Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 08 August 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Contributions of naturalistic parent-child conversations to children’s science learning during informal learning at an aquarium and at home

\r\nGrace Ocular*Grace Ocular1*Kimberly R. Kelly*Kimberly R. Kelly2*Lizbeth MillanLizbeth Millan2Savannah NevesSavannah Neves2Kateri AvilaKateri Avila2Betina HsiehBetina Hsieh3Claudine MalolesClaudine Maloles2
  • 1Child Language Interactions and Memory Lab, Department of Teacher Education, California State University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA, United States
  • 2Child Language Interactions and Memory Lab, Department of Human Development, California State University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Teacher Education, California State University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA, United States

This study examined the naturalistic conversations of 62 parent-child dyads during informal learning at an aquarium and with a subsample at home. Children (Mage = 69.8 months) with their parents were observed and audio recorded while exploring an aquarium exhibit, and a subset of dyads returned recorded home conversations reminiscing about the aquarium visit. Parent-child conversations at the aquarium were coded for child science talk and a range of parent talk variables, and parent-child conversations at home were coded for child science talk and talk about the value of the aquarium visit. Results revealed that parents tended to use more elaborative statements compared to other talk types in the aquarium. Yet, the different types of questions and statements that parents used with their children at the aquarium differentially related to their children’s science talk in the aquarium and while reminiscing at home. Findings highlight often-overlooked types of parent talk that provide meaningful ways for families to engage in science and may lead to positive child learning outcomes.

Introduction

Young children meaningfully engage in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) practices and begin to develop science habits of mind through early experiences (Campbell et al., 2018), which shape children’s science and educational outcomes (National Research Council, 2007; McClure et al., 2017). Informal learning environments (ILE) such as science museums and aquariums provide families with opportunities to engage with science in approachable ways. The home is also an important ILE in which families construct cultural knowledge about informal science processes and concepts (Riojas-Cortez et al., 2008). However, we know relatively little about how families connect their informal science learning between and across the informal spaces where children have early meaningful experiences. This issue is important because children and families may bring their Funds of Knowledge (FoK)—that is, historically undervalued skills and knowledge brought from home and community that are used during learning—(Moll et al., 1992; Basu and Calabrese Barton, 2007) to their participation in informal science across ILE spaces (Mathai, 2017), allowing everyday family practices in one space to build upon and strengthen the developing knowledge in another. This study examined parent-child conversations as families explored an aquarium exhibit, investigating to what extent parental talk connects with children’s talk about science at the aquarium and while reminiscing at home.

Aquariums are unique ILE. Visitor demographics and perceptions suggest they are comparatively more pluralistic spaces, with two-thirds of aquarium visitors earning less than $100,000 annually (Association of Zoos & Aquariums, 2021) compared to science museum visitors with incomes above $200,000 (Bingham, 2019). Visitor engagement with animals at aquariums fosters an educational experience that includes observation and information seeking (Kisiel et al., 2012), encouraging visitors to act as scientifically informed observers and participants (Cainey et al., 2012). Aquariums, as live animal settings, contribute to early development of biological knowledge (Geerdts et al., 2015) where children and parents have conversations that include talk about scientific processes, technology, math, and biological content (Kelly et al., 2020). Thus, aquariums promote learning about life sciences and conservationism (Ash et al., 2007; Bidart and Russell, 2017) and create opportunities for visitors to make connections with personal beliefs, attitudes, and experiences (Kisiel et al., 2012).

Parents and children also engage in informal science learning at home. Studies of home science literacy show that parents and children engage in science-related activities in various ways. Home science activity packets with prescribed prompts and questions engage families in scientific inquiry practices, including observation, recording data, and explanation (Strickler-Eppard et al., 2019). Parent-child dialogic science book reading with children at home supports children’s science knowledge gains (Mantzicopoulos et al., 2013). However, studies of science at home often focus on traditional science tasks and fewer have examined families’ everyday interactions. Cooking provides opportunities for parents and children to tap into their culture and existing knowledge about food and food preparation that can promote acquisition of science knowledge (e.g., measurement and chemical reactions) (Riojas-Cortez et al., 2008; Morris et al., 2021). Studies of family storytelling immediately following tinkering at a children’s science museum and days later at home (e.g., Jant et al., 2014; Marcus et al., 2021) also suggest the importance of investigating the families’ everyday science practices.

We take the view that children’s learning is a developmental process, and the development of thinking occurs within children’s everyday interactions (Vygotsky, 1978). From the sociocultural framework, children’s learning in community and home environments is situated within social interactions with expert others who guide children to attend to valued content and processes (Wood et al., 1976; Vygotsky, 1978). This view positions language as a potent cultural tool that mediates the acquisition of knowledge during learning activities and the development of new ways of thinking through dialog with others (Halliday, 1993; Schleppegrell, 2004). Within informal learning contexts, dialog between parents and children offers opportunities for engagement in scientific processes and science knowledge-building (Crowley et al., 2001a; Ash, 2003; Attisano et al., 2021).

