Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 22 July 2022
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Credition - An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Nature of Beliefs and Believing View all 42 articles

Grounding abstract concepts and beliefs into experience: The embodied perspective

  • 1Divisione di Neuroscienze, IRCCS San Raffaele and Università Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Milan, Italy
  • 2Faculty of Philosophy, Pontifical University Antonianum, Rome, Italy
  • 3DISF Research Center, Pontifical University of the Holy Cross, Rome, Italy

Introduction

The embodied perspective on language is now supported by several studies showing that activation of neural substrates processing the sensory and motor aspects of the world is not only associated with the processing of language referring to concrete aspects of the world (Buccino et al., 2005; Tettamanti et al., 2005; Gough et al., 2012, 2013; Marino et al., 2013, 2014; Visani et al., 2022) but is also causal to the understanding of concrete language (e.g., Bak et al., 2006; Sato et al., 2008; Kemmerer et al., 2012; Tremblay et al., 2012; Cardona et al., 2013; Fernandino et al., 2013; Desai et al., 2015; Klepp et al., 2015; Buccino et al., 2018; for review Buccino et al., 2016).

Less clear is the situation about abstract domains – i.e., language items referring to less concrete actions (e.g., “I give you my opinion”) or less tangible aspects of the world (e.g., “freedom”) (Glenberg et al., 2008; Boulenger et al., 2009). Such language items bring in a more conceptual and lexical-semantic dimension apparently less amenable to be understood in embodied terms. However, such abstract and conceptual domains are widespread in human linguistic practice and are, consequently, both problematic and interesting for the embodied approach (Gallese and Lakoff, 2005). Limited empirical findings and the variety of theoretical stances on abstract language (see Binder et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2010; Buccino et al., 2019 for reviews) have prompted “hybrid” models on abstract concepts and words. All these models share the embodied approach, but all also posit that acquisition and understanding of abstract concepts and words is only partially grounded in experience-related sensory-motor neural substrates and also resorts to supposed a-modal brain modules processing “pure” language aspects.

In the next section we will first briefly mention such hybrid models; then we will present a “fully embodied” approach (Buccino et al., 2019) and review the available evidence supporting it (Del Maschio et al., 2021). Finally, we will suggest how the advancements in embodied abstract language may shed light on the nature of beliefs.

From embodied abstract language to beliefs

The hybrid models for abstract language and concepts

Three main hybrid models for embodied abstract language have been proposed. The first model (Borghi et al., 2017, 2018) forwards that abstract words and concepts are mainly rooted in the social conventions and ensuing social interactions about abstract content, thus implying the existence of a brain system dedicated to processing propositional aspects of language. The second model maintains that specific features of words' meanings are indeed coded in sensory, motor or even emotional brain circuits. However, words' meaning is ultimately coded in specific, high-order, a-modal, linguistic regions, (Binder et al., 2009; Desai et al., 2015; Mahon, 2015) labeled as “semantic hubs”. The third model views the specificity of abstract words and concepts in the exalted emotional load they display and thus forwards that processing abstract contents specifically involves brain regions for coding, feeling and expressing emotions (Barsalou, 1999; Kousta et al., 2011; Moseley et al., 2012; Vigliocco et al., 2014). It is worth stressing here, however, that a number of studies (Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2011, 2013) have shown that emotions themselves are grounded in the neural structures where the experiences and experiential contexts emotion word refer to are represented.

A fully embodied approach

Buccino et al. (2019) forwarded a fully embodied approach to abstract language, that avoids assuming the existence of a-modal, purely linguistic systems to processing abstract words and concepts. This proposal is based on the idea that abstract words and concepts are such because of the complexity of the experiences attached to them, and not because they are far or detached from concrete experience. Specifically, such experiential complexity can increase according to (i) the number of effectors involved, (ii) the number of sensory systems engaged, as well as (iii) the accumulation over time of concrete life experiences (and related emotional load) attached to those words/concepts. Moreover, the distinction between abstract and concrete words/concepts may be one of degree and not of kind, as the complexity of experiences may increase along a continuum rather than sharply.

This approach allows for a strongly embodied interpretation of the evidence about the neural substrates processing abstract words, thus overcoming the need to elaborate hybrid models. Besides the data reviewed to advocate this fully embodied approach (see also Buccino et al., 2016, 2019), a recent meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies reporting activations related to abstract and concrete concepts further support this fully embodied approach (Del Maschio et al., 2021).

