- 1School of Physical Education Institute (Main Campus), Zhengzhou University, Zhengzhou, China
- 2Department of Sport Studies, Faculty of Education Studies, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Selangor, Malaysia
- 3Department of Foundation of Education, Faculty of Education Studies, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Seri Kembangan, Selangor, Malaysia
- 4Faculty of Business Management, City University Malaysia, Seri Kembangan, Selangor, Malaysia
Background: Due to causing inability of self-regulation (ego depletion) and executive functions such as directed attention and visual searching for relevant information (e.g., the ball location and the position of teammates), mental fatigue impairs skilled performance in various sports. On the other hand, natural scenes could improve directed attention, which may considerably benefit visual searching ability and self-regulation. However, nature exposure as a potential intervention to improve skilled performance among mentally fatigued athletes has not been discussed thoroughly.
Purpose: To propose the potential intervention for the impairment of skilled performance among mentally fatigued athletes and generate a framework for future studies.
Methods: A narrative review was applied to search broadly across disciplines, retrieving literature from several databases (PubMed, Web of Science, Scopus, and EBSCOhost).
Results: Thirty-three works of literature including 39 experiments (mental fatigue 15; ego depletion 5; and nature exposure 19) were obtained. Finally, a conceptual framework was built regarding the effect of nature exposure intervention on skilled performance in athletes for future studies.
Conclusion: Three theories (the psychobiological model of exercise performance, the strength model of self-regulation, and attention restoration theory) could be potentially integrated to be a conceptual framework and explain the mechanism of preventing prior mental exertion (e.g., mental fatigue and ego depletion). Future studies could examine more on the duration of the intervention.
Introduction
Mental fatigue is a psychobiological condition resulting from extended, taxing cognitive activity (Hancock and Desmond, 2001), characteristically found among athletes in the matches. For a long time, the effect of mental fatigue on athletes’ performance only existed as an anecdote. For instance, Manchester team manager Roberto Mancini, in an interview with Eurosport TV channel, talked about worries that his side could fail to obtain silverware due to mental fatigue (Marcora, 2014).
Marcora (2014) found that mental fatigue decreased by 15% of the running distance in a Yo-Yo soccer-specific endurance test. The negative evidence came out strongly among athletes (e.g., soccer). The results were well proven by Grgic et al. (2022). Grgic et al. (2022) pooled the effect from nine studies and showed a negative effect of mental fatigue on the Yo-Yo test (Cohen’s d: −0.49; p < 0.001). Later on, numerous studies found that the reductions did not only occur in specific endurance performances but also in technique and decision-making (Badin et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2017; Fortes et al., 2019, 2020; Gantois et al., 2020). However, the intervention that can improve skilled performance (technical and decision-making skills) in mentally fatigued athletes remains limited. Notably, technical and decision-making skills play a key role in matches and they have been defined as skilled performance in sports, which is the capacity to perform at a high standard efficiently (Koopmann et al., 2020). In sports, It is also referred to as technical performance or skilled execution (Rampinini et al., 2007). The most recent review confirmed the adverse effect of mental fatigue on skilled performance (Sun et al., 2021), which could determine the outcome or result in sports (Allard and Burnett, 1985).
Rating perception of effort (RPE) plays a key role in endurance-based performance (Marcora et al., 2009; Van Cutsem et al., 2017). In contrast, it may not mediate the effect of mental fatigue on skilled performance. Recently, Habay et al. (2021) summarized all 21 literature investigated the sport-specific psychomotor performance and found that only two studies reported higher RPE (Badin et al., 2016; Veness et al., 2017). Unfortunately, these two studies (Badin et al., 2016; Veness et al., 2017) also involved endurance tests (e.g., walking and repeat sprints). Consistently, Soylu et al. (2021) examined all studies about the psychobiological response in soccer athletes. They found that only one study showed higher RPE in athletes after Yo-Yo IR1 test, rather than other skilled performance (e.g., technical and decision-making skills).
Notably, the execution of skilled performance is highly associated with athletes’ capacity for effortful attention (e.g., directed attention; Kaplan and Berman, 2010). That is, athletes in team sports (e.g., soccer) should perform reciprocally and sequentially, meaning that a phase of play in matches involves frequent moments of decision-making skills (Araújo et al., 2006; Bennett et al., 2019). Thus, the capacity to react fast (e.g., defending in a split-second) and accurately (e.g., passing the ball to a targeted teammate) is crucial among players. It requires players continually maintain concentration to perceive relevant information in the complex and rapidly changing environment (Afonso et al., 2012).
On the other hand, prolonged demand for a cognitive task that activates the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which in turn may cause feelings of mental fatigue due to reduced transmission of dopamine (MacDonald Angus et al., 2000; Lorist et al., 2005). Many studies argued that the impairment of skilled performance (e.g., technical and decision-making skills) is attributed to the reduction in transmission of dopamine, for it negatively influences executive function (e.g., attention) (Smith et al., 2018; McMorris, 2020; Sun et al., 2021). Consequently, mentally fatigued athletes cannot fully maintain directed attention to ignore irrelevant and distracting stimuli, and cannot think one step ahead and find the targeted teammate accurately, which is shown as an impairment in skilled performance. Thus, it is important to know how to counter these impairments, for athletes are bound to be exposed to mental fatigue during actual match (Smith et al., 2018; Russell et al., 2019). In a most recent study, Proost et al. (2022) confirmed the underlying mechanism with countermeasures. Authors argued that to combat mental fatigue, a modulator (e.g., music and caffeine) for the pathway of dopamine and adenosine is necessary.
Considering athletic outcomes, self-regulation is the ability to adjust physiological and psychological states adaptively to a specific context (e.g., soccer) (Baumeister and Vohs, 2003; Nigg, 2017). The depletion condition of self-regulation called ego depletion also impairs the subsequent physical and cognitive performance in sports (Englert and Bertrams, 2012; Furley et al., 2013). Players in team sports (e.g., soccer, basketball, Australian football, etc.) should exert self-regulation to continually maintain concentration to perceive relevant information in the complex and rapidly changing environment (Englert and Bertrams, 2012; Furley et al., 2013). However, it is perceived as effortful to self-regulate and consistently leads to symptoms of mental fatigue (Inzlicht et al., 2015). Subsequently, a reduction in skilled performance appeared among athletes. Notably, training self-regulation may provide another way to tackle mental fatigue, as it will consume fewer internal resources (Proost et al., 2022).
Moreover, some studies highly suggested evaluating mental fatigue and ego depletion together, for they are induced by common cognitive tasks (e.g., Stroop task, flanker task, AX-CPT) involving same executive function (e.g., inhibition), and both conditions are involved to the brain region of ACC (Giboin and Wolff, 2019; Brown et al., 2020). Furthermore, Brown et al. (2020) argued that separating the investigation of mental fatigue and ego depletion is the “biggest limitation.” This suggestion is also corroborated by a recent study by Graham and Brown (2021). The authors provided an in-depth overview of the effect of prior mental exertion (mental fatigue and ego depletion) on self-regulation of subsequent sports performance. Thus, the current review links these two independent research fields (mental fatigue and ego depletion) and tries to evaluate deep insight from theories, providing suggestions for future studies.