Early family conversations are critical for the development of young children’s science thinking and learning (Callanan, 2012; Kelly et al., 2020) and may have long-term impacts on science interest and transfer of scientific knowledge to contexts beyond the ILE (Pagano et al., 2020). Conversations rich with elaborative language during informal learning allow children to use science-specific language (Marcus et al., 2017) and scaffold their short- and long-term science learning (Callanan and Jipson, 2001; Benjamin et al., 2010). Moreover, elaborative parent-child conversations at home following a science lesson at school boost children’s science learning (Leichtman et al., 2017).

From classic research, we know that parents who create highly elaborative contexts during day-to-day conversations with their children include more wh-questions and statements that add new information. Other parents use more yes/no questions, ask for the same information repetitively (Peterson and McCabe, 1992), creating a less elaborative context (Fivush et al., 2006). Parents who ask many wh-questions at science museums or as the event unfolds have children who show greater understanding, retention, and recall of their experiences during informal learning (Boland et al., 2003; Benjamin et al., 2010; Jant et al., 2014). Although the role of parent-child conversations in children’s science learning has been documented separately in the contexts of home and community learning spaces, less is known about how family conversations in one space extend, build upon, and exist within other learning spaces. To address this gap, we use a learning ecologies framework (Barron, 2006; Siemens, 2007) to explore and connect spaces where the development of informal science knowledge occurs. We view family reminiscing conversations as a tool that mediates family science engagement and children’s science learning across ILEs, allowing for the developmental gains in one space to transfer to another. Reminiscing conversations following an event influence the meaning that children make of the event, how memory is consolidated, and what information is retained and recalled (Ornstein et al., 2004; Haden, 2010) and are likely a natural extension of families’ at-home informal learning (Peterson and McCabe, 1994; Reese and Brown, 2000).

The current study examined family reminiscing after the ILE visit, because we view the content of such conversations as an outcome and continuation of family informal learning (Crowley et al., 2001a; Leinhardt et al., 2003). Moreover, family at-home conversations about their informal learning experiences may bridge children’s learning ecologies. Parent and child reflections contain science talk similar to that used during informal learning in a science museum (Marcus et al., 2017). Everyday family conversations can ultimately serve to bridge developmental processes occurring across learning ecologies. The FoK perspective (Moll et al., 1992) provides a counternarrative to deficit models of learning. From an FoK lens, we can look beyond the historically privileged ways of doing science to acknowledge science learning in everyday interactions. Reflecting on the value of the ILE visit may be one way that parents bridge children’s learning ecologies. Families may reflect on the value of informal learning experiences while exiting a museum exhibit (Pagano et al., 2019). However, research has not yet explored how families talk about the value of ILE visits when reminiscing at home.

The current study focused on the relations between parent talk at the aquarium and children’s talk about science during and after informal learning. We examined how parent-child conversations at the aquarium linked to their at-home reminiscing conversations. As such, we observed and recorded the naturalistic interactions of parent-child dyads as they explored a live animal exhibit in an aquarium. We later collected a recorded conversation from the dyad in which they reminisced about the aquarium visit.

Transcripts of the conversations at the aquarium were analyzed for elements of parent elaborative talk, including wh-questions and elaborative statements, adapting approaches by Benjamin et al. (2010) and Jant et al. (2014). We extend prior work by also analyzing parent talk for closed-ended questions and repetitions (Reese et al., 1993; Peterson and McCabe, 1994), acknowledging that families use a diverse array of conversational styles during informal learning. Research generally reports no difference in parent-child reminiscing by parent education or income (Fivush et al., 2006). Differences in reminiscing by child gender have been mixed (Farrant, 2000; Newcombe and Reese, 2004), with some studies showing that parents are more elaborative with their daughters than sons (Reese et al., 1996) and others finding no differences (Laible, 2004; Melzi et al., 2011). During informal science interactions, however, parents explain more to boys than girls (Crowley et al., 2001b). For our first research aim, we explored the extent to which parents used elaborative talk with their children during informal learning at an aquarium and what, if any, differences occurred across family demographics.

Also, children’s talk at the aquarium and at home was coded for references to science processes, technology, math, and biological science content (henceforth: STMB), using a coding system developed for the aquarium (Kelly et al., 2020). For our second research aim, we investigated what type of parental talk at the aquarium predicted children’s science talk at the aquarium and at home. We expected that parents who used more elaborative talk at the aquarium would have children who used more STMB talk at the aquarium and at home (Benjamin et al., 2010; Jant et al., 2014). Lastly, reminiscing conversations were also investigated for the degree to which families reflected on valuing the aquarium experience, adapting work by Pagano et al. (2019). While some parents value museum visits more for recreational family experiences than for children’s learning (Letourneau et al., 2017), others value seeing their children’s learning during their visit (Luke et al., 2019). Our third research aim examined whether parental talk at the aquarium related to parent-child talk about valuing the aquarium visit at home. The research aims that investigate the relations of parent talk in the aquarium with child home STMB talk and dyad value talk were explored with pilot data from a subsample of families who recorded home conversations.