This meta-analysis shows that extensive clusters in the left temporal lobe (including the middle and inferior temporal gyri) and in the left motor cortex, as well as activations in right parietal cortex, left inferior frontal gyrus, and prefrontal regions are found for both concrete and abstract concepts. This suggests that (a) processing of these two kinds of concepts is not sharply segregated in the brain, (b) abstract concepts, like concrete ones, engage brain circuits involved in subjects' interaction with the world, and (c) abstract concepts are not pre-eminently processed in linguistic/propositional format, in semantic hubs or in emotion-related areas (in contrast to what hybrid models propose). Consequently, since semantic hubs are neural structures engaged by both concrete and abstract concepts, it is hard to accept the notion that they may be the “apex” of hierarchical structures progressively moving from processing concrete to abstract situations. Rather, these semantic hubs may play the role to contextualize actions (and related linguistic material) independently of their degree of abstractness.

The metanalysis by Del Maschio et al. (2021) also unveils that brain regions more active for abstract than concrete concepts encompass two major clusters in the left inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis and orbitalis, largely overlapping Broca's region) and middle temporal gyrus, as well as smaller clusters in medial frontal cortex and bilateral temporal poles.

According to the hybrid models, the stronger activation of Broca's region during the processing of abstract language supports the notion that abstract language is coded in a propositional format, since Broca's region is classically considered a linguistic region. In contrast with the classical view, many functions are now attributed to the Broca's region (Amunts and Zilles, 2012; Hardwick et al., 2018). First, in Broca's region there is a motor representation of mouth, hand-arm and, likely, foot actions (Binkofski et al., 1999; Nishitani et al., 2005). Secondly, Broca's region also processes observed and imagined actions (Binder et al., 2009; Hardwick et al., 2018). Thirdly, and more generally, there is also representation of mimicked actions, i.e., actions where the effector is used independently of the object (Lui et al., 2008); mimicked actions may be regarded as a first step in generalizing over object-oriented actions. Finally, Broca's region also codes actions able to mediate a semantic meaning, such as in emblems, but always using a biological effector (Andric et al., 2013).

All this suggests that Broca's region can support a process of generalization (indeed, of abstraction) of actions, but always starting from concrete situations and contexts: it might be said that Broca's region can grasp “what is common” to various instantiations of actions in varying contexts and situations. This view of Broca's activation is consistent with the notion that abstract language engages multiple effectors and contexts in which the use of the effector is not bound to specific objects. Put differently, because abstract concepts and their corresponding verbal labels express actions or entities that are dynamic in time and space, executed by different effectors, and coded in different systems (Buccino et al., 2019), their content, more strongly than for concrete concepts and words, is coded “motorically” in a brain region where actions are represented in a conceptual manner.

Other areas found more active for abstract language (specifically, medial frontal cortex and middle temporal gyrus) are indeed part of the proposed a-modal semantic hubs (Binder et al., 2009; Desai et al., 2015; Mahon, 2015). However, these brain regions are also known to be part of the “default-mode” network that is modulated by demanding cognitive tasks or by social cognition (Mars et al., 2012; Raichle, 2015); their engagement in processing abstract language (i.e., language items attached to complex experience) can be explained assuming that they may contribute to define an appropriate context for the processed words and their link with life experiences and personal beliefs.

Summing up, a fully embodied approach would account for the available data about processing of abstract language in the brain consistently with current knowledge of the functions of brain regions not directly involved in sensory-motor processing and without postulating the existence of a-modal, purely linguistic brain modules.

Implications for belief

Beliefs are high mental processes implying abstract conceptualization and generalization. In this context, the notion of “belief” should be understood broadly, so to encompass moral contexts related to value and religion as well as cognitive convictions on how the world is done and works (and on how we should consequently behave in it). Moreover, beliefs should be conceptualized in strict connection with actions and life conduct. Philosopher C. Peirce stated that “a conception, that is, the rational purport of a word or other expression lies exclusively in its conceivable bearing upon the conduct of life” (Peirce, 1905; p. 162). A recent neuroscientific approach to beliefs indeed posits that selection of beliefs is virtually equivalent to selection of actions (Sugiura et al., 2015). The link between beliefs and actions, as well as the understanding of beliefs as conceptual items, suggests the relevance of an embodied approach to abstractness for the issue of beliefs.

Interestingly, recent developments in the neuroscience of action has established a link between action-related brain processes and the issue of beliefs and personal identity (Jeannerod, 2001, 2006, 2009; see also Colagè and Gobbi, 2017). According to this theory, the assessment of our experiences, especially the results of complex actions, may lead us to build up, and possibly revise, our belief system, which in turn allows for the planning of complex actions (Jeannerod, 2009, p. 263–269).

For this reason, a fully embodied approach to abstract words and concepts may shed light onto the process of building up and revising beliefs, specifically suggesting that beliefs, much like other conceptual domains, can be grounded in actual experiences and their complexity. Three further hints can be added.