Nature exposure is a “direct physical or sensory contact with the natural environment” (Kamitsis and Francis, 2013). The relevant intervention could be a type of psychological therapy without any physical exercise and could be a technique (visualization or imagery training) to make people immerse in the natural settings (e.g., scenes) (Sun et al., 2022a). Many studies show that individuals can benefit in various ways from being exposed to nature, such as an enhancement in well-being (Grinde and Patil, 2009; Howell et al., 2011) and release of pressure (Pilotti et al., 2015). Most impor tantly, evidence demonstrates that nature exposure could restore directed attention (e.g., Berto, 2005; Beute and de Kort, 2014; Chow and Lau, 2015; Li and Sullivan, 2016), which may make mentally fatigued athletes concentrate back on the restoration of directed attention to retrieve relevant information and block out irrelevant stimuli (e.g., worrisome, cluster) in the competitive setting. Subsequently, the execution of skilled performance may be improved.
Furthermore, this benefit from nature scenes is undeniable and always happens, for humans are always inclined to affiliate with nature, which is called biophilia, supported by several substantial investigations (Joye, 2007; Howell et al., 2011; Berto et al., 2018). Also, nature exposure might enhance dopamine transportation in ACC (Darna et al., 2015), which could lead to restored directed attention and self-regulatory ability (Kaplan and Berman, 2010).
Sun et al. (2022a) did a promising study and examined the nature exposure intervention and showed it could considerably improve decision-making skills among mentally fatigued soccer players. Moreover, the best duration of the intervention was examined as 12.50 min, when the nature stimuli (e.g., nature scenes) were fixed. However, there is no conceptual framework to guide and show clear picture about why there was an improvement. More to the point, directed attention plays a key role in skilled performance, and it could be restored with nature stimuli (e.g., scenes) in a mentally fatigued or ego depletion population. Therefore, a new model or a conceptual framework is needed to comprehensively connect these factors and give suggestions to future studies.
Since a narrative review could be an important scholarly tool to support theoretical explanations and discussions and unlike a systematic review, it enables searching more broadly across disciplines (Green et al., 2006; Jahan et al., 2016), the current study is a conceptual analysis based on a narrative literature review in three academic fields (mental fatigue, ego depletion, and nature exposure academic fields). It aims to build a conceptual framework that might significantly counter mental fatigue and improve the subsequent skilled performance. Figure 1 shows the problem statement identified in the current review.
Methodology
Following the methodological framework to write a narrative review (Green et al., 2006), we conducted the current review with four steps; they are: identifying sources, defining the searching strategy and setting parameters, defining selection criteria, and summarizing and synthesizing results.
Identifying sources
A literature search was conducted employing several databases, including PubMed, Web of Science, Scopus, and EBSCOhost (CENTRAL, SPORTDicus) from the inception of databases to March 2022. Moreover, we also manually searched Google Scholar for gray literature that could have evaded our search parameters in these databases due to different indexing.
Defining the searching strategy
To comprehensive boundaries for the literature, two series of search terms were used: (i) “mental fatigue” OR “mental exertion” OR “cognitive fatigue” OR “cognitive exertion” OR “mental exhaustion” OR “mental tiredness” OR “ego depletion” AND “athletic performance” OR “technical skill*” OR “skill*” OR “technique” OR “decision making” OR “performance” AND sport*; and (ii) “mental fatigue” OR “mental exertion” OR “cognitive fatigue” OR “cognitive exertion” OR “mental exhaustion” OR “mental tiredness” OR “ego depletion” AND Nature*. The results of the searching show in Supplementary Tables 1, 2.
Defining selection criteria
To identify the relevant literature, several steps were done separately by two series of search terms (Supplementary Figure 1); they are (i) yielded a total number of the literature searching in databases and Google Scholar; (ii) using endnote and removing duplicates; (iii) title and abstract screening to identify whether relate to ‘skilled performance’; or ‘nature exposure’ in mental fatigue or ego depletion academic field; (iv) full-text review proceeded. Moreover, any investigation conducted without prior mental exertion task to induce fatigue and with unhealthy subjects was excluded. Two reviewers independently evaluated the inclusion of these works. Any disagreement was solved by consensus or a third reviewer discussion.
Summary and synthesis
Excel was used to design a data extraction sheet to compile a summary of the research topic based on the included literature. And synthesized the purpose of each study, methods or material utilized, variables investigated, any findings related to ‘skilled performance’ and ‘nature exposure,’ and comments or note from authors. The relevant findings finally were presented in the following section.
Results
Thirty-three works of literature including thirty-seven experiments (mental fatigue 15; ego depletion 5; and nature exposure 19) were obtained. The details showed in Tables 1–3, respectively.
Table 1. Summary studies of knowledge about the effect of mental fatigue on skilled performance in sports.
Table 2. Summary studies of knowledge about the effect of ego depletion on skilled performance in sports.
The effect of mental fatigue on skilled performance
As shown in Table 1, mental fatigue negatively influences skilled performance, including technical and decision-making skills in several sports (soccer, basketball, table tennis, boxing, and Australian football).
It is worthy to note that among the prominent theories of mental fatigue is the psychobiological model of exercise performance that was proposed by Marcora (2008). The model is based on motivational intensity theory (Brehm and Self, 1989), and provides an explanation for the adverse effects of mental fatigue on physical performance (reduced exhaustion time and output of self-selected strength). The model further highlighted two factors: motivation and RPE that determined the consciously regulated behavior. The model has also been applied to soccer, however, the same as the original model merely related to the physical domain, intermittent endurance showed impairment due to elevated RPE (Smith et al., 2015). Furthermore, Martin and colleagues indicated that mental fatigue alters the concentration of dopamine in the ACC and that raising exercise motivation or decreasing the RPE could counter this negative alteration (Martin et al., 2018; Figure 2).
Figure 2. The representation of the psychobiological model. The representation of the model is from the study of Martin et al. (2018).
However, the current review (Table 1) found inconsistent evidence, as there are only two studies that showed the level of RPE was significantly different in the mental fatigue group (Badin et al., 2016; Fortes et al., 2021a). That is, the reductions of skilled performance could not be attributed to RPE, including technical and decision-making skill.
Notably, Moreira et al. (2018) did a well-done study and examined the effect of mental fatigue on basketball performance. According to their findings, mental fatigue increases levels of salivary testosterone, which could explain why athletes make more mistakes in technical skills. To be sure, the dopamine-containing mesolimbic reward system is believed to be regulated by testosterone (Robbins and Everitt, 1996). Dopamine transmission may be affected by lower testosterone levels in mentally fatigued athletes, according to Moreira and colleagues. Mental fatigue reduces dopamine levels in the ACC, which impairs executive functions, as has been shown in previous studies (Smith et al., 2018; Sun et al., 2021).