Materials and methods

Participants

The sample included 62 children (31 girls; Mage = 69.8 months, SD = 21.04) and 62 parents (35 mothers; Mage = 36.7 years, SD = 6.26) from a larger study of families1 who were recruited using convenience sampling at an aquarium in Southern California. One parent-child dyad was excluded because they primarily spoke in a language other than English during observations. Participants came from diverse racial/ethnic and socioeconomic backgrounds (see Table 1). Families were included if they had at least one child that was between three and eight years old and the child was accompanied by a legal guardian. Families received a small thank you gift for participating.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for child, parent, and family demographic variables.

Procedure and measures

Aquarium

Participants were recruited at one of two live sea animal exhibits, which were comparable in length and location.

For the observation group (n = 50), researchers invited one child and one parent to participate at the exhibit entrance. If the family group included more than one eligible child, the parent(s) chose which child would participate. After informed consent, researchers gave parents a Bluetooth microphone to place on their clothing and children wore a backpack with a microphone on the strap. Researchers observed and audio recorded parent-child dyads while they explored the exhibit. At the end of exhibit, parents completed the study survey. For the survey group (n = 153)2, researchers recruited families at the exhibit exit. After informed consent, parents completed the study survey. The survey asked for child and parent gender and age, parent education, and family income.

Home follow-up

Twenty-five dyads were retained for the study follow-up including 13 from the observation group and 12 from survey group. To collect home conversations, a researcher contacted parents within two weeks and provided step-by-step instructions. Parents were instructed to record a home conversation with the target child using either a researcher-provided audio recorder or the voice memo app on their personal smartphones. Parents were asked to go to a quiet area with their child and “talk with your child about the day at the aquarium as you normally would.” Parents returned the recorders by mail in a prepaid envelope, or parents texted the voice memos to the researcher.

Parent-child conversations

Trained research assistants transcribed verbatim the recorded parent-child conversations using a version of the CHAT language transcription system (MacWhinney, 2000) and verified transcripts for accuracy. Parent and child language was parsed at the utterance level. The end of the utterance was determined by intonation and pauses or by coinciding with grammatically or meaningfully cohesive units.

Coding

The principal investigator (second author) and trained research assistants coded transcripts of the aquarium and home conversations.

Parental talk

Using a coding system that bridges approaches in applied linguistics and developmental psychology, we first identified whether parent utterances were declarative statements or questions to capture the full range of parent utterances. Then using codes adapted from Reese et al. (1993) and Haden et al. (2014), we coded parent questions as either wh-questions, which were open-ended requests for new information relating to the aquarium, exhibit or sea animals (e.g., “Where is the Bluestreak Cleaner?”), or either/or questions, which were closed-ended requests that require a simple confirmation or negation of information (e.g., “Do you like this jellyfish?”) or choice from a finite set of options offered by the speaker (e.g., “Is it a puffer fish or a clownfish?”). We coded parent declarative statements that provided new information relating to the aquarium, exhibit, or sea animals as elaborative statements (e.g., “Look they’re upside down”). Requested or offered information that the parent asked or provided previously was coded as repetition (e.g., “What is it? What is that?”).

Child STMB talk

We measured children’s talk about science at the aquarium and at home using the STMB coding system developed for informal learning at the aquarium by Kelly et al. (2020). Child STMB talk included: science process - talk that problem-solved; characterized shape, color, or size; compared similarities or differences; and hypothesized (e.g., “What are they doing? They’re exploring the ocean.”); technology - mentions of technology in the exhibit (e.g., “What does that button do?”); mathematics – talk of numbers or geometric shapes and that quantified an amount (e.g., “What shape is its body? A triangle.”); and biological science - talk specific to biological, ecological, and life science contexts including life cycle, biological function or labeling using a scientific animal name (e.g., “Oh, they’re good at camouflaging.”).

Dyad value talk

We measured the extent to which parents and children revealed valuing their experiences at the aquarium using the value coding system by Pagano et al. (2019). Parent and child utterances coded for value talk included mentions of what the experience meant to the visitor, including emotional expressions (e.g., “I like petting them”), mentions of learning (e.g., “What else did you learn about?”), and evaluations of their experiences (e.g., “When he said the penguins are in a rush, that was super funny”).

Reliability

The second author and trained research assistants used 20% of the transcripts to establish reliability. Cohen’s kappa was k = 0.88 for parent talk, k = 0.77 for child STMB talk, and k = 0.76 for dyad value talk. Disagreements were resolved by consensus and included in the final dataset. Research assistants coded the remaining data.

