- 1CRP-CPO, UR UPJV 72723, Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens, France
- 2INSPE de l’Académie d’Amiens, Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens, France
- 3School of Psychology, Charles Sturt University, Bathurst, NSW, Australia
Prospective Memory (PM), the ability to remember to realize intended actions in the future, is crucial for maintaining autonomy. Decades of research has focused on a so-called age PM paradox, where older adults outperformed younger adults on some PM tasks, but not others. Contributing to this paradox is heterogeneity in and a lack of valid assessment methods. Previous research showed a lack of convergent validity between performance-based PM and both self-report and informant-report measures. We argue that questionnaires may be relevant to obtain information regarding patients’ awareness of their PM difficulties but need to be used in conjunction with performance-based tools. Within performance-based PM tools there are also difficulties in measurement: 15–60 min experimental tasks and batteries have a good reliability but cannot usually fit in a standard clinical evaluation, while shorter PM measures have lower reliability and sensitivity. In this perspective paper, we encourage researchers to develop more ecologically valid tools. Innovative PM paradigms that allow participants to generate their own intentions and that take task costs into consideration should be developed. Future research will also need to focus on cognitive factors, personality and online evaluation, to improve PM assessment and develop ad-hoc rehabilitation programs.
Introduction
Prospective Memory (PM) is the ability to remember to realize intended actions in the future (Einstein and McDaniel, 1990). Most research in this field has distinguished between (1) event-based tasks, where actions involve the detection of an external cue (e.g., tell your partner you had a promotion when you see them) and (2) time-based tasks, where actions involve checking the time (e.g., go to the Liverpool stadium at 8:45 p.m. to attend the Champions’ League game). To date literature on PM has brought many tools, theories and models, however there is no consensus on how to assess PM (Ellis, 1996; Burgess and Shallice, 1997; McDaniel and Einstein, 2007). The choice of assessment methods has usually relied on the specific research question (event-based versus time-based) or feasibility and clinical relevance (e.g., patients’ fatigability).
Prospective memory is a crucial factor in maintaining autonomy in daily life (i.e., medical adherence, social, and professional meetings) for many clinical groups (Henry, 2021) and during normal aging (Blondelle et al., 2015, 2016). As such, much research in the last three decades has focused on understanding why younger adults outperform older adults on some PM measures, but not in others.
Conceptual implications
A not so paradoxical paradox
During the past three decades, researchers have investigated the Age PM Paradox, as older participants were found to perform worse than young participants in experimental conditions and perform better in ecological conditions (Henry et al., 2004). However, this so-called paradox might not be due to age itself as previously thought, but because of cognitive processes that are differentially affected by aging (Haines et al., 2019).
The so-called Age PM Paradox could be due to the heterogeneity of PM assessments (Azzopardi et al., 2017; Haines et al., 2020; Schnitzspahn et al., 2020). In these studies, deficits in older adults were only found for event-based tasks in the lab, with no age difference for naturalistic event-based tasks. Similarly, benefits for older adults were found for naturalistic time-based tasks, without any age difference for lab time-based tasks. Another important parameter affecting age differences is the source of PM task generation: participants performed better with self-generated intentions rather than experimenter-generated actions to perform. As a result of these findings, now, the so-called age PM paradox is no longer popular and research has shifted its focus toward cognitive factors more likely to explain individual differences in PM, such as executive functions, retrospective memory or IQ (Hainselin et al., 2011; Azzopardi et al., 2015; Koo et al., 2021). Research has also focused on the ecological validity of PM tasks in assessing these factors involved in PM as well as their relationship with self-report measures (Hainselin et al., 2021; Sugden et al., 2021a).