Impairment of executive functions, such as attentional direction, has been linked to technical skill impairment (Badin et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2016a,2017). As a result of mental fatigue, attention can be diverted from goal-directed to irrelevant stimuli-directed processing (Boksem et al., 2005; Csathó et al., 2012). Furthermore, mental fatigue also diminished the ability to prepare to control the ball movement (Smith et al., 2017). The scenario could be more likely found in actual match due to the additional distracted stimuli (Smith et al., 2016a).
The effect of ego depletion on skilled performance
When considering athletic outcomes, self-regulation is the capacity to adjust physiological and psychological states adaptively to the specific context (e.g., soccer) (Baumeister and Vohs, 2003; Nigg, 2017). Athletes must also be able to voluntarily decrease anxiety and maintain attention (Englert and Bertrams, 2012); in other words they must be capable of self-regulation (Baumeister and Vohs, 2003; Nigg, 2017). Consistently trying to self-regulate has shown to increase the risk of future self-regulation failure. Ego depletion refers to this psychological state (Baumeister et al., 2000).
Table 2 shows that ego depletion impairs skilled performance among athletes in the sports of basketball, tennis, and dart throwing. Importantly, these impairments have been attributed to the reduction of an executive function, which is directed attention (Englert and Bertrams, 2012; Furley et al., 2013; Englert et al., 2015b; Shaabani et al., 2020). For example, when making a tactical decision in team sports (e.g., basketball) while the opposing audience is calling an apparent rule violation, athletes should exert directed attention to block out irrelevant distractions and only concentrate on the relevant information. Also, dart athletes need to make longer fixations on the target and simultaneously suppress attention to task-irrelevant stimuli. Because all self-regulation processes use the same finite amount of brain resources, as proposed by the strength model (Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996; Baumeister et al., 1998), applying self-control throughout a cognitive task such as transcription task would have a negative impact on the ability to perform skilled task (Table 2) that unrelated self-regulation demanding. Therefore, to counter this negative effect, it is necessary to increase the athletes’ ability of attention to retrieval of relevant information.
Consistently, the current review confirmed the suggestion for the combined investigation of mental fatigue and ego depletion (Giboin and Wolff, 2019; Brown et al., 2020), for two fields usually recruited the same cognitive function to induce the certain condition. Table 2 shows the most common cognitive task which was used to induce the condition of ego depletion is 6 min transcription task. In the depletion group, athletes were usually asked to omit the letters “e” and “n.” For successful task completion, inhibition is inevitable to be exerted as one has to volitionally override one’s writing habits (Schmeichel, 2007). Similarity, inhibition is typically assessed with versions of the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935) in which participants have to override or inhibit a prepotent response to the different color of words used frequently to induce mental fatigue conditions (see Table 1).
The effect of nature exposure on prior mental exertion
According to the results in Table 3, many studies showed nature exposure intervention restored directed attention after a particular mental exertion task. For example, Berto (2005) recruited 5 min Sustained Attention to Response Test to induce mental exertion. The result showed after 6.25 min nature exposure, the directed attention measured as reaction time and correct response were significantly improved. Consistently, Chow and Lau (2015) employed transcription, perception, and retyping tasks as the prior mental exertion. The result showed that nature exposure intervention improved directed attention dramatically measured in anagram and logical reasoning tasks.
Notably, Table 2 also shows some other studies did not improve directed attention significantly after the intervention. Probably because the ‘dosage’ or durations of nature exposure intervention used in all of these studies are different. Specifically, Laumann et al. (2003) employed a 20-min nature exposure and found significant results after the 15-min mental exertion exercise done by its participants, whereas Bennett (2019) used a 4-min intervention to counteract the effects of a 15-min mental exertion, but found no improvement. In addition to those studies, Beute and de Kort (2014) found that 3 min of nature exposure significantly increased cognitive performance after performing a typing task. In contrast, they did not find any improvement after adding a 4-min Stroop task to their prior mental exertion task with the same nature exposure intervention time. This ‘dosage’ or duration issue also was raised by a previous systematic review (see Stevenson et al., 2018).
Besides the duration, the type is also crucial for future studies to implement the intervention. Berman et al. (2008) particularly investigated two types (actual and virtual scenes) of nature exposure. In the first study, the authors recruited the actual setting and did a 50–55 min intervention. The result showed after 50–55 min walking in this actual nature environment, there was a significant restoration in directed attention. The improvement was also detected, when participants were exposed to the virtual scenes including some particular nature stimuli shown on the computer (see the detail of virtual nature in the following section: The Generation of a Conceptual Framework); however, the duration of the intervention is much shorter than actual setting (5.83 min vs. 50–55 min; Table 3). In fact, it is particularly difficult to maintain ecological validity in actual environments. To contrast, the ecological validity could be reconciled very well in virtual nature (e.g., virtual reality) with experimental control. Research into how to make a person feel more comfortable and fully immersed in a virtual environment has been extensive (Stone, 2008). The realism of a setting could be considered a loose analogy for the concept of immersion in this context (Brown and Cairns, 2004). Biophilia of humans is the key and responsible for positive cognitive responses, restoring directed attention capacities in nature (Joye, 2007; Howell et al., 2011; Berto et al., 2018). Across studies, it was typical for actual exposure to be longer than virtual exposures (Berman et al., 2008; Chow and Lau, 2015; Zhang et al., 2017; Neilson et al., 2020). Since people decreased outdoor activities with the COVID-19 situation (Freeman and Eykelbosh, 2020), virtual exposure could be considered more in future studies.
Theories rational
The conceptual framework developed in this study was based on three theories, which are the most prestigious ones in mental fatigue, ego depletion, and nature exposure reach field, respectively; they are The pychobiological model of exercise performance (Marcora, 2008, 2009), The strength model of self-regulation (Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996), and attention restoration theory (ART) (Kaplan and Berman, 2010).
The psychobiological model of exercise performance
The original psychobiological model of exercise performance mainly indicates the reason for physical performance is impaired by mental fatigue and has two factors, which shows in the dotted line of Figure 3 shown in the study of Martin et al. (2018).
Figure 3. The potential update of the mechanism for psychobiological model of exercise performance. The schematic representation of the mechanism for the model (inside the dotted line) is from Martin et al. (2018).
Inconsistent with the model, only Badin et al. (2016) and Fortes et al. (2021a) reported a significantly different RPE between the experimental and control group (see Table 1: outcome). Other studies did not detect any difference between mental fatigue and the control group regarding RPE and motivation level (e.g., Smith et al., 2016a; Filipas et al., 2021b). Therefore, an updated model is required to apply the model to the domain of skilled performance.
Notably, the skilled performance was significantly impaired by a reduction in executive function, which may have been caused by mental fatigue tasks activating the ACC. In turn, it may have resulted in elevated adenosine and decreased dopamine level (Smith et al., 2018; Fortes et al., 2020). Especially, attention plays a key role in this impairment in terms of reduction of executive function.