Results

Preliminary analysis

Preliminary analyses focused on differences in outcome variables (child STMB talk and dyad value talk) to explore potential covariates. Table 2 shows bivariate correlations between covariate and criterion variables. Families’ visit length averaged 16.3 min in the exhibits (range = 2–32 min). Visit length correlated with all four parent talk variables, ps < 0.025, as well as child STMB talk at home and dyad value talk at home, ps < 0.05. Thus, visit length was included in analyses with criterion variables measured in the home conversations. The number of adults and children varied for each family. Number of children negatively correlated with parent wh-questions and either/or questions, ps ≤ 0.02. No significant correlations were found with the criterion variables, ps > 0.18. Child age, parent education, nor family income were correlated with any of the criterion variables, ps > 0.20. Separate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) revealed all but one relationship between the remaining background variables (child gender, adult gender, and race/ethnicity) and the criterion variables was non-significant, ps > 0.45. Children used more STMB talk at the aquarium with fathers (M = 16.67, SD = 12.46) than with mothers (M = 9.59, SD = 8.52), F(1, 48) = –2.38, p = 0.021. Thus, parent gender was included in analyses with child aquarium STMB talk.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between covariates and outcomes variables.

Main analyses

Parent elaborative talk and family demographics during informal learning

We first examined the descriptive statistics of the four parent talk variables. On average, parents used elaborative statements (M = 24.30, SD = 22.56) more than either/or questions (M = 9.34, SD = 7.61), which in turn were used more than wh-questions (M = 7.22, SD = 6.10) and repetitions (M = 3.86, SD = 5.56). Correlations and multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) examined relations between parent talk and parent, child, and family demographics (child age, gender, parent gender, education, and family income), with relevant covariates. Child age was negatively correlated with parent wh-questions [r(47) = –0.33, p = 0.023], either/or questions [r(47) = –0.37, p = 0.008], and repetitions [r(47) = –0.38, p = 0.007], controlling for visit length. Figure 1 shows the average frequency and standard errors for parent talk at the aquarium by child gender. MANCOVA revealed parents used more elaborative statements with boys (M = 40.00, SD = 35.50) than with girls (M = 21.31, SD = 14.40), F(1, 47) = 5.16, MSE = 353.53, partial η2 = 0.099, p = 0.027. As indicated in Figure 1, parents averaged more wh-questions and repetitions with girls than with boys, but the difference was not statistically significant.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average frequency of parent talk by child gender.

Parent talk at the aquarium with child STMB talk and dyad value talk

Children averaged 12.56 (range: 0–47, SD = 10.83) instances of STMB talk in the aquarium and 13.04 (range 1–65: SD = 15.37) instances of STMB talk at home. Dyads averaged 14.75 (range: 1–42, SD = 11.51) instances of value talk at home. We conducted multiple linear regressions, using the stepwise method, to explore initial models, predicting separately child aquarium STMB talk, home STMB talk, and dyad value talk at home based on parent elaborative talk (i.e., wh-questions plus statements), either/or questions, and repetitions in the aquarium, with relevant covariates. Following a commonly used method (i.e., Reese et al., 1993; Laible, 2004; Leyva et al., 2020), we combined wh-questions and elaborative statements creating a single parent elaborative talk variable to simplify the models. We did so because the two variables highly correlate and both theoretically and empirically function together to create a rich conversational context (Fivush et al., 2006).

STMB talk at the aquarium

The regression for child STMB talk at the aquarium yielded R2 = 0.24, F (4,45) = 3.58, p < 0.013. The top of Table 3 displays the significant predictor variables. Consistent with our hypothesis, parental elaborative talk was significantly positively associated with child aquarium STMB talk over and above visit length, child age and parent gender, with an average increase of.41 instances of child aquarium STMB talk for every 1-unit increase in parental elaborative talk.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multiple linear regression models predicting child STMB talk at the aquarium (top), child STMB talk at home (middle), and dyad value talk at home (bottom) from parent talk in the aquarium with covariates.

STMB talk at home

The regression for child STMB talk at home yielded R2 = 0.75, F (5,11) = 3.58, p = 0.08. The middle of Table 3 displays the significant predictor variables. Contrary to our expectations, parental elaboration was not significantly associated with child home STMB talk. Instead, parent either/or questions were marginally positively associated with child home STMB talk, with an average increase of 2.31 instances of child home STMB talk for every 1-unit increase in parent either/or questions at the aquarium. Also, parent repetitions were marginally negatively associated with child home STMB talk, with an average decrease of 2.87 instances of child home STMB talk for every 1-unit increase in parent repetitions.

Value talk at home

The regression for dyad value talk yielded R2 = 0.69, F (5,11) = 2.70, p = 0.13. The bottom of Table 3 displays the model results. Parent talk variables were not significantly associated with value talk, when controlling for time in the aquarium. Only time in the exhibit significantly predicted value talk.

Discussion

This study explored whether and how parent-child conversations bridge children’s science learning across informal learning ecologies. Parents tended to use more elaborative statements compared to other talk types in the aquarium. Yet, the different types of questions and statements that parents used with their children in the aquarium differentially related to their children’s science talk in the aquarium and while reminiscing at home.