Assessment of a multidimensional prospective memory construct
Assessment of cognitive resource allocation
According to the Multiprocess Framework (McDaniel and Einstein, 2000), PM performance is determined by the allocation of cognitive resources based on the nature of the task (e.g., time-based versus event-based), ongoing task, retrieval cues (e.g., frequency and focality), socio-environmental context (e.g., use of compensatory aids), and individual characteristics (e.g., personality). To capture the multidimensionality of PM, various performance-based laboratory, naturalistic, and virtual reality tasks have been used across studies, which have unsurprisingly produced inconsistent findings across studies. In a meta-analysis, Anderson et al. (2019) synthesized inconsistent findings regarding the effects of performing a PM task in conjunction with an ongoing activity, as opposed to performing the ongoing task alone. They found that increasing the number of non-focal cues in the PM task did not affect PM accuracy but affected speed of responding in the ongoing task (i.e., ongoing task costs), whereas, increasing focal cues affected PM accuracy but did not exert significant costs on ongoing task response times. These findings do not fully support the assumptions of the multiprocess framework, which suggests that increasing the number of focal cues that rely on spontaneous retrieval processes should not affect PM accuracy. Extensions of monitoring theories that have proposed that attentional resources can be flexibly allocated during a PM task in response to ongoing task demands (e.g., the Dynamic Multiprocess Framework and the Preparatory Attentional and Memory model in context) (Scullin et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2017) also do not explain discrepancies resulting from different numbers of PM cues in focal tasks. Given these gaps in existing frameworks, a framework that can consolidate the influence of PM task characteristics on PM performance is needed.
What do prospective memory self-reports measure?
Self-report measures of PM differ in the aspects of PM that are measured [see Sugden et al. (2021a) for a review]. The Prospective Memory Questionnaire (PMQ) (Hannon et al., 1995) measures PM lapses and techniques to remember; the Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire focuses on PM versus RM failures (PRMQ) (Smith et al., 2000); the Comprehensive Assessment of Prospective Memory (CAPM) (Shum and Fleming, 2014) and the Brief Assessment of Prospective Memory (BAPM) (Man et al., 2011) assess forgetting of activities of daily living, with the CAPM also measuring task importance and reasons for forgetting; and the Prospective Memory Concerns Questionnaire (PMCQ) (Sugden et al., 2021b) investigates forgetting behaviors, memory concerns, and retrieval failures. Illustrating these differences in self-report subscales, Cuttler and Taylor (2012) found cannabis users reported more PM failures than non-users on the PMQ internally-cued subscale, but there were no differences on other PMQ subscales or the PRMQ.
In addition to inconsistencies within performance-based and self-report methodologies, an enduring finding is a lack of convergent validity between self-report and performance-based PM measures. In their scoping review of self-report PM questionnaires, Sugden et al. (2021a) found that the relationships between self-report and performance-based PM measures were mostly non-significant and weak to moderate in size. While this may suggest that self-report measures are not valid indicators of PM ability (Uttl and Kibreab, 2011), we contend that poor convergent validity across measurement methods may equally reflect issues with the reliability and validity of performance-based criterion measures.
According to frameworks proposed by Phillips et al. (2008) and built on by Jones et al. (2021), the most ecologically valid tasks are those observing self-generated intentions within the individuals’ everyday environments, such as medication adherence observations (e.g., Woods et al., 2009). Naturalistic experimental tasks such as the envelope task (Huppert et al., 2000) or PM test batteries are deemed slightly less ecologically valid. While many of these tasks permit compensatory aids, and are carried out in neuropsychological or home settings, they are typically single-trial, experimenter-generated, short-interval tasks. These tasks are therefore prone to ceiling effects, restricted scoring ranges, and poor reliability [see Blondelle et al. (2020) for a review of PM measures]. Virtual reality tasks such as the Virtual Reality Everyday Assessment Lab (Kourtesis et al., 2020) involve immersive environments such as shopping precincts. Yet, ecological validity is still inhibited due to the use of experimenter-generated intentions and limiting of strategic aids normally used in everyday PM tasks (Haas et al., 2022). Laboratory-based tasks designed to isolate and control factors underlying PM performance are the least ecologically valid of performance-based measures, due to their use of computerized, experimenter-generated intentions in artificial settings (Phillips et al., 2008). Using this framework of task ecological validity, Jones et al. (2021) found PM intervention studies typically had low to moderate ecological validity, which culminated in few improvements observed in long-term everyday PM in the studies reviewed. Their framework placed self-report measures as the least ecologically valid, due to their low convergent validity with performance-based measures. However, it was acknowledged that self-report measures do still assess performance on ecologically valid tasks and may detect transfer of training to everyday PM tasks. Supporting this, Sugden et al. (2021a) found self-reported PM improvements following open skills interventions (i.e., broader psychoeducation on health behaviors and memory strategies) and performance-based improvements in closed skill interventions (i.e., specific cognitive tasks). As such, self-report measures of PM differ from performance-based measures in that they capture additional psychological and environmental aspects of the multidimensional PM construct.