Moreover, the shift in attention focus impairs athletes’ decision-making. Decision-making in soccer is heavily reliant on the human brain’s ability to perceive relevant information from a complex environment while filtering out irrelevant distractions (Baker et al., 2003; Gantois et al., 2020). Selective attention governs this process (Kastner et al., 1999; Murray and Wojciulik, 2004). Selectivity allows athletes to make flexible decisions by preventing them from reacting reflexively to environmental stimuli. However, it was hampered by mental fatigue (Smith et al., 2016b; Fortes et al., 2019, 2021b; Gantois et al., 2020). Directed attention refers to the ability to focus attention selectively and sustainably (Cohen, 2018).
Although the actions of dopamine in the brain are complex, research shows that the natural environment enhances dopamine transportation (Darna et al., 2015), restoring directed attention and improving the ability of self-regulation (Francis and Inzlicht, 2016). Also, because directed attention and executive function are reciprocal (Karyotaki et al., 2017), a third alternative is that replenishment of the capacity of directed attention through restorative environments may lead to reduced performance declines as mental fatigue is minimized or prevented. Consequently, the psychobiological model may be expanded (see Figure 3, outside the dotted line).
The strength model of self-regulation
There are two gist in the strength model of self-regulation: (i) global and (ii) finite.
(i) Global
The model has proposed that self-regulation is universal global strength that enables people to perform tasks (Hagger et al., 2010). Baumeister and Vohs (2003) emphasized that self-regulation is a common resource for responding to diverse tasks and functions. A body of research exemplified this by showing how one form of response impairs the subsequent control over other various forms of responses. Even the common prefrontal region in the brain is activated for different types of tasks that each require resources. Researchers further concluded that any act of self-control should tax one’s strength, and that afterward, one is less ability of self-regulation. A fluctuating capacity in any subject means that self-regulation is costly in short term, which would underpin individual variation in performances requiring self-regulation.
(ii) Finite
Another major tenet of this theory is that the capacity of self-regulation is a finite “reservoir.” The resource is recognized as limited, to be depleted over time. Originally, Baumeister and Heatherton (1996) and Baumeister et al. (1998) indicated that the strength is the same as that of the muscle, which requires energy to perform and becomes fatigued over a period of consecutive exertion. This means that the exertion of self-regulation can only be maintained for a limited period, and the resource is prone to depletion over time. Similarly, it can also be recuperated after a period of rest.
Therefore, after the first cognitive task (e.g., Stroop task), the self-regulation resources were depleted, induced condition ego depletion or mental fatigue, and subsequent performance was impaired.
Attention restoration theory
Attention restoration theory comes from the research field of environmental psychology. It proposes that nature exposure can restore directed attention. Specifically, James (1892, as cited in Kaplan and Berman, 2010) suggested that voluntary effort to resist temptation and behave oneself is typical of self-regulation and the executive function, due to the centrality of attention in both. As he briefly defined it, “voluntary effort is the effort of attention” (James, 1892, p. 317, as cited in Kaplan and Berman, 2010). James further believed that the nebulous concept of resources and effort is made up of more concrete processes (e.g., attention), which can help us to define more precisely the finite resource.
In addition, James (1892, as cited in Kaplan and Berman, 2010) identified two forms of attention, differentiated by the amount of effort it takes to use each one. when something interesting or exciting happens, such as the appearance of a wild animal, it is so-called involuntary attention. Another form of attention was elicited by these interesting stimuli, which could be interpreted as an act of “seeking to discover what is going on.” For example, a color snake in the wild is witnessed by a young child who has never seen it before. Such a scene may cause everything else in the world to disappear (Larson and Memtt, 1991). Generally, environmental features capture this attention. However, it needs to be sufficiently gentle to be far away from other interferences and is referred to as ‘soft fascination’ according to ART, which largely exists in the natural environments (Kaplan and Berman, 2010).
On the other hand, the counterpart of attention, the so-called voluntary attention or directed attention requires a lot more effort (Morecraft et al., 2015), forces oneself to focus on something uninteresting or tedious. A study on the mechanism found that voluntary attention is driven more by the prefrontal cortex neurons (Buschman and Miller, 2007), which is the same for self-regulation (Baumeister and Vohs, 2004), whereas involuntary attention is driven more by other parts of the brain.
Based on James’ identification of attention, Kaplan, 1995, 2001 proposed ART and suggested that voluntary attention is effortful and can be tiring, whereas involuntary attention is effortless and helps the attention system to rest and restore. Kaplan and Berman (2010) further argued that directed attention shares a common resource with self-regulation. The hypothesis was tested by some studies. For example, Naghavi et al. (2018) demonstrated that cognitive training could improve the capacity of self-regulation measured as psychophysiological indicators (e.g., HRV and skin conductance). Similarly, the attentional control theory and the strength model of self-regulation were integrated by Englert and Bertrams (2015). The authors indicated that under high levels of anxiety, attention regulation is more susceptible to distraction. Players could focus on the relevant target while blocking out distracting stimuli. Therefore, they could maintain their sports performance.
To summarize, there are potential connections among these three theories (the psychobiological model of exercise performance, the strength model of self-regulation, and attention restoration theory). It may be fruitful to integrate them and provides a guide to future studies to improve the skilled performance among mentally fatigued sports players. However, a conceptual framework is needed.
The generation of a conceptual framework
According to an in-depth literature review, a conceptual framework was generated as shown in Figure 4 with three theories regarding the nature exposure intervention.
Specifically, the strength model of self-regulation indicates that reduced performance in the second cognitive task (e.g., skilled performance) appears after the first prolonged cognitive task since the self-regulation resources are limited. The condition of depleted resources is ego depletion or mental fatigue.
Moreover, ART indicates that nature exposure effectively attracts involuntary attention, leading to restoring voluntary attention (directed attention). This process is relevant to the increase of dopamine in ACC (Darna et al., 2015). The mentally fatigued players may have better self-regulation ability, and subsequent decision-making may be also improved after nature exposure intervention since directed attention is overlapped with self-regulation (Kaplan and Berman, 2010).
Directed attention may be the third factor to prevent reduced performance declines as mental fatigue is minimized, for directed attention and executive functions are reciprocal (Karyotaki et al., 2017), and dopamine transportation is enhanced (Darna et al., 2015) in the Psychobiological Model of Exercise Performance.
Discussion
The current review built a conceptual framework through a thorough literature review and gave suggestions for future studies. Eventually, the review associated nature exposure with skilled performance and showed potential integration according to three theories.
This framework further helps athletes and coaches to understand the mechanism of preventing prior mental exertion (e.g., mental fatigue and ego depletion) based on some variables, such as the duration of nature exposure, self-regulation and directed attention shown in Figure 4.