First, we have seen that mesial pre-frontal cortex activates in processing abstract language and that this activation can be explained by the need to contextualize and frame abstract words on the background of one's life experiences. Specifically, studies suggest that this brain region is modulated, during the judgment of different relevant social situations and contexts, by the degree of similarity with our own beliefs, attitudes and inclinations (Mitchell et al., 2006; Zaki et al., 2014). Given the complexity of experiences attached to abstract words, these activations may help focusing on a relevant subset of the complex array of experiences attached to an abstract word. It is interesting to note that mesial pre-frontal cortex is proposed as key structure for processing beliefs, and specifically for integrating perception-, action- and emotion/value-related information (Seitz and Angel, 2012; Sugiura et al., 2015).

Secondly, mesial pre-frontal cortices are also known to be part of the mentalizing and affect-related brain systems (Frith and Frith, 2012). Activation of mesial frontal cortex in processing abstract words/concepts may reflect the need of the subject to retrieve his/her social and self-related beliefs to understand abstract linguistic items properly (see also Buccino and Colagè, 2017).

Finally, a fully embodied approach to beliefs is also consistent with the idea that such linguistic transactions among human beings are anyway grounded in real experiences. Linguistic transactions are effective in belief formation to the extent to which they help us sharing and combining our experiential baggage (Colagè and Buccino, 2016).

Author contributions

GB conceptualized the paper and revised the manuscript. IC wrote the first draft of the manuscript and revised it. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This paper is funded by Dr. Rüdiger Seitz, via the Volkswagen Foundation, Siemens Healthineers, and the Betz Foundation.

Acknowledgments

Both authors are grateful to the Special Issue Editors, and especially to H.-F. Angel and R. Setiz for past meetings they organized in Graz and the unique opportunity they offered to share ideas and experiences in the field of beliefs and credition.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Amunts, K., and Zilles, K. (2012). Architecture and organizational principles of Broca's region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 418–426. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andric, M., Solodkin, A., Buccino, G., Goldin-Meadow, S., Rizzolatti, G., and Small, S.L. (2013). Brain function overlaps when people observe emblems, speech, and grasping. Neuropsychologia 51, 1619–1629. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., Yancopoulou, D., Nestor, P. J., Xuereb, J. H., Spillantini, M. G., Pulvermüller, F., et al. (2006). Clinical, imaging and pathological correlates of a hereditary deficit in verb and action processing. Brain 129, 321–332. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W.. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems and emotion. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 612–613. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99252144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Posse, S., Seitz, R. J., Rizzolatti, G., and Freund, H. J. (1999). A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: Evidence from an fMRI-study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 3276–3286. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00753.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., and Tummolini, L. (2018). Abstract concepts, language and sociality: from acquisition to inner speech. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373, 20170134. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., and Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychol. Bull. 143, 263–292. doi: 10.1037/bul0000089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., and Pulvermueller, F. (2009). Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1905–1914. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., and Colagè, I. (2017). “New concepts on the motor system: implications for emotions and creditions”, in Processes of Believing: The Acquisition, Maintenance, and Change in Creditions, eds H. -F.Angel, R. F. Paloutzian, A. L. C. Runehov, and R. J. Seitz (Cham: Springer International Publishing AG), 97–109. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-50924-2_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Colagè, I., Gobbi, N., and Bonaccorso, G. (2016). Grounding meaning in experience: a broad perspective on embodied language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Colagè, I., Silipo, F., and D'Ambrosio, P. (2019). The concreteness of abstract language: an ancient issue and a new perspective. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1385–1401. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01851-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Dalla Volta, R., Arabia, G., Morelli, M., Chiriaco, C., Lupo, A., et al. (2018). Processing graspable object images and their nouns is impaired in Parkinson's disease patients. Cortex 100, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 355–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardona, J. F., Gershanik, G., Gelormini-Lezama, C., Houck, A. L., Cardona, S., Kargieman, L., et al. (2013). Action-verb processing in Parkinson's disease: new pathways for motor-language coupling. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 1355–1373. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0510-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colagè, I., and Buccino, G. (2016). Il linguaggio e l'esperienza: dalle neuroscienze all'ambiente naturale e culturale. Antonianum 91, 1047–1061.