There is evidence to suggest that variance in self-report PM measures can be explained by factors such as personality (Buchanan, 2017), mood (Steinberg et al., 2013), use of compensatory strategies (Uttl and Kibreab, 2011), and social motivation (Penningroth et al., 2011). Measuring these factors in self-report PM questionnaires provides important insights into individuals’ beliefs, concerns, and goals which can then be used to identify reasons for forgetting and inform treatment strategies (Roche et al., 2002). However, these factors may also confound and provide inaccurate representations of an individual’s PM performance.
Self-reports of PM may be problematic when an individual lacks insight into their PM ability. Individuals may overestimate or underestimate their ability (Cauvin et al., 2019), forget instances of forgetting, or experience neurological impairments that affect metacognitive awareness (Roche et al., 2002). In these situations, informant reports of PM where a proxy observes an individual’s PM performance, may better reflect performance-based PM. However, informant-reports, just like self-reports, may be influenced by factors such as the patient’s age, or the informant’s own levels of subjective burden and distress (Smith et al., 2000; Morrell et al., 2019). Indeed, Sugden et al. (2021a) found in their scoping review that informant-reports had similar non-significant, weak to moderate sized relationships with performance-based measures to self-report measures. Moreover, relationships between self-report and informant-report PM were inconsistent. They concluded that self-report measures of PM may be more accurate in non-clinical populations, but cognitive impairments become more severe, informant reports that rely on observations may provide a more valid report of PM performance. Nevertheless, we recommend further research into variables that influence self-reports and informant-reports of PM.
Recommendations to improve and expand prospective memory assessment instruments
To date, there is a whole arsenal of tools devoted to the assessment of PM abilities such as paper and pencil test batteries, experimental procedures, single-trial procedures and questionnaires [for review, see Blondelle et al. (2020); see also Table 1 for an overview of the existing PM measures]. This diversity of PM measures and their characteristics shows that no single tool can meet all clinical requirements. PM test batteries and experimental procedures generally have a good reliability as they offer a wide range of trials (between 4 and 8 for PM test batteries and between 18 and 50 for experimental procedures). Such measures are also conceptually relevant as they allow an examination of the main dimensions of PM (event- and time-based). However, they can be difficult to incorporate into a standard neuropsychological assessment because they require significant administration time ranging from 15 to 60 min. In this context, clinicians may prefer the use of single-trials such as the Key task (Babicz et al., 2019) and the Envelope Task (Huppert et al., 2000) due to their low cost and brevity. However, it should be noted that these brief PM measurements have lower reliability and sensitivity compared to test batteries and experimental procedures which may limit their utility, particularly in clinical settings. Because of their structure, the use of such measures does not accurately characterize the nature of PM difficulties encountered by patients. Therefore, the use of single-trial PM measures may be primarily useful for global screening purposes and to demonstrate to patients and their families how difficulties in retrieving intentions may impair autonomy in everyday-life situations. Finally, while questionnaires do not appear to be valid measures of PM performance as measured by laboratory and naturalistic PM tasks, their use may be relevant for obtaining information regarding patients’ awareness of their PM difficulties and for targeting interventions toward areas of greatest concern for the individual.
In a recent review on performance-based PM assessment instruments, Blondelle et al. (2020) pointed out that most of the PM measures suffer from a lack of adequate normative data, translation into another language, and cross-cultural adaptations. This result is not very surprising, and is consistent with clinicians’ concerns about neuropsychological assessment (Rabin et al., 2005, 2016). For example, the lack of adequate normative data is one of the most challenging issues in the field of neuropsychological assessment for several reasons including substantial heterogeneity on important variables (e.g., sample size, age, gender, education, language, culture, location) and the inconsistent use of non-clinical versus specific clinical populations in standardization studies (Strauss et al., 2006; Puente et al., 2013). Future validation and standardization PM studies should take into account these essential aspects by referring to the guidelines proposed by the International Test Commission (Hernández et al., 2020) to allow a better interpretation of scores and their comparability across cultures.