Notably, nature exposure intervention should be implemented based on the restorative environment, which is full of “soft fascination” (Kaplan and Berman, 2010). This soft fascination is essential, for it softly attracts involuntary attention while at the same time limiting the need for directed attention. It is suitable to implement this intervention virtually, rather than in the real environment. There are mainly two reasons: (i) Stevenson et al. (2018) argued that to achieve the restorativeness, exposure to a virtual environment can be shorter than a real environment, probably because it is too difficult to control distractions (e.g., artificial scenes or sound) in the real environment; (ii) People greatly decrease the outdoor exposure time since COVID-19 outburst (Pancani et al., 2021; Wright et al., 2021).
However, compared to natural stimuli, such as images, sound, though a part of a natural setting, has fewer positive effects (Haga et al., 2016). Thus, to make people fully immersed, future studies should fully consider the criteria selection of nature scenes. Nature scenes, according to Balling and Falk (1982), are those in which there are no artifacts, but there is no evidence of human management, as in national forests or national parks. Hence, the domain of nature is by no means limited only to the wilderness (Ulrich, 1983). From a broad perspective, Ulrich (1983) and Han (2003) indicated that nature scenes should be fulfilling three conditions: (a) the dominant contours are curvilinear or irregular rather than rectilinear or regular; (b) artificial features are absent or concealed; and (c) the presented landscape is dominated by vegetation, water, and mountains. However, previous studies did not pay attention to individual differences in immersion (e.g., Li and Sullivan, 2016; Lanki et al., 2017; Hicks et al., 2020), which is mentioned by Berto (2020). It may lead to different results in studies that used the same experimental protocol (Berto, 2005; Neilson et al., 2020).
Skilled performance highly relies on cognitive functions (Scharfen and Memmert, 2019), such as inhibition, working memory, flexibility, reasoning, and planning. For example, players must process information (e.g., working memory) to find the best solution with a time constraint (e.g., planning, reasoning, and creativity) to make a decision (Sakamoto et al., 2018). This process demands directed attention to block out irrelevant distractions and only focus on the important information (e.g., the movements of opponents and teammates). Since nature scenes could restore directed attention, there is an association between nature exposure and skilled performance shown in the conceptual framework.
Therefore, to apply this framework, future studies may consider about the following hypotheses: (i) nature exposure intervention could significantly improve skilled performance (technical and decision-making skills) in mentally fatigued athletes; (ii) the dosage or duration of the intervention plays a key role when apply the intervention. That is, longer and shorter duration may have different effects to influence skilled performance in mentally fatigued athletes, when nature stimuli (e.g., scenes) fixed. (iii) Voluntary attention (directed attention) may benefit different orders (high: working memory and flexibility vs. low: inhibition) of self-regulation in mentally fatigued athletes.
It is worthy to note that although the resource model is the best-known model of self-regulation so far, it may be manipulated to counter prior mental exertion (e.g., mental fatigue and ego depletion) (Sun et al., 2022b). However, it has met many challenges in the recent decade (Inzlicht and Schmeichel, 2012; Inzlicht et al., 2014). For example, glucose hypothesis. A study by Gailliot et al. (2007) showed that blood glucose could be the metaphorical resource. Despite its obvious appeal, this hypothesis was never test successfully (Finley et al., 2019).
In contrast the process self-regulation model recognized some shifted centres (e.g., motivation) of self-regulation and ignored the metaphorical resource (Inzlicht and Schmeichel, 2012; Inzlicht et al., 2014). However, Baumeister and Vohs (2003); Baumeister and Vohs (2007) updated the Strength Model and acknowledged the remedied motivation as an variable. Specifically, the authors contented that the monitoring process may compensate for relatively low self-regulatory resources through if individuals have high motivation to need standards.
Limitations and future studies
It is important to highlight the limitations of the current review. Firstly, other factors that may affect cognitive performance were not discussed, such as motivation. Thus, the future studies may it to be a covariate, for motivation could determine the maximal level of effort and utilization of self-regulatory resources to perform to succeed in some tasks such as the Stroop task (Soutschek et al., 2014), and the subsequent skilled performance (Barte et al., 2018).
Secondly, the level of proficiency of athletes (elite, professional or amateur) was not discussed. However, Martin et al. (2018) indicated that professional athletes may have a better capacity of self-regulation than their recreational counterparts. Moreover, different types of athletes have their own traits in the execution of skilled performance such as open or close skills, thus they may have different responses regarding mental fatigue (Coyne et al., 2021). Future studies could consider the level of athletes and different types of sports when applying the conceptual framework.
Finally, the specific or practical duration was not discussed. Sun et al. (2022a) showed that 12.50 min may be the optimal choice for nature exposure. However, some studies showed that long time (e.g., 30 min) screen exposure could induce fatigue (Fortes et al., 2021a,b). That is, players may be fatigued again, if natural stimuli (e.g., scene) are presented for too long time on the screen, the relationship being inverted U-shaped. Future studies may examine more on the duration to find the cut-off point.
Conclusion
Three theories (the psychobiological model of exercise performance, the strength model of self-regulation and attention restoration theory) could be integrated to generate a conceptual framework. The framework provided an overall picture of the counteractive effect of nature exposure to improve skilled performance, including technical and decision-making skills in mentally fatigued athletes. The improvement could be from the restoration of directed attention and self-regulation. However, some criteria should be paid attention to, such as the development of stimuli (e.g., nature scenes) and the duration of the intervention, and different traits of each individual (e.g., immersive tendency).
Author contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Funding
This research was funded by the National Social Science Fund of China (Grant No. 21ATY009).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.941299/full#supplementary-material
References
Afonso, J., Garganta, J., and Mesquita, I. (2012). Decision-making in sports: the role of attention, anticipation and memory. Brazilian J. Kinanthr. Hum. Perform. 14, 592–601. doi: 10.5007/1980-0037.2012v14n5p592
Allard, F., and Burnett, N. (1985). Skill in sport. Can. J. Psychol. Rev. Can. Psychol. 39, 294–312. doi: 10.1037/h0080063
Araújo, D., Davids, K., and Hristovski, R. (2006). The ecological dynamics of decision making in sport:an ecological dynamics perspective. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 7, 653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.07.002
Badin, O. O., Smith, M. R., Conte, D., and Coutts, A. J. (2016). Mental fatigue: impairment of technical performance in small-sided soccer games. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 11, 1100–1105. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2015-0710
Baker, J., Côté, J., and Abernethy, B. (2003). Sport specific training, deliberate practice and the development of expertise in team ball sports. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 15, 12–25. doi: 10.1080/10413200390180035
Balling, J. D., and Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preference for natural environments. Environ. Behav. 14, 5–28. doi: 10.1177/0013916582141001
Barte, J. C. M., Nieuwenhuysa, A., Geurtsa, S. A. E., and Kompier, M. A. J. (2018). Motivation counteracts fatigue-induced performance decrements in soccer passing performance. J. Sports Sci. 37, 1189–1196. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2018.1548919
Baumeister, R. F., and Vohs, K. D. (2003). “Willpower, choice, and self-control,” in Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice, eds G. Loewenstein, D. Read, and R. Baumeister (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 201–216.