Children whose parents used more elaborative talk during informal learning talked more about science in the aquarium. Social interactions, including family conversations, guide children to attend to content and processes that are highly valued within their communities (Wood et al., 1976). Parents who asked for and provided new information during informal learning signal to their children what scientific content and processes are important to attend to and thus talk about. Our finding that naturally occurring elaborative talk in everyday family conversations provided children with opportunities to engage in science extends Jant et al.’s (2014) study, which cued parents to ask elaborative questions at a museum. Children’s learning can occur in everyday conversations (Falk and Dierking, 2000; Hirsch-Pasek et al., 2015), which, as our results suggest, promotes their developing scientific thinking and knowledge.

Additionally, parents used more elaborative statements with boys than with girls in the aquarium. This finding is consistent with studies on parent scientific explanations (Crowley, 2000; Crowley et al., 2001b). In our study, this disparity may have been mitigated by parents’ use of slightly more wh-questions and repetitions with girls, as there were no gender differences in child STMB talk. Nevertheless, this finding is a reminder for ILE educators and designers of the pernicious presence of gendered behaviors in family informal science interactions, which can have long-term ramifications for children’s scientific literacy (Tenenbaum et al., 2005).

Contrary to our expectations, parental elaborative talk in the aquarium was not related to child science talk while reminiscing at home. Instead, it appears that children whose parents used more either/or questions may talk more about science while reminiscing about the aquarium. These results must be taken with caution due to the reduction of our retained sample for the home conversations. Nevertheless, these preliminary findings add dimension to a body of research that has often focused on the role of parent wh-questions or elaborative statements in children’s informal science learning (e.g., Boland et al., 2003; Jant et al., 2014). Because families from different backgrounds bring to informal learning their FoK (Moll et al., 1992) and ways of talking with each other (Heath, 1983; Carmiol et al., 2019), our findings highlight the importance of investigating the full range of conversational styles in ILE.

Often-overlooked types of parent talk likely provide meaningful ways for families to engage in science and lead to positive child learning outcomes. Thus, this study broadens past informal learning work by considering all types of talk beyond privileged and formal ways of talking about science (i.e., causal talk and science explanations) (Callanan and Oakes, 1992; Joy et al., 2021). From a FoK perspective, all family conversation styles should be considered and leveraged across learning ecologies for children’s science learning, highlighting that knowledge and skills are acquired through everyday experiences that are historically and culturally unique (Moll et al., 1992). Our study expands the boundaries of what we define as science engagement in ILE and emphasizes that participation, conversations, and engagement in these spaces is what counts as science (Callanan et al., 2013).

Finally, parent talk in the aquarium was not related to the degree to which dyads reflected on the value of the aquarium visit. Only the amount of time that families spent in the exhibit was predicted value talk at home. Although the direction of the relationship cannot be determined due to the correlational nature of the study, this finding is consistent with theories of learning with adults (Astin, 1984) and studies of effective educational practice (Chickering and Gamson, 1987) that time spent on task is a powerful predictor of educational effectiveness. Debate about the role of time spent in museums is longstanding (e.g., Serrell, 1997; Shettel, 1997). Nonetheless, that the time a family spent in the aquarium predicted the extent to which they later revealed valuing the experience has meaningful implications for exhibit design. For example, exhibits that are intended to foster values, such as conservationism or multicultural awareness, should have a longer hold time for lasting impact.

Limitations

Certain study features limit our conclusions. We focused only on how parents talked with their children during informal learning. Future research examining the content of parent talk could reveal the relative contributions of quality versus content of informal learning conversations to understand better the signals that parents send to their children about what is valued as science. Also, we experienced sample attrition in the follow-up, despite our best efforts to make the follow-up easy and free for families. Not only did the small sample likely reduce statistical power in our regression analyses, it also limits the extent to which we can generalize the findings of links between parent talk in the aquarium and talk at home. The results from the pilot home data are preliminary but show promise for future studies with larger samples. Future studies should implement new video conferencing technologies into procedures, which may streamline the recording and collection of home conversations. Although we cannot draw conclusions about causality, we believe the naturalistic study design is a strength for its ecological validity.

Conclusions

This study provides new information about the kinds of parent-child interactions that may promote children’s talk about science content as well science processes during informal learning and across learning ecologies. The results suggest that ILE educators can model a conversational style that includes abundant either/or questions and wh-questions, acknowledging and leveraging families’ existing strengths to support learning in aquariums and other ILEs. Young learners see connections to science across the everyday settings in which they participate, including school, home, and community (Zimmerman and Bell, 2014). Continuing to identify ways to bridge children’s learning ecologies can enhance theoretical understandings of how science learning in one setting can promote learning across other learning contexts. Finding ways to create “seamless ecologies” (Meyers et al., 2013) can empower families to use their strengths across familiar and new settings to promote science learning.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board’s Committee at California State University Long Beach. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

KK conceptualized and designed the study. GO, KK, BH, and CM contributed to data collection. GO, KK, and LM contributed to data coding and database organization. KK performed the statistical analysis. GO wrote the first draft of the “Materials and methods” section, and KK wrote the first draft of the “Results” section. GO and KK contributed equally to the “Introduction” and “Discussion” sections. SN and KA contributed to the literature review. GO and KK revised the manuscript, read, and approved the submitted version. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded, in part, with support from CSULB Multidisciplinary Grant, CSULB Summer Student Research Fund and Graduate Research Fellowship Award. Funding sources had no involvement in the research at any stage.