Methodological implications
Developing paradigms that more broadly capture the prospective memory processes involved in everyday life
Over the past three decades, researchers have developed many innovative paradigms and analytical techniques to study PM. These paradigms, techniques, and tasks have been very helpful the predictions of these theoretical approaches (McDaniel and Einstein, 2000; Smith, 2003; Scullin et al., 2013; Strickland et al., 2019), isolating the strategic and spontaneous retrieval processes that contribute to PM remembering [for a review, see McDaniel et al. (2015)] and identifying the vulnerability of certain processes to forgetting. Notwithstanding these advances, we believe that it is essential to continue to develop PM paradigms that are able to capture the processes involved in real-life situations.
In laboratory studies, PM is typically studied using variants of the paradigm initially developed by Einstein and McDaniel (1990) in which participant are required to perform an ongoing task (e.g., lexical decision task) while remembering to generate an alternative response at some time point or upon the occurrence of a specific target event (e.g., press a designated key when the syllable “tor” appears on the screen during a lexical decision task). However, the definition of PM appears to be broad enough to overcome some limitations inherent to the design of most PM tasks. As an example, most everyday event-based PM tasks could have any delay, be performed more or less regularly, triggered by a wide variety of target events, and be related to other individuals. In practice, these aspects are mostly examined in isolation when designing PM tasks, mainly for experimental control and measurement reasons (e.g., researchers often decide to present the same target event several times to improve the reliability of PM tasks), which tends to obscure PM demands in daily life.
While this approach may be of interest for confronting different theoretical positions, it may not be entirely appropriate for understanding the role of PM processes in everyday life and how we manage to remember to take medication and to fulfill our social and professional obligations. However, it is precisely these field questions that have driven research on PM during the last three decades. In natural settings, one of the most key features of PM tasks is that individuals tend to formulate their own intentions in contrast to laboratory tasks where intentions are assigned always by the experimenter. Future studies could develop innovative laboratory PM paradigms that allow participants to generate their own intentions or to choose intentions that are close to those they are asked to perform in a laboratory setting. Some existing flexible PM paradigms, such as the Virtual Week (Rendell and Craik, 2000), may be appropriate candidates to address this challenge by allowing participants to choose activities that are most appropriate for their lifestyles.
Interpreting ongoing task costs and limitations
One of the most common findings in experimental studies on PM is that the introduction of a PM task produces a detrimental effect on the ongoing activity in which the PM task is embedded [referred to as ongoing task costs; Smith (2003)]. In a PM condition, the presence of ongoing task costs is traditionally interpreted as reflecting a change in resource allocation policies away from the ongoing task toward the PM task. As an example, some theoretical positions such as the Preparatory Attentional and Memory Processes theory (Smith, 2003) argue that successful PM performance entails resource-demanding processes which take the form of strategic monitoring to search for relevant environmental cues in order to carry out the intended action. Since monitoring involves attentional capacities that are inherently limited, these are diverted away from the ongoing task, thereby resulting in slower and less accurate responses [for meta-analysis, see Anderson et al. (2019)].
While there has long been a consensus among researchers that costs are true markers of sharing resources between the ongoing task and the PM tasks, more recent research has provided an alternative explanation to this dominant interpretation. For example, the Delay Theory (Loft and Remington, 2013; Heathcote et al., 2015) argues that costs may be due to a tendency for individuals to slow down their responding to give themselves time to accumulate sufficient PM-related information while avoiding being absorbed by ongoing task processing. Scientific advances in mathematical modeling have made it possible, using the diffusion and linear ballistic accumulator model (Brown and Heathcote, 2008), to generate parameters that reflect the processes underlying costs. Such models are useful because they take into account both accuracy and response times, two measures that are typically considered separately when considering ongoing task costs. They also have the advantage of fitting the empirical data well (Heathcote et al., 2015) and account for the anticipatory mechanisms deployed by individuals to deal with the ongoing task demand following the addition of a PM task. By adjusting their decision thresholds, individuals will have more time to discriminate between ongoing task trials and those specific to the PM task.
