Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2022
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Credition - An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Nature of Beliefs and Believing View all 42 articles

Inter-brain plasticity underlies empathic learning in social interactions

  • School of Psychological Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

Introduction

Empathy, our capacity to react to the suffering of others, is not a monolithic process and involves emotional (e.g., shared pain) and cognitive (e.g., perspective taking) components (Gonzalez-Liencres et al., 2013). While previous studies have focused on investigating the neural underpinnings of cognitive and emotional empathy in the target, it is increasingly acknowledged that integrative brain models for understanding the dynamic interaction between the target and the observer are warranted.

While the research on empathy emphasizes first and foremost its contribution to distress regulation in the target, few studies have examined how empathic responses of the observer actually change the state of the target. Indeed, although empathy occurs in social interactions, research on empathy have largely focused on covert mechanisms of empathy in the observer (empathizer), without exploring how empathic reactions affect the distress of the target (Main et al., 2017; Shamay-Tsoory and Mendelsohn, 2019).

To examine the role of empathy in regulating the target's distress, Reeck et al. (2016) have proposed a model of interpersonal emotion regulation that takes into account both the target and the observer. This model holds that empathy plays a major role in interpersonal emotion regulation, as the distress of the target may trigger an empathetic reaction in the observer. This model describes the participation of several empathy-related brain regions in the interpersonal emotion regulation cycle, including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). The dmPFC and TPJ are parts of the brains default mode network, a system that instantiates processes that support self-referential mental activity, mentalization and the recollection of prior experiences (Raichle, 2015). In addition to the default mode network, a central role in the empathy feedback loop is played by the observation-execution system also known as the mirror neurons system that includes the IFG as well as the inferior parietal lobe (IPL), regions which were suggested to play a role in emotional empathy (Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Korisky et al., 2020). The mirror neurons, which were first discovered in the monkey ventral premotor cortex (area F5), discharge both during action performance and action observation (Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004) and are believed to allow gaining knowledge of the observed action of others from a personal perspective (Buccino et al., 2004). Notably, it is increasingly acknowledged that the IFG is not only a structure that mediates speech production, but it is involved in action recognition (Buccino et al., 2004) and even in representing abstract representations of behavior (Del Maschio et al., 2022), which may allow representation of others goals intentions and emotions.

Although empathic interactions may unfold over time, the model of Reeck et al. (2016) does not address how empathic reactions change during interactions. To address this issue, Shamay-Tsoory and Hertz (2022) have recently coined the term adaptive empathy, to represent the ability to learn how to adapt one's responses to another's distress. The concept of adaptive empathy points out that it is essential to study how empathic reactions are adapted over time, based on feedback in the context of interactions between empathizer and target (Kozakevich-Arbel et al., 2021). In this context, the empathizer reacts to the distress of the target and may change their own response, based on feedback from the target. Examining how empathic reactions change over time represents a new approach, describing an empathic interactions feedback loop consisting of an empathizer providing responses that change during interactions, based on feedback from the target (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A model of inter-brain plasticity and empathy: the target is experiencing distress, which triggers empathy (emotional/cognitive) in the empathizer. Emotional and cognitive empathy contribute to reduction of distress by multiple means (e.g., mimicry, synchrony, and verbal responses). Activation in the observation-execution system is coupled between the empathizer and the target. This coupling reduces the target's distress by activating reward. As empathic interactions continue the empathizer learns how to adapt her reactions based on the target's feedback. As the empathizer adapts her response to the target, the inter-brain networks between them reconfigure.

The continuous updating of empathic responses demands the participation of a neural network that observes the target's actions and responses and activates the same representations of this behavior. Given the role of the IFG in action recognition (Buccino et al., 2004), which required continues updating of the others' behavior, this region may play a key role in empathic learning. Indeed, previous studies confirm that the IFG is essential for emotional empathy (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009) and that the IFG is activated during empathic learning (Hein et al., 2010), supporting the suggestion that this a core region in the adaptive empathy networks.

Notably, it was recently suggested that activations in the IFG may be demonstrated not only within a single brain, but also simultaneously recorded in the brains of interacting individuals (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2019). Such inter-brain coupling represents coordinated brain activity of two or more interaction partners. Evidence from hyperscanning fNIRS studies shows that inter-brain coupling in the IFG of interacting dyads may underlie various forms of connection, from coordination during dialogues (Jiang et al., 2012) to movement synchrony (Gamliel et al., 2021) and singing in synchrony (Osaka et al., 2015). Furthermore, corroborating evidence from EEG studies further reveals that inter-brain coupling in the alpha band (8 to 12 or 13Hz), which is associated with the mirror neurons system, plays a role in empathic touch (Goldstein et al., 2018), suggesting that inter-brain coupling may also mediate affective empathy.

