, 562–593. doi: 10.1037/a0023341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. H., and Bodfish, J. W. (1998). Repetitive behavior disorders in autism. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disab. Res. Rev. 4, 80–89. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2779(1998)4:2<80::AID-MRDD4>3.0.CO;2-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loh, A., Soman, T., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., et al. (2007). Stereotyped motor behaviors associated with autism in high-risk infants: a pilot videotape analysis of a sibling sample. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 225–236. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0333-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., and Luyster, R. (2006). Early diagnosis of children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin. Neurosci. Res. 6, 189–1194. doi: 10.1016/j.cnr.2006.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., and Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 694–701. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, R., Green, G., Mansfield, R., Geckeler, A., Gardenier, N., Anderson, J., et al. (2007). Stereotypy in young children with autism and typically developing children. Res. Dev. Disab. 28, 266–277. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, W. E. Jr., Ellis, D. N., Galbreath, H. N., Halpern, L. F., and Baumeister, A. A. (1991). Rhythmic motor behavior of preambulatory motor impaired, Down Syndrome, and nondisabled children: a comparative analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 19, 319–330. doi: 10.1007/BF00911234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manor-Binyamini, I., and Schreiber-Divon, M. (2019). Repetitive behaviors: listening to the voice of people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spec. Disord. 64, 23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., and Fodstad, J. C. (2009). Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Dev. Neurorehabil. 12, 122–127. doi: 10.1080/17518420902936730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Sun, S., Iosif, A., Young, G. S., Belding, A., Tubbs, A., et al. (2021). Repetitive behavior with objects in infants developing autism predicts diagnosis and later social behavior as early as 9 months. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 130, 665–675. doi: 10.1037/abn0000692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirenda, P., Smith, I. M., Vaillancourt, T., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., Szatmari, P., et al. (2010). Validating the repetitive behavior scale-revised in young children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1521–1530. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1012-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mooney, E. L., Gray, K. M., and Tonge, B. J. (2006). Early features of autism. Eur. Child Adol. Psychiatry 15, 12–18. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0499-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mooney, E. L., Gray, K. M., Tonge, B. J., Sweeney, D. J., and Taffe, J. R. (2009). Factor analytic study of repetitive behaviours in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 765–774. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0680-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, V., and Goodson, S. (2003). How well does early diagnosis of autism stand the test of time? Follow-up study of children assessed for autism at age 2 and development of an early diagnostic service. Autism 7, 47–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007001005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, L., Wetherby, A. M., and Barber, A. (2008). Repetitive and stereotyped movements in children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 826–837. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01904.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadig, A., Lee, I., Singh, L., Bosshart, K., and Ozonoff, S. (2010). How does the topic of conversation affect verbal exchange and eye gaze? A comparison between typical development and high-functioning autism. Neuropsychologia 48, 2730–2739. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson Jobs, E., Bölte, S., and Falck-Ytter, T. (2019). Preschool staff spot social communication difficulties, but not restricted and repetitive beahviors in young autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 1928–1936. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-03867-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z., and Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan - a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 5, 210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A.-M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adol. Psychiatry 49, 256–266.e1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., and Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism 12, 457–472. doi: 10.1177/1362361308096402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purpura, G., Constanzo, V., Chericoni, N., Puopolo, M., Scattoni, M. L., Muratori, F., et al. (2017). Bilateral patterns of repetitive movements in 6- to 12-month old infants with autism spectrum disorders. Front. Psychol. 8, 1168. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsey, R. K., Nichols, L., Ludwig, N. N., Fein, D., Adamson, L. B., and Robins, D. L. (2018). Brief report: sex differences in parental concerns for toddlers with autism risk. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 4063–4069. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3583-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raulston, T. J., and Machalicek, W. (2018). Early intervention for repetitive behavior in autism spectrum disorder: a conceptual model. J. Dev. Phys. Disab. 30, 89–109. doi: 10.1007/s10882-017-9566-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ray-Subramanian, C. E., and Ellis Weismer, S. (2012). Receptive and expressive language as predictors of restricted and repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2113–2120. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1463-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richler, J., Bishop, S. L., Kleinke, J. R., and Lord, C. (2007). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 73–85. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0332-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richler, J., Huerta, M., Bishop, S. L., and Lord, C. (2010). Developmental trajectories of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Psychopath. 22, 55–69. doi: 10.1017/s0954579409990265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., and Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33, 631–642. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schertz, H. H., Odom, S. L., Baggett, K. M., and Sideris, J. H. (2016). Parent-reported repetitive behavior in toddlers on the autism spectrum. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 3308–3316. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2870-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sifre, R., Berry, D., Wolff, J. J., and Elison, J. T. (2021). Longitudinal change in restricted and repetitive behaviors from 8–36 months. J. Neurodev. Disord. 13, 7. doi: 10.1186/s11689-020-09335-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