While the Delay Theory provides insights into the origin of costs by showing that threshold shifts as derived from diffusion and linear ballistic accumulator explain much of variance in ongoing task response times, it does not propose a specific mechanism responsible for PM performance (Strickland et al., 2019). The Prospective Memory Decision Control framework (Strickland et al., 2018) has been proposed to allow for a more comprehensive analysis of task performance including reaction times and accuracy for both the ongoing task and the PM task. The model also allows researchers to study the two classes of cognitive control proposed by Braver (2012) in the context of PM research: (1) the proactive control which is active in advance of a future cognitively demanding event (i.e., the target event) in order to prepare the response and (2) the reactive control that occurs when the target event is encountered in order to produce the intended response. This new model provides a fruitful research avenue for understanding when changes to accumulation rates impact performance of ongoing activities and the intended actions in various contexts.
Conclusion
Summary
We now have many PM assessment tools, maybe too many. Performance-based PM tools are heterogeneous regarding duration (15–60 min experimental tasks), psychometric validation and feasibility: batteries have a good reliability but cannot usually fit in a standard clinical evaluation, while shorter PM measures have lower reliability and sensitivity. Questionnaires may be relevant to obtain information regarding patients’ awareness of their PM difficulties and need to be used with performance-based tools.
Future studies and perspectives
Future research will have to select PM assessment instruments with the best psychometric qualities and cultural adaptation, as recently highlighted in a systematic review (Blondelle et al., 2020). The effects of culture on psychological assessment and the need for culturally appropriate tests are widely discussed in the literature beyond PM. Therefore, the PM research community should work to avoid being another WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) research topic (Henrich et al., 2010). In the near future, we will need to use more comprehensive assessment methods, combining experimental and ecological tasks with self- and informant-reports to improve PM understanding and clinical practice. Because of the growing availability of videoconference tools, we will also have to evaluate the impact of PM online evaluation, which seems to be as valid as in person evaluation, but still needs improvement to ensure that everyone, especially minority groups, women and the elderly, can access these services (Zuber et al., 2022).
The next big PM issue to deal with will be how to facilitate improvements of PM skills in the elderly and clinical groups. A recent systematic review highlighted the benefits of mental imagery and external aids to improve PM during aging (Tsang et al., 2021). Moreover, research has found that some smartphone applications, such as voice recorders and reminders, seem to be helpful for patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) (Scullin et al., 2022). Future research will need to focus on the effectiveness of these interventions, as well as the methods used to assess intervention outcomes. Similarly to PM assessments, PM improvement strategies and rehabilitation should focus on factors influencing it. Recent research pointed to the role of memory load, likelihood of distraction, working memory, metacognitive confidence, and offloading frequency on PM strategies efficacy (Gilbert, 2015; Ball et al., 2022).
In the same vein, an event-related potential study conducted in the field of cognitive training using validated PM tasks such as the Virtual Week reported an enhancement of PM performances on the task with a use of better self-strategies to support PM remembering in everyday life at the end of the training session. Importantly, this performance improvement was accompanied by an increased performance to real-world PM tasks and activities of daily living (Rose et al., 2015). The event-related potential (ERP) results revealed that the benefit of training was associated with a substantial reduction of an event-related potential component associated with the monitoring of PM cues, suggesting a more spontaneous PM retrieval. These results suggest that laboratory-based PM tasks may be good candidates for supporting interventions to improve PM abilities in daily life.
Together, this research will have implications for how we conduct research into PM, for clinical practice, and will have an impact far beyond, with applications for assessing PM and developing interventions for medical settings (e.g., medication adherence), aviation and education.
