- 1Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
- 2Department of Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore, Singapore
Third Culture Kids (TCKs) are children of expatriates who live in a culture other than their country of nationality or their parent's country of nationality for a significant part of their childhood. Past research has indicated that adjustment is a key factor in the success of global mobility. However, current research in the area of TCK adjustment is lacking. This systematic review aims to present and summarize all available published scientific data on the adjustment of internationally mobile children and adolescents who relocate with their families. We aim to understand factors related to TCK adjustment, highlight lacking research areas, and define areas of interest for future research. The eligibility criteria for inclusion in the review were: traditional TCKs; aged 7–17 years; measures taken during the relocation; outcome variables of wellbeing, psychological adjustment or social adjustment, or socio-cultural adjustment or adjustment. An initial search across eight databases in December 2021 yielded 9,433 studies, which were included in COVIDENCE and reviewed independently by two researchers at each phase. We finally included 14 studies in this study, 10 of which presented quantitative data. Extracted quantitative and qualitative studies were abstracted, and the main findings are presented using a consistent grid of codes: an initial computerized lexical scan (Leximancer) of all included papers generated a preliminary list of topics and their frequencies. We refined these initial topics using the most prominent theories around the topics of TCK, adjustment, and the extracted theories from selected papers and created a codebook. Then we abstracted the quantitative data from the selected studies and organized the statistically significant findings according to the codes. Lastly, we abstracted and synthesized the findings from qualitative studies. Efforts were made to present the available data within a reading grid, which enhances the understanding of mechanisms specific to the sample population and also makes it apparent where more research is needed. Specifically, findings suggest a need for a more inclusive multi-trajectory adjustment model and a better definition of the ecological sample. The coding system for the extraction and analysis in this systematic review may be a guide for researchers planning future studies on TCK adjustment.
Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42020151071, identifier: CRD42020151071.
Introduction
In 2021, there were an estimated 87 million expatriates worldwide (Finaccord, 2018). As approximately half of all expatriates relocate with a partner or child (Caligiuri and Bonache, 2016), understanding the challenges of global mobility for expatriates and their families is paramount to supporting this population.
Children of expatriates or Third Culture Kids (TCKs) are defined as children “accompanying one's parent(s) into a country that is different from at least one parent's passport country(ies) due to a parent's choice of work or advanced training” (Pollock et al., 2010, p. 44). TCK refers to the fact that these individuals grow up being influenced by three cultures: the heritage culture(s), the host-country culture(s), and the culture of expatriates and other TCKs. Although elements from each culture are assimilated into the TCK's life and identity, these individuals often have a greater sense of belonging with other TCKs and the international community rather than with the host or heritage culture (Pollock et al., 2010). TCKs, such as children of military, foreign service, corporate and missionary families, are distinctly different from other populations such as immigrants, refugees, and international adoptees (Pollock et al., 2010). Although these groups share the common experience of moving internationally, the transient nature of their stay and high-mobility patterns distinguish TCKs from other similar groups.
Extensive literature has highlighted the importance of positive adjustment during global mobility for expatriates and their families (e.g., Shaffer et al., 1999; Andreason, 2008; Takeuchi, 2010; Sterle et al., 2018). Expatriate adjustment is a complex process of change in various domains in response to a new environment and culture (Haslberger et al., 2014). Adjustment has been measured through constructs such as wellbeing, levels of satisfaction with self and the environment, psychological and emotional comfort, and the degree of fit and effectiveness between the person and their environment (Dawis and Lofquist, 1984; Taft, 1988; Black and Stephens, 1989; Haslberger and Brewster, 2009). While past adjustment theories (e.g., Berry, 1990, 1997; Searle and Ward, 1990) set the stage for research and provide a framework for understanding this concept, they do not encapsulate the full complexities of expatriate adjustment. The more recent 3-D Model of Adjustment (Haslberger et al., 2014) offers a more holistic view of adjustment by proposing an interplay between internal and external dimensions, several domains, and time. In the existing literature, expatriate adjustment is often measured in terms of psychological and socio-cultural adjustment. Psychological adjustment can be measured through indicators of wellbeing and mental health, such as internalizing (i.e., depression and anxiety) or externalizing symptoms (behavior problems), stress, and self-esteem (Pollard and Lee, 2003). Socio-cultural adjustment can be competence and mastery of behaviors, emotions and cognitions fitting to the host culture (Haslberger, 2005).