Given that empathic interaction develop over time, the question remains whether inter-brain coupling can increase over the course of one or multiple interactions. Recently it was suggested that inter-brain plasticity, the ability of interacting brains to modify the coupling between brains in reaction to repeated interactions underlies learning in social interactions (Shamay-Tsoory, 2021). The interbrain plasticity approach views the brain activity of interaction partners as components of an extended neural network that includes interbrain and intra-brain connections that change during interactions. In the case of empathy, it is possible that as the observer adapts her response to the target, the inter-brain networks between them reconfigure. In the initial phase of the interaction the observer may adapt her emotions to those of the target. This involves representing the behavior of each other in the observation-execution system (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2016). The target observes her emotions mimicked by the observer, representing her emotions and then adapts her emotions to be aligned with the observer. This feedback is identified by the observer who may modify her emotions. During repeated interactions, the target and the observer represent each other's emotions in a similar manner and regions in their observation-execution system become gradually coupled. As inter-brain and intra-brain plasticity emerges, fewer sensorimotor signals are required to establish empathy. Over time the observer may improve her empathic responses and share their emotions better. This framework may explain how empathic responses may improve over time and how we learn to mutually adapt our responses during social intereactions.

Conclusion

An abundance of studies examined empathy by focusing on the empathizer, limiting our understanding of the interaction between the empathizer and the target during social interactions. Here, I integrate disparate lines of evidence into a new model of empathic learning. A feedback loop model of empathy is offered, one that accounts for learning how to change empathic reactions based on feedback over time. This model is supported by the new concept of inter-brain plasticity that examines changes in inter-brain coupling during interactions. While the literature on empathy discusses each of the stages of the model, no study to date has directly examined how brain-to-brain coupling change over time.

The model proposed here extends the interpersonal emotion regulation model of Reeck et al. (2016) by taking into account changes in the coupling between the observation execution systems of interaction partners over time. Changes in inter-brain coupling in the IFG represent a core component in this loop. This model offers new insight on the neural basis of empathy and may have clinical implications for understanding population with empathy difficulties.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., and Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain Lang. 89, 370–376. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00356-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Maschio, N., Fedeli, D., Garofalo, G., and Buccino, G. (2022). Evidence for the concreteness of abstract language: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sci. 12, 35–37. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamliel, H. N., Nevat, M., Probolovski, H. Z. G., Karklinsky, M., Han, S., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., et al. (2021). Inter-group conflict affects inter-brain synchrony during synchronized movements. NeuroImage 245, 118661. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, P., Weissman-Fogel, I., Dumas, G., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2018). Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 115, E2528–E2537. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Brüne, M. (2013). Towards a neuroscience of empathy: Ontogeny, phylogeny, brain mechanisms, context and psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1537–1548. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D., and Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68, 149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Dai, B., Peng, D., Zhu, C., Liu, L., Lu, C., et al. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. J. Neurosci. 32, 16064–16069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korisky, A., Eisenberger, N. I., Nevat, M., Weissman-Fogel, I., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2020). A dual-brain approach for understanding the neural mechanisms that underlie the comforting effects of social touch. Cortex 127, 333–346. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozakevich-Arbel, E., Shamay-Tsoory, S., and Hertz, U. (2021). Adaptive Empathy: Empathic response selection as a dynamic, feedback-based learning process. Front. Psychiatry 12, 1248. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.706474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Main, A., Walle, E. A., Kho, C., and Halpern, J. (2017). The interpersonal functions of empathy: a relational perspective. Emot. Rev. 9, 358–366. doi: 10.1177/1754073916669440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osaka, N., Minamoto, T., Yaoi, K., Azuma, M., Shimada, Y. M., Osaka, M., et al. (2015). How two brains make one synchronized mind in the inferior frontal cortex: FNIRS-based hyperscanning during cooperative singing. Front. Psychol. 6, 1811. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain's default mode network. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeck, C., Ames, D. R., and Ochsner, K. N. (2016). The social regulation of emotion: an integrative, cross-disciplinary model. Trends Cognit. Sci. 20, 47–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognit. Brain Res. 3, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2016). The mirror mechanism: a basic principle of brain function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 757–765. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17, 18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2021). Brains that fire together wire together: interbrain plasticity underlies learning in social interactions. Neuroscientist 1073858421996682. doi: 10.1177/1073858421996682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 617–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Hertz, U. (2022). Adaptive empathy: a model for learning empathic responses in response to feedback. Pers. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1008–1023. doi: 10.1177/17456916211031926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Mendelsohn, A. (2019). Real-life neuroscience: an ecological approach to brain and behavior research. Pers. Psychol. Sci. 14, 841–859. doi: 10.1177/1745691619856350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Saporta, N., Marton-Alper, I. Z., and Gvirts, H. Z. (2019). Herding brains: a core neural mechanism for social alignment. Trends Cognit. Sci. 23, 174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, inter-brain coupling, synchrony, learning, plasticity

Citation: Shamay-Tsoory SG (2022) Inter-brain plasticity underlies empathic learning in social interactions. Front. Psychol. 13:951248. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.951248

Received: 23 May 2022; Accepted: 01 September 2022;
Published: 10 November 2022.

Edited by:

Rüdiger J. Seitz, Heinrich Heine University of Düsseldorf, Germany

Reviewed by:

Giovanni Buccino, Ospedale San Raffaele (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2022 Shamay-Tsoory. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, c3NoYW1heSYjeDAwMDQwO3BzeS5oYWlmYS5hYy5pbA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.