Author contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Funding
GB received funding from the Université de Picardie Jules Verne’s Soutien au Rayonnement de la Recherche (S2R) and the Maison Européenne des Sciences de l’Homme et de la Société (MESHS) for the publication fees.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Nabila Terchani for handling all the administrative issues.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Anderson, F. T., Strube, M. J., and McDaniel, M. A. (2019). Toward a better understanding of costs in prospective memory: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 145, 1053–1081. doi: 10.1037/bul0000208
Azzopardi, B., Auffray, C., and Juhel, J. (2015). L’effet paradoxal du vieillissement sur la mémoire prospective?: hypothèses explicatives. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 13, 64–72. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2014.0513
Azzopardi, B., Auffray, C., and Kermarrec, C. (2017). Effet paradoxal du vieillissement sur des tâches de mémoire prospective time-based en situation de laboratoire et en milieu naturel?: rôle des fonctions exécutives. Can. J. Aging 36, 30–40. doi: 10.1017/S0714980816000738
Babicz, M. A., Sullivan, K. L., Weinborn, M., Bucks, R. S., Ng, A., Hodgson, E., et al. (2019). Does the key task measure prospective memory? Cautionary findings from parallel studies in HIV disease and older adults. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 1438–1444. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acz009
Ball, H., Peper, P., Alakbarova, D., Brewer, G., and Gilbert, S. J. (2022). Individual differences in working memory capacity predict benefits to memory from intention offloading. Memory 30, 77–91. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1991380
Blondelle, G., Hainselin, M., Gounden, Y., and Quaglino, V. (2020). Instruments measuring prospective memory: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 35, 576–596. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa009
Blondelle, G., Hainselin, M., Gounden, Y., Heurley, L., Voisin, H., Bressous, E., et al. (2015). Effet de la valence émotionnelle sur la mémoire prospective au cours du vieillissement [effect of emotional valence in prospective memory during aging]. Rev. Neuropsychol. 7, 189–198. doi: 10.1684/nrp.2015.0350
Blondelle, G., Hainselin, M., Gounden, Y., Heurley, L., Voisin, H., Megalakaki, O., et al. (2016). Regularity effect in prospective memory during aging. Socioaffect. Neurosci. Psychol. 6:31238. doi: 10.3402/snp.v6.31238
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
Brown, S. D., and Heathcote, A. (2008). The simplest complete model of choice response time: linear ballistic accumulation. Cogn. Psychol. 57, 153–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002
Buchanan, T. (2017). Self-assessments of memory correlate with neuroticism and conscientiousness, not memory span performance. Pers. Individ. Diff. 105, 19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.031
Burgess, P. W., and Shallice, T. (1997). “The relationship between prospective and retrospective memory: neuropsychological evidence,” in Cognitive Models Of Memory Studies In Cognition, ed. M. A. Conway (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 247–272.
Cauvin, S., Moulin, C., Souchay, C., Schnitzspahn, K., and Kliegel, M. (2019). Laboratory vs. naturalistic prospective memory task predictions: young adults are overconfident outside of the laboratory. Memory 27, 592–602. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1540703
Cuttler, C., and Taylor, S. (2012). Did i forget to lock the door? The link between prospective memory failures and doubt in the compulsion to check. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 3, 437–454.
Einstein, G. O., and McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 16, 717–726. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.16.4.717
Ellis, J. (1996). “Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: a conceptual framework for research,” in Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications, eds M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, and M. A. Mcdaniel (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 1–22.
Gilbert, S. J. (2015). Strategic offloading of delayed intentions into the external environment. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 68, 971–992. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.972963
Haas, M., Mehl, M. R., Ballhausen, N., Zuber, S., Kliegel, M., and Hering, A. (2022). The sounds of memory: extending the age–prospective memory paradox to everyday behavior and conversations. J. Gerontol. 77, 695–703. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbac012
Haines, S. J., Randall, S. E., Terrett, G., Busija, L., Tatangelo, G., McLennan, S. N., et al. (2020). Differences in time-based task characteristics help to explain the age-prospective memory paradox. Cognition 202:104305. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104305