Baumeister, R. F., and Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol. Inquiry 7, 1–15.
Baumeister, R. F., and Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and motivation. Soc. Personali. Psychol. Compass 1, 115–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00001.x
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 74, 1252–1265. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion: a resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Soc. Cogn. 18, 130–150. doi: 10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.130
Bennett, K. J. M., Novak, A. R., Pluss, M. A., Coutts, A. J., and Fransen, J. (2019). Assessing the validity of a video-based decision-making assessment for talent identification in youth soccer. J. Sci. Med. Sport 22, 729–734. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.12.011
Bennett, M. (2019). The effect of sound on attention restoration. Brockport, NY: The College at Brockport.
Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., and Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x
Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 249–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001
Berto, R. (2020). It is undeniable that nature has to be restorative to restore attention, otherwise the effect is unreliable. J. Environ. Psychol. 71, 101495–101495. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101495
Berto, R., Barbiero, G., Barbiero, P., and Senes, G. (2018). 无CP和PP an individual’s connection to nature can affect perceived restorativeness of natural environments. some observations about biophilia. Behav. Sci. 8:34. doi: 10.3390/bs8030034
Beute, F., and de Kort, Y. A. W. (2014). Natural resistance: exposure to nature and self-regulation, mood, and physiology after ego-depletion. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.06.004
Bian, C., Ali, A., Nassis, G. P., and Li, Y. (2022). Repeated interval loughborough soccer passing tests: an ecologically valid motor task to induce mental fatigue in soccer. Front. Physiol. 12:803528. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.803528
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
Brehm, J. W., and Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 40, 109–131. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545
Brown, D., Graham, J. D., Innes, K. I., Harris, S., Flemington, A., and Bray, S. R. (2020). Effects of prior cognitive exertion on physical performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 50, 497–529. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01204-8
Brown, E., and Cairns, P. (2004). “A grounded investigation of game immersion,” in Proceedings of the CHI’04 extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems (Vienna: ACM), 1297–1300. doi: 10.1145/985921.986048
Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science (New York, N.Y.) 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071
Chow, J. T., and Lau, S. (2015). Nature gives us strength: exposure to nature counteracts ego-depletion. J. Soc. Psychol. 155, 70–85. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2014.972310
Cohen, R. (2018). Directed attention. Encycl. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2018, 56782–56782. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2
Coyne, J. O. C., Coutts, A. J., Newton, R. U., and Haff, G. G. (2021). The influence of mental fatigue on sessional ratings of perceived exertion in elite open and closed skill sports athletes. J. Strength Cond. Res. 35, 963–969. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000003980
Csathó, Á, Van Der Linden, D., Hernádi, I., Buzás, P., and Kalmár, G. (2012). Effects of mental fatigue on the capacity limits of visual attention. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 511–524. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.658039
Darna, M., Beckmann, J. S., Gipson, C. D., Bardo, M. T., and Dwoskin, L. P. (2015). Effect of environmental enrichment on dopamine and serotonin transporters and glutamate neurotransmission in medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Res. 1599, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.12.034
Emfield, A. G., and Neider, M. B. (2014). Evaluating visual and auditory contributions to the cognitive restoration effect. Front. Psychol. 5:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00548
Englert, C., and Bertrams, A. (2012). Anxiety, ego depletion, and sports performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 580–599. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.5.580
Englert, C., and Bertrams, A. (2015). Integrating attentional control theory and the strength model of self-control [mini review]. Front. Psychol. 6:824. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00824
Englert, C., Bertrams, A., Furley, P., and Oudejans, R. R. D. (2015a). Is ego depletion associated with increased distractibility? Results from a basketball free throw task. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 18, 26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.12.001
Englert, C., Zwemmer, K., Bertrams, A., and Oudejans, R. R. D. (2015b). Ego depletion and attention regulation under pressure: is a temporary loss of self-control strength indeed related to impaired attention regulation? J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 37, 127–137. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2014-0219
Evensen, K. H., Raanaas, R. K., Hagerhall, C. M., Johansson, M., and Patil, G. G. (2015). Restorative elements at the computer workstation: a comparison of live plants and inanimate objects with and without window view. Environ. Behav. 47, 288–303. doi: 10.1177/0013916513499584
Filipas, L., Borghi, S., La Torre, A., and Smith, M. R. (2021a). Effects of mental fatigue on soccer-specific performance in young players. Sci. Med. Football 5, 150–157. doi: 10.1080/24733938.2020.1823012
Filipas, L., Ferioli, D., Banfi, G., La Torre, A., and Vitale, J. A. (2021b). Single and combined effect of acute sleep restriction and mental fatigue on basketball free-throw performance. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 2021, 1–6. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2020-0142
Finley, A. J., Tang, D., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2019). Sweet nothings: No effects of self-control exertion on blood glucose levels. Soc. Psychol. 50, 322–331. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000376
Fortes, L. S., De Lima-Junior, D., Fiorese, L., Nascimento-Júnior, J. R. A., Mortatti, A. L., and Ferreira, M. E. C. (2020). The effect of smartphones and playing video games on decision-making in soccer players: a crossover and randomised study. J. Sports Sci. 38, 552–558. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1715181
Fortes, L. S., Gantois, P., de Lima-Júnior, D., Barbosa, B. T., Ferreira, M., Nakamura, F. Y., et al. (2021). Playing videogames or using social media applications on smartphones causes mental fatigue and impairs decision-making performance in amateur boxers. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 1–12. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2021.1927036 [Epub ahead of print].
Fortes, L. S., Fonseca, F. S., Nakamura, F. Y., Barbosa, B. T., Gantois, P., de Lima-Júnior, D., et al. (2021b). Effects of mental fatigue induced by social media use on volleyball decision-making, endurance, and countermovement jump performance. Perceptual Motor Skills 128, 2745–2766. doi: 10.1177/00315125211040596
Fortes, L. S., Lima-Junior, D., Nascimento-Junior, J. R. A., Costa, E. C., Matta, M. O., and Ferreira, M. E. C. (2019). Effect of exposure time to smartphone apps on passing decision-making in male soccer athletes [article]. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 44, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.05.001
Francis, Z. L., and Inzlicht, M. (2016). “Proximate and ultimate causes of ego depletion,” in Self-regulation and ego control, eds E. R. Hirt, J. J. Clarkson, and L. Jia (Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Scarborough), 373–398. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801850-7.00018-4
Freeman, S., and Eykelbosh, A. (2020). COVID-19 and outdoor safety: Considerations for use of outdoor recreational spaces. National Collaborating Centre for Environmental Health. Available online at: https://policycommons.net/artifacts/2075791/covid-19-and-outdoor-safety/2831090/ (accessed February 18, 2022).