South, M., Ozonoff, S., and McMahon, W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 145–158. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephenson, K. G., Norris, M., and Butter, E. M. (2021). Sex-based differences in autism symptoms in a large, clinically-referred sample of preschool-aged children with ASD. J. Autism Dev. Disord. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04836-2. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stronach, S., and Wetherby, A. M. (2014). Examining restricted and repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorder during two observational contexts. Autism 18, 127–136. doi: 10.1177/1362361312463616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Anim. Behav. 27(Pt 3), 699–715. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90006-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyb, E., Knoch, K., Herlihy, L., Stevens, M. C., Chen, C., Barton, M., et al. (2016). Restricted and repetitive behaviors as predictors of outcome in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 1282–1296. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2668-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M. (1999). Repetitive behaviour in autism: a review of psychological research. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40, 839–849. doi: 10.1111/14697610.00502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uljarević, M., Arnott, B., Carrington, S. J., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., McConachie, H., et al. (2017). Development of restricted and repetitive behaviors from 15 to 77 months: stability of two distinct subtypes? Dev. Psychol., 53, 1859–1868. doi: 10.1037/dev0000324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uljarević, M., Frazier, T. W., Jo, B., Billingham, W. D., Cooper, M. N., Youngstrom, E. A., et al. (2022). Big data approach to a characterize restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism. J Am. Acad. Child Adol. Pscyhiatry 61, 446–457. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uljarević, M., Hedley, D., Nevill, R., Evans, D. W., Ying Cai, R., Butter, E., et al. (2018). Brief report: poor self-regulation as a predictor of individual differences in adapative functioning in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 11, 1157–1165. doi: 10.1002/aur.1953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Akker, O., Peters, G. Y., Bakker, C., Carlsson, R., Coles, N. A., Corker, K. S., et al. (2020). Inclusive systematic review registration form. MetaArXiv. doi: 10.31222/osf.io/3nbea

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watt, N., Wetherby, A. M., Barber, A., and Morgan, L. (2008). Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1518–1533. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0532-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherby, A. M., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., and Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 473–493. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-2544-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., et al. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 945–953. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, J. J., Dimian, A. F., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., et al. (2019). A longitudinal study of parent-reported sensory responsiveness in toddlers at-risk for autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 314–324. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zohar, A. H., and Felz, L. (2001). Ritualistic behavior in young children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 121–128. doi: 10.1023/A:1005231912747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaigenbaum, L. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Inter. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23, 143–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaigenbaum, L., Duku, E., Fombonne, E., Szatmari, P., Smith, I. M., Bryson, S. E., et al. (2018). Developmental functioning and symptom severity influence age of diagnosis in Canadian preschool children with autism. Paediatr. Child Health 24, e57–e65. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxy076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: repetitive behavior, ritualistic, autism spectrum disorder, birth to three, sensory

Citation: Chaxiong P, Dimian AF and Wolff JJ (2022) Restricted and repetitive behavior in children with autism during the first three years of life: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 13:986876. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.986876

Received: 05 July 2022; Accepted: 06 October 2022;
Published: 31 October 2022.

Edited by:

Lisa A. Osborne, The Open University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Roberta Fadda, University of Cagliari, Italy
Sarah Rotschafer, Mercer University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Chaxiong, Dimian and Wolff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jason J. Wolff, amp3b2xmZiYjeDAwMDQwO3Vtbi5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.