Haines, S. J., Shelton, J. T., Henry, J. D., Terrett, G., Vorwerk, T., and Rendell, P. G. (2019). “Prospective memory and cognitive aging,” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology, (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Available online at: https://oxfordre.com/psychology/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.001.0001/acrefore-9780190236557-e-381
Hainselin, M., Gounden, Y., and Blondelle, G. (2021). Assessing prospective memory beyond experimental tasks. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 17, 459–459. doi: 10.1038/s41582-021-00499-4
Hainselin, M., Quinette, P., Desgranges, B., Martinaud, O., Hannequin, D., de La Sayette, V., et al. (2011). Can we remember future actions yet forget the last two minutes? Study in transient global amnesia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 4138–4149. doi: 10.1162/ajocn_a_00076
Hannon, R., Adams, P., Harrington, S., Fries-Dias, C., and Gipson, M. T. (1995). Effects of brain injury and age on prospective memory self-rating and performance. Rehabil. Psychol. 40, 289–298. doi: 10.1037/0090-5550.40.4.289
Heathcote, A., Loft, S., and Remington, R. W. (2015). Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. Psychol. Rev. 122, 376–410. doi: 10.1037/a0038952
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466, 29–29. doi: 10.1038/466029a
Henry, J. D. (2021). Prospective memory impairment in neurological disorders: implications and management. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 17, 297–307. doi: 10.1038/s41582-021-00472-1
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., and Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychol. Aging 19, 27–39. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27
Hernández, A., Hidalgo, M. D., Hambleton, R. K., and Gómez-Benito, J. (2020). International test commission guidelines for test adaptation: a criterion checklist. Psicothema 32, 390–398. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.306
Huppert, F. A., Johnson, T., and Nickson, J. (2000). High prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly and in early-stage dementia: findings from a population-based study. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, S63–S81. doi: 10.1002/acp.771
Jones, W. E., Benge, J. F., and Scullin, M. K. (2021). Preserving prospective memory in daily life: a systematic review and meta-analysis of mnemonic strategy, cognitive training, external memory aid, and combination interventions. Neuropsychology 35, 123–140. doi: 10.1037/neu0000704
Koo, Y. W., Neumann, D. L., Ownsworth, T., and Shum, D. H. K. (2021). Revisiting the age-prospective memory paradox using laboratory and ecological tasks. Front. Psychol. 12:691752. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.691752
Kourtesis, P., Korre, D., Collina, S., Doumas, L. A. A., and MacPherson, S. E. (2020). Guidelines for the development of immersive virtual reality software for cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology: the development of virtual reality everyday assessment lab (vr-eal), a neuropsychological test battery in immersive virtual reality. Front. Comput. Sci. 1:12. doi: 10.3389/fcomp.2019.00012
Loft, S., and Remington, R. W. (2013). Wait a second: brief delays in responding reduce focality effects in event-based prospective memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 1432–1447. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.750677
Man, D. W. K., Fleming, J., Hohaus, L., and Shum, D. (2011). Development of the brief assessment of prospective memory (BAPM) for use with traumatic brain injury populations. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 884–898. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.627270
McDaniel, M. A., and Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, S127–S144. doi: 10.1002/acp.775
McDaniel, M. A., and Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective Memory: An Overview and Synthesis of an Emerging Field. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
McDaniel, M. A., Umanath, S., Einstein, G. O., and Waldum, E. R. (2015). Dual pathways to prospective remembering. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:392. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00392
Morrell, L., Camic, P. M., and Genis, M. (2019). Factors associated with informant-reported cognitive decline in older adults: a systemised literature review. Dementia 18, 2760–2784. doi: 10.1177/1471301218759836
Penningroth, S. L., Scott, W. D., and Freuen, M. (2011). Social motivation in prospective memory: higher importance ratings and reported performance rates for social tasks. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 3–11. doi: 10.1037/a0022841
Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., and Martin, B. A. (2008). “Adult aging and prospective memory: the importance of ecological validity,” in Prospective Memory: Cognitive, Neuroscience, Developmental, And Applied Perspectives, eds G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, and M. Kliegel (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).