Furley, P., Bertrams, A., Englert, C., and Delphia, A. (2013). Ego depletion, attentional control, and decision making in sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 900–904. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.08.006
Gailliot, M. T., Plant, E. A., Butz, D. A., and Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Increasing self-regulatory strength can reduce the depleting effect of suppressing stereotypes. Personali. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 281–294. doi: 10.1177/0146167206296101
Gantois, P., Caputo Ferreira, M. E., Lima-Junior, D. D., Nakamura, F. Y., Batista, G. R., Fonseca, F. S., et al. (2020). Effects of mental fatigue on passing decision-making performance in professional soccer athletes [article]. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 20, 534–543. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1656781
Giboin, L. S., and Wolff, W. (2019). The effect of ego depletion or mental fatigue on subsequent physical endurance performance: a meta-analysis. Perform. Enhanc. Health 7, 100150. doi: 10.1016/j.peh.2019.100150
Graham, J. D., and Brown, D. M. (2021). “Understanding and interpreting the effects of prior cognitive exertion on self-regulation of sport and exercise performance,” in Handbook of self-regulation and motivation in sport and exercise, eds C. Englert and I. Taylor (London: Routledge), 1–21.
Green, B. N., Johnson, C. D., and Adams, A. (2006). Writing narrative literature reviews for peer-reviewed journals: secrets of the trade. J. Chiropr. Med. 5, 101–117. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3467(07)60142-6
Grgic, J., Mikulic, I., and Mikulic, P. (2022). Negative effects of mental fatigue on performance in the yo-yo test, loughborough soccer passing and shooting tests: a meta-analysis. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 7:10010. doi: 10.3390/JFMK7010010
Grinde, B., and Patil, G. G. (2009). Biophilia: does visual contact with nature impact on health and well-being? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 6, 2332–2343. doi: 10.3390/ijerph6092332
Habay, J., Van Cutsem, J., Verschueren, J., De Bock, S., Proost, M., De Wachter, J., et al. (2021). Mental fatigue and sport-specific psychomotor performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 2021:0123456789. doi: 10.1007/s40279-021-01429-6
Haga, A., Halin, N., Holmgren, M., and Sörqvist, P. (2016). Psychological restoration can depend on stimulus-source attribution: a challenge for the evolutionary account? Front. Psychol. 7:1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01831
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486
Han, K. T. (2003). A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Land. Urban Plan. 64, 209–232. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(02)00241-4
Hancock, P. A., and Desmond, P. A. (2001). Stress, workload, and fatigue. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hicks, L. J., Smith, A. C., Ralph, B. C. W., and Smilek, D. (2020). Restoration of sustained attention following virtual nature exposure: undeniable or unreliable? J. Environ. Psychol. 71, 101488–101488. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101488
Howell, A. J., Dopko, R. L., Passmore, H.-A., and Buro, K. (2011). Nature connectedness: associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personali. Indiv. Diff. 51, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.037
Inzlicht, M., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 450–463. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454134
Inzlicht, M., Bartholow, B. D., and Hirsh, J. B. (2015). Emotional foundations of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.004
Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009
Jahan, N., Naveed, S., Zeshan, M., and Tahir, M. A. (2016). How to conduct a systematic review: a narrative literature review. Cureus 8:e864. doi: 10.7759/cureus.864
Joye, Y. (2007). Architectural lessons from environmental psychology: the case of biophilic architecture. Rev. General Psychol. 11, 305–328. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.4.305
Kamitsis, I., and Francis, A. J. P. (2013). Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 36, 136–143. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.07.013
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward and integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 13, 169–182.
Kaplan, S. (2001). Meditation, restoration, and the management of mental fatigue. Environ. Behav. 33, 480–506. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973106
Kaplan, S., and Berman, M. G. (2010). Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5, 43–57. doi: 10.1177/1745691609356784
Karyotaki, M., Drigas, A., and Skianis, C. (2017). Attentional control and other executive functions. Int. J. Emerg. Technol. Learn. 12, 219–233. doi: 10.3991/ijet.v12i03.6587
Kastner, S., Pinsk, M. A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1999). Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 22, 751–761. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80734-5
Koopmann, T., Faber, I., Baker, J., and Schorer, J. (2020). Assessing technical skills in talented youth athletes: a systematic review. Sports Med. 50, 1593–1611. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01299-4
Lanki, T., Siponen, T., Ojala, A., Korpela, K., Pennanen, A., Tiittanen, P., et al. (2017). Acute effects of visits to urban green environments on cardiovascular physiology in women: a field experiment. Environ. Res. 159, 176–185. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.07.039
Larson, G. E., and Memtt, C. R. (1991). Can accidents be predicted? An empirical test of the cognitive failures questionnaire. Appl. Psychol. Int. Rev. 40, 37–45.
Laumann, K., Gärling, T., and Stormak, K. M. (2003). Selective attention and heart rate responses to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 125–134. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00110-X
Le Mansec, Y., Pageaux, B., Nordeza, A., Dorela, S., and Jubeaua, M. (2017). Mental fatigue alters the speed and the accuracy of the ball in table tennis. J. Sports Sci. 36, 2751–2759. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2017.1418647
Lee, K. E., Williams, K. J. H., Sargent, L. D., Williams, N. S. G., and Johnson, K. A. (2015). 40-second green roof views sustain attention: the role of micro-breaks in attention restoration. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.04.003
Li, D., and Sullivan, W. C. (2016). Impact of views to school landscapes on recovery from stress and mental fatigue. Land. Urban Plan. 148, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.12.015
Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018
MacDonald Angus, W., Cohen Jonathan, D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter Cameron, S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288, 1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
Marcora, S. (2009). Perception of effort during exercise is independent of afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 2060–2062. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.90378.2008
Marcora, S. (2014). The effects of mental fatigue on repeated sprint ability and cognitive performance in football players. Available online at: http://www.uefa.org/MultimediaFiles/Download/uefaorg/Medical/02/20/41/86/2204186_DOWNLOAD.pdf (accessed December 15, 2021).