Acknowledgments

We thank the families who participated in the study and the Aquarium of the Pacific in Long Beach, CA for their partnership. We also thank Heidi Alvarado, Atazshii Austin, Jo Brunton and Eva Drexler for their assistance in data collection as well as Queena Hoang and Victoria Min for their assistance with the reference page.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ The larger study sample included N = 204 parent-child dyads, which consists of dyads who completed only the study survey (N = 153) and those who completed an additional observation (N = 51).
  2. ^ Data for families who agreed to the survey only procedures but did not complete the follow-up home observation (n = 141) were not included in the current study.

References

Ash, D. (2003). Dialogic inquiry in life science conversations of family groups in a museum. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 40, 138–162. doi: 10.1002/tea.10069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ash, D., Crain, R., Brandt, C., Loomis, M., Wheaton, M., and Bennett, C. (2007). Talk, tools, and tensions: observing biological talk over time. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 29, 1581–1602. doi: 10.1080/09500690701494118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Association of Zoos & Aquariums (2021). Visitor Demographics. Available online at: http://aza.org/partnerships-visitor-demographics

Google Scholar

Astin, A. W. (1984). Student involvement: a developmental theory for higher education. J. Coll. Stud. Pers. 25, 297–308.

Google Scholar

Attisano, E., Nancekivell, S. E., and Denison, S. (2021). Components and mechanisms: how children talk about machines in museum exhibits.’. Front. Psychol. 12:636601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.636601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barron, B. (2006). Interest and self-sustained learning as catalysts of development: a learning ecology perspective. Hum. Dev. 49, 193–224. doi: 10.1159/000094368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basu, S. J., and Calabrese Barton, A. (2007). Developing a sustained interest in science among urban minority youth. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 44, 466–489. doi: 10.1002/tea.20143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., and Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver–child interactions in a children’s museum. Dev. Psychol. 46:502. doi: 10.1037/a0017822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidart, T., and Russell, C. (2017). “Learning science in aquariums and on whale watching boats: the hidden curriculum of the deployment of other animals,” in Animals and Science Education, eds M. Mueller, D. J. Tippins, and A. J. Steward (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 41–50. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-56375-6_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bingham, G. (2019). Are Museums Still Relevant in the Digital age? Understanding Museum Attendance by Visitor Demographics. Available online at: https://www.ubimo.com/blog/articles-and-research/museum-attendance-by-visitor-demographics/ (accessed September 19, 2019).

Google Scholar

Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., and Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children’s memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. J. Cogn. Dev. 4, 39–65. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2003.9669682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cainey, J., Bowker, R., Humphrey, L., and Murray, N. (2012). Assessing informal learning in an aquarium using pre- and post-visit drawings. Educ. Res. Eval. 18, 265–281. doi: 10.1080/13803611.2012.670400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callanan, M. A. (2012). Conducting cognitive developmental research in museums: theoretical issues and practical considerations. J. Cogn. Dev. 13, 137–151. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.666730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callanan, M. A., and Jipson, J. L. (2001). “Explanatory conversations and young children’s developing scientific literacy,” in Designing for Science: Implications From Everyday, Classroom, and Professional Science, eds K. Crowley, C. Schunn, and T. Okada (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).

Google Scholar

Callanan, M. A., and Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: causal thinking in everyday activity. Cogn. Dev. 7, 213–233.

Google Scholar

Callanan, M., Luce, M., Triona, L., Rigney, J., Siegel, D., and Jipson, J. (2013). “What counts as science in everyday and family interactions?,” in Lost Opportunities, eds B. Bevan, P. Bell, R. Stevens, and A. Razfar (Dordrecht: Springer), 29–48.

Google Scholar

Campbell, C., Speldewinde, C., Howitt, C., and MacDonald, A. (2018). STEM practice in the early years. Creat. Educ. 9:11. doi: 10.4236/ce.2018.91002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmiol, A. M., Kelly, K. R., Ocular, G., Ríos-Reyes, M., González-Chaves, M., and Plascencia, J. (2019). Talking about past experiences in two cultural contexts: children’s narrative structure and maternal elaboration in dyads from Costa Rica and the United States. Early Educ. Dev. 31, 234–249.

Google Scholar

Chickering, A. W., and Gamson, Z. F. (1987). Seven principles for good practice in undergraduate education. AAHE Bull. 3:7.