Puente, A. E., Perez-Garcia, M., Vilar Lopez, R., Hidalgo-Ruzzante, N. A., and Fasfous, A. F. (2013). “Neuropsychological assessment of culturally and educationally dissimilar individuals,” in Handbook of Multicultural Mental Health, eds A.-M. Yamada and F. A. Paniagua (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 225–241. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394420-7.00012-6
Rabin, L. A., Paolillo, E., and Barr, W. B. (2016). Stability in test-usage practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the united states and canada over a 10-year period: a follow-up survey of ins and nan members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 31, 206–230. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw007
Rabin, L., Barr, W., and Burton, L. (2005). Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the united states and canada: a survey of INS, NAN, and APA division 40 members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 33–65. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005
Rendell, P. G., and Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Virtual week and actual week: age-related differences in prospective memory. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, S43–S62. doi: 10.1002/acp.770
Roche, N. L., Fleming, J. M., and Shum, D. H. K. (2002). Self-awareness of prospective memory failure in adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury 16, 931–945. doi: 10.1080/02699050210138581
Rose, N. S., Rendell, P. G., Hering, A., Kliegel, M., Bidelman, G. M., and Craik, F. I. M. (2015). Cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults’ prospective memory following training with the Virtual Week computer game. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00592
Schnitzspahn, K. M., Kvavilashvili, L., and Altgassen, M. (2020). Redefining the pattern of age-prospective memory-paradox: new insights on age effects in lab-based, naturalistic, and self-assigned tasks. Psychol. Res. 84, 1370–1386. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1140-2
Scullin, M. K., Jones, W. E., Phenis, R., Beevers, S., Rosen, S., Dinh, K., et al. (2022). Using smartphone technology to improve prospective memory functioning: a randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 70, 459–469. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17551
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., and Shelton, J. T. (2013). The dynamic multiprocess framework: evidence from prospective memory with contextual variability. Cogn. Psychol. 67, 55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.07.001
Shum, D., and Fleming, J. M. (2014). Comprehensive Assessment Of Prospective Memory [Clinical Assessment And User Manual]. Gold Coast, QLD: Griffith Health Institute.
Smith, G., Del Sala, S., Logie, R. H., and Maylor, E. A. (2000). Prospective and retrospective memory in normal ageing and dementia: a questionnaire study. Memory 8, 311–321. doi: 10.1080/09658210050117735
Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. J. Exp. Psychol. 29, 347–361. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347
Smith, R. E., Hunt, R. R., and Murray, A. E. (2017). Prospective memory in context: moving through a familiar space. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 189–204. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000303
Steinberg, S. I., Negash, S., Sammel, M. D., Bogner, H., Harel, B. T., Livney, M. G., et al. (2013). Subjective memory complaints, cognitive performance, and psychological factors in healthy older adults. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 28, 776–783. doi: 10.1177/1533317513504817
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd Edn. Oxford: OUP.
Strickland, L., Elliott, D., Wilson, M. D., Loft, S., Neal, A., and Heathcote, A. (2019). Prospective memory in the red zone: cognitive control and capacity sharing in a complex, multi-stimulus task. J. Exp. Psychol. 25, 695–715. doi: 10.1037/xap0000224
Strickland, L., Loft, S., Remington, R. W., and Heathcote, A. (2018). Racing to remember: a theory of decision control in event-based prospective memory. Psychol. Rev. 125, 851–887. doi: 10.1037/rev0000113
Sugden, N., Thomas, M., and Kiernan, M. (2021a). A scoping review of the utility of self-report and informant-report prospective memory measures. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 1–31. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2021.1875851
Sugden, N., Thomas, M., Kiernan, M., and Wilesmith, M. (2021b). Validation of the prospective memory concerns questionnaire (PMCQ). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:421. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.686850
Tsang, A. P. L., Au, A., and Lo, H. H. M. (2021). Prospective memory training for healthy older adults: a systematic review. Clin. Gerontol. 45, 486–502. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2021.1950253
Uttl, B., and Kibreab, M. (2011). Self-report measures of prospective memory are reliable but not valid. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 57–68. doi: 10.1037/a0022843
Woods, S. P., Dawson, M. S., Wever, E., Gibson, S., Grant, I., and Atkinson, J. H. (2009). Timing is everything: antiretroviral nonadherence is associated with impairment in time-based prospective memory. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15:42. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708090012
Keywords: future, action, experimental task, self-report, informant-report, prospective memory, assessment, memory for intentions
Citation: Blondelle G, Sugden N and Hainselin M (2022) Prospective memory assessment: Scientific advances and future directions. Front. Psychol. 13:958458. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.958458
Received: 31 May 2022; Accepted: 11 July 2022;
Published: 28 July 2022.
Edited by:
Lawrence M. Parsons, The University of Sheffield, United KingdomReviewed by:
Seth Ryan Koslov, University of Pennsylvania, United StatesLars Evald, Aarhus University, Denmark
Copyright © 2022 Blondelle, Sugden and Hainselin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Mathieu Hainselin, mathieu.hainselin@u-picardie.fr