Marcora, S. M. (2008). Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance? Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 104, 929–931. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0818-3
Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., and Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 857–864. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008
Martin, K., Meeusen, R., Thompson, K. G., Keegan, R., and Rattray, B. (2018). Mental fatigue impairs endurance performance: a physiological explanation. Sports Med. 48, 2041–2051. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0946-9
McMorris, T. (2020). Cognitive fatigue effects on physical performance: the role of interoception. Sports Med. 50, 1703–1708. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01320-w
Morecraft, R. J., Geula, C., and Mesulam, M. M. (2015). Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. Arch. Nerol. 50, 279–284. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540030045013
Moreira, A., Aoki, M. S., Franchini, E., da Silva Machado, D. G., Paludo, A. C., and Okano, A. H. (2018). Mental fatigue impairs technical performance and alters neuroendocrine and autonomic responses in elite young basketball players. Physiol. Behav. 196, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.015
Murray, S. O., and Wojciulik, E. (2004). Attention increases neural selectivity in the human lateral occipital complex. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 70–74. doi: 10.1038/nn1161
Naghavi, N., Taheri, M., and Irandoust, K. (2018). Psychophysiological responses to cognitive and physical training in obese elderly. Brief Rep. 1:e83935. doi: 10.5812/intjssh.83935
Neilson, B. N., Craig, C. M., Curiel, R. Y., and Klein, M. I. (2020). Restoring attentional resources with nature: A replication study of Berto’s (2005) paradigm including commentary from Dr. Rita Berto. Hum. Factors 63, 1046–1060. doi: 10.1177/0018720820909287
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: on the relations among self- regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Physiol. Behav. 176, 139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.040
Pancani, L., Marinucci, M., Aureli, N., and Riva, P. (2021). Forced social isolation and mental health: a study on 1,006 italians under COVID-19 lockdown [original research]. Front. Psychol. 2021:12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663799
Pilotti, M., Klein, E., Golem, D., Piepenbrink, E., and Kaplan, K. (2015). Is viewing a nature video after work restorative? Effects on blood pressure, task performance, and long-term memory. Environ. Behav. 47, 947–969. doi: 10.1177/0013916514533187
Proost, M., Habay, J., Wachter, J. D., Pauw, K. D., and Rattray, B. (2022). How to tackle mental fatigue : a systematic review of potential countermeasures and their underlying mechanisms. Sports Med. 2022:123456789. doi: 10.1007/s40279-022-01678-z
Rampinini, E., Bishop, D., Marcora, S. M., Ferrari Bravo, D., Sassi, R., and Impellizzeri, F. M. (2007). Validity of simple field tests as indicators of match-related physical performance in top-level professional soccer players. Int. J. Sports Med. 28, 228–235. doi: 10.1055/s-2006-924340
Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1996). Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 228–236. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80077-8
Russell, S., Jenkins, D., Rynne, S., Halson, S. L., and Kelly, V. (2019). What is mental fatigue in elite sport? Perceptions from athletes and staff. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 19, 1367–1376. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1618397
Sakamoto, S., Takeuchi, H., Ihara, N., Ligao, B., and Suzukawa, K. (2018). Possible requirement of executive functions for high performance in soccer. PLoS One 13:1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201871
Scharfen, H. E., and Memmert, D. (2019). The relationship between cognitive functions and sport-specific motor skills in elite youth soccer players. Front. Psychol. 10:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00817
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. J. Exp. Psychol. General 136, 241–255. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241
Shaabani, F., Naderi, A., Borella, E., and Calmeiro, L. (2020). Does a brief mindfulness intervention counteract the detrimental effects of ego depletion in basketball free throw under pressure? Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 9, 197–215. doi: 10.1037/spy0000201
Smith, M. R., Coutts, A. J., Merlini, M., Deprez, D., Lenoir, M., and Marcora, S. M. (2016a). Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 267–276. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000762
Smith, M. R., Zeuwts, L., Lenoir, M., Hens, N., De Jong, L. M. S., and Coutts, A. J. (2016b). Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific decision-making skill. J. Sports Sci. 34, 1297–1304. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1156241
Smith, M. R., Fransen, J., Deprez, D., Lenoir, M., and Coutts, A. J. (2017). Impact of mental fatigue on speed and accuracy components of soccer-specific skills. Sci. Med. Football 1, 48–52. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1252850
Smith, M. R., Marcora, S. M., and Coutts, A. J. (2015). Mental fatigue impairs intermittent running performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 47, 1682–1690. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000592
Smith, M. R., Thompson, C., Marcora, S. M., Skorski, S., Meyer, T., and Coutts, A. J. (2018). Mental fatigue and soccer: current knowledge and future directions. Sports Med. 48, 1525–1532. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0908-2
Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., and Schubert, T. (2014). Motivational and cognitive determinants of control during conflict processing. Cogn. Emot. 28, 1076–1089. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.870134
Soylu, Y., Arslan, E., and Kilit, B. (2021). Psychophysiological responses and cognitive performance: a systematic review of mental fatigue on soccer performance. Rev. Article 4:e124244. doi: 10.5812/intjssh.124244
Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T., and Bentsen, P. (2018). Attention restoration theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B Crit. Rev. 21, 227–268.
Stone, R. J. (2008). Human factors guidelines for interatcive 3D and game-based training systems design, 1st Edn. Birmingham: Human Factors Integration Defence Technology Centre Publication. University of Birmingham.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
Sun, H., Soh, K. G., and Xu, X. (2022a). Nature scenes counter mental fatigue-induced performance decrements. Soccer 13, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.877844
Sun, H., Soh, K. G., Norjali Wazir, M. R. W., Ding, C., Xu, T., and Zhang, D. (2022b). Can self-regulatory strength training counter prior mental exertion? a systematic review of randomized controlled trials [systematic review]. Front. Public Health 10:374. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.904374
Sun, H., Soh, K. G., Roslan, S., Wazir, M., and Soh, K. L. (2021). Does mental fatigue affect skilled performance in athletes? Systematic review. PLoS One 16:e0258307. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258307
Trecroci, A., Boccolini, G., Duca, M., Formenti, D., and Alberti, G. (2020). Mental fatigue impairs physical activity, technical and decision-making performance during small-sided games. PLoS One 15:e0238461.
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). “Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment,” in Human behavior and environment: Behavior and natural environment, Vol. 6, eds I. Altman and J. Wohlwill (New York, NY: Plenum), 85–125.
Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Stephen, B., Meeusen, R., and Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 47, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0
Veness, D., Patterson, S. D., Jeffries, O., and Waldron, M. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on cricket-relevant performance among elite players. J. Sports Sci. 35, 2461–2467. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1273540
Weerakkody, N. S., Taylor, C. J., Bulmer, C. L., Hamilton, D. B., Gloury, J., O’Brien, N. J., et al. (2021). The effect of mental fatigue on the performance of Australian football specific skills amongst amateur athletes. J. Sci. Med. Sport 24, 592–596. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.12.003
Wright, L. J., Williams, S. E., and Veldhuijzen van Zanten, J. J. C. S. (2021). Physical activity protects against the negative impact of coronavirus fear on adolescent mental health and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic [original research]. Front. Psychol. 12:580511. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580511
Keywords: nature exposure, self-regulation, skilled performance, mental fatigue, intervention, conceptual framework
Citation: Sun H, Soh KG, Roslan S, Wazir MRWN, Mohammadi A, Ding C and Zhao Z (2022) Nature exposure might be the intervention to improve the self-regulation and skilled performance in mentally fatigue athletes: A narrative review and conceptual framework. Front. Psychol. 13:941299. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.941299
Received: 12 May 2022; Accepted: 04 July 2022;
Published: 02 August 2022.
Edited by:
Bernadette Ann Murphy, Ontario Tech University, CanadaReviewed by:
Jeffrey D. Graham, Ontario Tech University, CanadaMorteza Taheri, Imam Khomeini International University, Iran
Copyright © 2022 Sun, Soh, Roslan, Wazir, Mohammadi, Ding and Zhao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Zijian Zhao, emp6aGFvQHp6dS5lZHUuY24=