Google Scholar

Colagè, I., and Gobbi, N. (2017). “Creditions and Self-Identity: Embodiment Between Actions and (Life) Decisions”, in Processes of Believing: The Acquisition, Maintenance, and Change in Creditions, eds H. -F.Angel, R. F. Paloutzian, A. L. C. Runehov, and R. J. Seitz (Springer International Publishing AG), 247–262. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-50924-2_18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Maschio, N., Fedeli, D., Garofalo, G., and Buccino, G. (2021). Evidence for the concreteness of abstract language: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sci. 12, 32. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desai, R. H., Herter, T., Riccardi, N., Rorden, C., and Fridriksson, J. (2015). Concepts within reach: action performance predicts action language processing in stroke. Neuropsychologia 71, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandino, L., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Blindauer, K., Hiner, B., Spangler, K., et al. (2013). Where is the action? Action sentence processing in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 51, 1510–1517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 287–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallese, V., and Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain's concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 22, 455–479. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., and Buccino, G. (2008). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61 905–919. doi: 10.1080/17470210701625550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, P. M., Campione, G. C., and Buccino, G. (2013). Fine tuned modulation of the motor system by adjectives expressing positive and negative properties. Brain Lang. 125, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, P. M., Riggio, L., Chersi, F., Sato, M., Fogassi, L., and Buccino, G. (2012). Nouns referring to tools and natural objects differentially modulate the motor system. Neuropsychologia 50, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardwick, R. M., Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S. B., and Swinnen, S. P. (2018). Neural correlates of action: comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 94, 31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M.. (2001). Neural simulation of actions: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14, 103–109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M.. (2006). Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M.. (2009). Le Cerveau Volontaire. Paris: Odile Jacob.

Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D., Rudrauf, D., Manzel, K., and Tranel, D. (2012). Behavioral patterns and lesion sites associated with impaired processing of lexical and conceptual knowledge of actions. Cortex 48, 826–848. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klepp, A., Niccolai, V., Buccino, G., Schnitzler, A., and Biermann-Rubena, K. (2015). Language–motor interference reflected in MEG beta oscillations. NeuroImage 109, 438–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., and Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 140, 14–34. doi: 10.1037/a0021446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lui, F., Buccino, G., Duzzi, D., Benuzzi, F., Crisi, G., Baraldi, P., et al. (2008). Neural substrates for observing and imagining non-object-directed actions. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 261–275. doi: 10.1080/17470910701458551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahon, B. Z.. (2015). What is embodied about cognition? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 420–429. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.987791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, B., Sirianni, M., Dalla Volta, R., Magliocco, F., Silipo, F., Quattrone, A., et al. (2014). Viewing photos and reading nouns of natural graspable objects similarly modulate motor responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 968. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, B. F., Gough, P. M., Gallese, V., Riggio, L., and Buccino, G. (2013). How the motor system handles nouns: a behavioral study. Psychol. Res. 77, 64–73. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Neubert, F. X., Noonan, M. A. P., Sallet, J., Toni, I., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 189. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial prefrontal contribution to judgements of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 18, 655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1634–1647. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishitani, N., Schürmann, M., Amunts, K., and Hari, R. (2005). Broca's region: from action to language. Physiology 20, 60–69. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00043.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peirce, C. S.. (1905). What pragmatism is. Monist 15, 161–181. doi: 10.5840/monist190515230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E.. (2015). The brain's default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, M., Mengarelli, M., Riggio, L., Gallese, V., and Buccino, G. (2008). Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing. Brain Lang. 105, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitz, R. J., and Angel, H. F. (2012). Processes of believing – a review and conceptual account. Rev Neurosci. 23, 303–309. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiura, M., Seitz, R. J., and Angel, H. F. (2015). Models and neural bases of the believing process. J. Behav. Brain. Sci. 5, 12–23. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2015.51002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 273–281. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., Sato, M., and Small, S. L. (2012). TMS-induced modulation of action sentence priming in the ventral premotor cortex. Neuropsychologia 50, 319–326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S.-T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., et al. (2014). The neural representation of abstract words: the role of emotion. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1767–1777. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visani, E., Rossi Sebastiano, D., Duran, D., Garofalo, G., Magliocco, F., Silipo, F., et al. (2022). The semantics of natural objects and tools in the brain: a combined behavioral and MEG study. Brain Sci. 12, 97. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., and Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1459–1468. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., and Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Situating emotional experience. Front. Human Neurosci. 26, 764. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., and Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia 49, 1105–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., Lòpez, G., and Mitchell, J. P. (2014). Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex co-varies with revealed social preferences: evidence for person-invariant value. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 9, 464–469. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: embodied language, abstract concepts, beliefs, semantic hub, Broca's region

Citation: Buccino G and Colagè I (2022) Grounding abstract concepts and beliefs into experience: The embodied perspective. Front. Psychol. 13:943765. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.943765

Received: 14 May 2022; Accepted: 01 July 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.

Edited by:

Rüdiger J. Seitz, Heinrich Heine University of Düsseldorf, Germany

Reviewed by:

Loïc P. Heurley, Université Paris Nanterre, France

Copyright © 2022 Buccino and Colagè. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giovanni Buccino, YnVjY2luby5naW92YW5uaSYjeDAwMDQwO2hzci5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.