Google Scholar

Crowley, K. (2000). Parent explanations during museum visits: gender differences in how children hear informal science. Visit. Stud. Today 3, 21–28.

Google Scholar

Crowley, K., Callanan, M., Jipson, J., Galco, J., Topping, K., and Shrager, J. (2001a). Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent-child activity. Sci. Educ. 85, 712–732. doi: 10.1002/sce.1035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., and Allen, E. (2001b). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychol. Sci. 12, 258–261. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, J., and Dierking, L. (2000). Learning from Museums: Visitor Experiences and the Making of Meaning. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.

Google Scholar

Farrant, K. (2000). A Tale of Autobiographical Memory Development: New Zealand Style. Doctoral Dissertation. Dunedin: University of Otago.

Google Scholar

Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., and Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 77, 1568–1588. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerdts, M. S., Van de Walle, G. A., and LoBue, V. (2015). Parent–child conversations about animals in informal learning environments. Visit. Stud. 18, 39–63. doi: 10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child Dev. Perspect. 4, 62–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00119.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haden, C. A., Jant, E. A., Hoffman, P., Marcus, M., Geddes, J. R., and Gaskins, S. (2014). Supporting family conversations and children’s STEM learning in a children’s museum. Early Childhood Res. Q. 29, 333–344. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. (1993). Towards a language-based theory of learning. Linguist. Educ. 5, 93–116. doi: 10.1016/0898-5898(93)90026-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heath, S. B. (1983). Research currents: a lot of talk about nothing. Lang. Arts 60, 999–1007.

Google Scholar

Hirsch-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., et al. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1071–1083.

Google Scholar

Jant, E. A., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., and Babcock, E. (2014). Conversation and object manipulation influence children’s learning in a museum. Child Dev. 85, 2029–2045. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joy, A., Law, F., McGuire, L., Mathews, C., Hartstone-Rose, A., Winterbottom, M., et al. (2021). ‘Understanding parents’ roles in children’s learning and engagement in informal science learning sites. Front. Psychol. 12:635839. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, K. R., Ocular, G., and Austin, A. (2020). Adult-child science language during informal science learning at an aquarium. Soc. Sci. J. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1080/03623319.2020.1727226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kisiel, J., Rowe, S., Ani Vartabedian, M., and Kopczak, C. (2012). Evidence for family engagement in scientific reasoning at interactive animal exhibits. Sci. Educ. 96, 1047–1070. doi: 10.1002/sce.21036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laible, D. J. (2004). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child’s past behavior at 30 months: links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill Palmer Q. 50, 159–180. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2004.0013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leichtman, M. D., Camilleri, K. A., Pillemer, D. B., Amato-Wierda, C. C., Hogan, J. E., and Dongo, M. D. (2017). Talking after school: parents’ conversational styles and children’s memory for a science lesson. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 156, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leinhardt, G., Crowley, K., and Knutson, K. (eds) (2003). Learning Conversations in Museums. Abingdon: Taylor & Francis. doi: 10.4324/9781410606624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Letourneau, S. M., Meisner, R., Neuwirth, J. L., and Sobel, D. M. (2017). What do caregivers notice and value about how children learn through play in a children’s museum? J. Museum Educ. 42, 87–98. doi: 10.1080/10598650.2016.1260436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyva, D., Reese, E., Laible, D., Schaughency, E., Das, S., and Clifford, A. (2020). ‘Measuring parents’ elaborative reminiscing: differential links of parents’ elaboration to children’s autobiographical memory and socioemotional skills. J. Cogn. Dev. 21, 23–45. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1668395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luke, J. J., Tomczuk, E. D., Foutz, S., Rivera, N., Brahms, L., Nelson, K., et al. (2019). What caregivers observe about their children’s learning during a visit to the children’s museum. J. Museum Educ. 44, 427–438. doi: 10.1080/10598650.2019.1672136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Mantzicopoulos, P., Patrick, H., and Samarapungavan, A. (2013). Science literacy in school and home contexts: kindergarteners’ science achievement and motivation. Cogn. Instr. 31, 62–119. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2012.742087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, M., Haden, C. A., and Uttal, D. H. (2017). STEM learning and transfer in a children’s museum and beyond. Merrill Palmer Q. 63, 155–180. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.63.2.0155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, M., Tõugu, P., Haden, C. A., and Uttal, D. H. (2021). Advancing opportunities for children’s informal STEM learning transfer through parent–child narrative reflection. Child Dev. 92, e1075–e1084. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathai, S. (2017). Exploring Hybrid Spaces Through an Informal Science Learning Programme. Sarjapura: Azim Premji University.

Google Scholar

McClure, E. R., Guernsey, L., Clements, D. H., Bales, S. N., Nichols, J., Kendall-Taylor, N., et al. (2017). STEM Starts Early: Grounding Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math Education in Early Childhood. New York, NY: The Joan Ganz Cooney Center at Sesame Workshop.

Google Scholar

Melzi, G., Schick, A. R., and Kennedy, J. L. (2011). Narrative elaboration and participation: two dimensions of maternal elicitation style. Child Dev. 82, 1282–1296. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01600.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyers, E. M., Erickson, I., and Small, R. V. (2013). Digital literacy and informal learning environments: an introduction. Learn. Media Technol. 38, 355–367. doi: 10.1080/17439884.2013.783597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., and Gonzalez, N. (1992). Funds of knowledge for teaching: using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms’. Theory Pract. 31, 132–141. doi: 10.1080/00405849209543534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, B. J., Zentall, S. R., Murray, G., and Owens, W. (2021). Enhancing informal stem learning through family engagement in cooking. Proc. Singapore Natl. Acad. Sci. 15, 119–133. doi: 10.1142/S2591722621400111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council (2007). Taking Science to School: Learning and Teaching Science in Grades K-8. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Google Scholar

Newcombe, R., and Reese, E. (2004). Evaluations and orientations in mother–child narratives as a function of attachment security: a longitudinal investigation. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28, 230–245. doi: 10.1080/01650250344000460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ornstein, P. A., Haden, C. A., and Hedrick, A. M. (2004). ‘Learning to remember: social-communicative exchanges and the development of children’s memory skills. Dev. Rev. 24, 374–395. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2004.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagano, L. C., Haden, C. A., and Uttal, D. H. (2020). Museum program design supports parent–child engineering talk during tinkering and reminiscing. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 200:104944. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagano, L. C., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., and Cohen, T. (2019). Conversational reflections about tinkering experiences in a children’s museum. Sci. Educ. 103, 1493–1512. doi: 10.1002/sce.21536

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and McCabe, A. (1992). Parental styles of narrative elicitation: effect on children’s narrative structure and content. First Lang. 12, 299–321.

Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Dev. Psychol. 30:937. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reese, E., and Brown, N. (2000). Reminiscing and recounting in the preschool years. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, 1–17. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200001)14:1<1::AID-ACP625>3.0.CO;2-G

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reese, E., Haden, C. A., and Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: relationships of style and memory over time. Cogn. Dev. 8, 403–430. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(05)80002-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reese, E., Haden, C. A., and Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers, fathers, daughters, sons: gender differences in autobiographical reminiscing. Res. Lang. Soc. Interact. 29, 27–56. doi: 10.1207/s15327973rlsi2901_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riojas-Cortez, M., Huerta, M. E., Flores, B. B., Perez, B., and Clark, E. R. (2008). Using cultural tools to develop scientific literacy of young Mexican American preschoolers. Early Child Dev. Care 178, 527–536. doi: 10.1080/03004430600851223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schleppegrell, M. J. (2004). The Language of Schooling: A Functional Linguistics Perspective. Abingdon: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781410610317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrell, B. (1997). Paying attention: the duration and allocation of visitors’ time in museum exhibitions. Curator Museum J. 40, 108–125.

Google Scholar

Shettel, H. H. (1997). Time—is it really of the essence? Curator Museum J. 40, 246–249. doi: 10.1111/j.2151-6952.1997.tb01308.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siemens, G. (2007). “Connectivism: creating a learning ecology in distributed environments,” in Didactics of Microlearning. Concepts, Discourses and Examples, ed. T. Hug (New York, NY: Waxman), 53–68.

Google Scholar

Strickler-Eppard, L., Czerniak, C. M., and Kaderavek, J. (2019). Families’ capacity to engage in science inquiry at home through structured activities. Early Childhood Educ. J. 47, 653–664. doi: 10.1007/s10643-019-00958-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenenbaum, H. R., Snow, C. E., Roach, K. A., and Kurland, B. (2005). Talking and reading science: longitudinal data on sex differences in mother–child conversations in low-income families. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 26, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Mental Process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., and Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. J. Child Psychiatry Psychol. 17, 89–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, H. T., and Bell, P. (2014). Where young people see science: everyday activities connected to science. Int. J. Sci. Educ. B 4, 25–53. doi: 10.1080/21548455.2012.741271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: informal STEM learning, parent elaboration, parent-child conversations, early childhood, funds of knowledge

Citation: Ocular G, Kelly KR, Millan L, Neves S, Avila K, Hsieh B and Maloles C (2022) Contributions of naturalistic parent-child conversations to children’s science learning during informal learning at an aquarium and at home. Front. Psychol. 13:943648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.943648

Received: 14 May 2022; Accepted: 01 July 2022;
Published: 08 August 2022.

Edited by:

Akira Takada, Kyoto University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Riikka Pauliina Svane, Aarhus University, Denmark
Shirley Wyver, Macquarie University, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Ocular, Kelly, Millan, Neves, Avila, Hsieh and Maloles. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Grace Ocular, b2N1bGFyZ3JhY2VAZ21haWwuY29t; Kimberly R. Kelly, S2ltYmVybHkuS2VsbHlAY3N1bGIuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.