Despite the extensive literature focused on expatriate, spouse, and family adjustment, the study of adjustment in TCKs is still a relatively neglected area. In recent years, comprehensive reviews have been conducted on the concept of family systems in expatriate adjustment, transition programs, and identity development, as well as adult and college student TCK research (Sterle et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2020; Tan et al., 2021). While these are undoubtedly essential data, there still exists a gap in the literature for a review specifically focused on adjustment in TCKs. Additionally, many TCK adjustment studies were conducted through retrospective studies of childhood experiences (e.g., Decuyper et al., 2019) or by respondents other than the TCK themselves (Izumi and Gullón-Rivera, 2018). And although retrospective studies offer valuable insights into TCK adjustment, they also carry threats to internal and external validity (Tofthagen, 2012).
The current paper aims to fill this gap by providing a comprehensive systematic review synthesizing the available empirical evidence on adjustment in TCKs and focuses exclusively on findings during their relocation. To expand on current reviews, external indicators such as family functioning, stress, structure, social support, and demographic and mobility variables (such as age, gender, length and duration of expatriation, number of moves, home country, and host country) which predict adjustment were also included. We aim to understand factors related to TCK adjustment, highlight lacking research areas, and define areas of interest for future research.
Methods
Retrieval procedures
This review aimed to capture all available English-language peer-reviewed journal articles on the adjustment of school-aged TCKs aged 5 to 18 years during their international stay. We included all published articles from the beginning of time until December 2021 across nine electronic databases: APA Psychinfo, PSYNDEXplus Literature, and Audiovisual Media, ERIC, MEDLINE, web of science, Scopus, SocINDEX, and sociological abstracts (Supplementary Datasheet 1).
Inclusion/exclusion criteria
The following eligibility criteria were set according to the PICO guidelines:
• Population: expatriate, third culture, cross-cultural, international, family relocation, sojourner, military, missionary, oil industry, oil patch, diplomat/Age sample: Kid, child, adolescent, youth, teen, family, student.
• Intervention: international relocation, measures are taken during the relocation.
• Comparison: some studies may use comparison groups (non-international/local). Both quantitative and qualitative studies were considered for inclusion.
• Outcome: wellbeing, adjustment, psychological adjustment, social adjustment, or adaptation.
The following conditions were set for inclusion:
• Participants aged between 5 and 17 years,
• Child/adolescent is the respondent,
• Child/adolescent has relocated internationally with their parent(s)/family,
• Measures have been taken during the international relocation,
• Expatriation is linked to parent/caregiver's employment,
• Adjustment is the primary outcome (including behavioral, affective, cognitive, academic, and socio-cultural determinants (Haslberger et al., 2014),
• Peer-reviewed published scientific articles.
We decided to focus on school-aged children as they are likely to interact within host communities, have developed language, friendships, and social references before the international move, and are therefore expected to be more affected by the stress from the relocation than younger children. We excluded late adolescents (19–21 years), tertiary students, and young adults as this population is likely to have moved away from their parents' homes to study and may need to adjust to circumstances other than the international move. We excluded papers that studied other expatriate populations (such as international students at the tertiary level, education migrants, high school exchange students, first and second-generation immigrants and migrants, child and adolescent adoptees, military deployment of a parent without family, and non-international relocation) as these populations have specific characteristics which may not entirely compare with traditional TCKs. Studies, where the respondent was not the child themself (teachers, parents, or retrospective studies from adult TCK) were excluded to limit the methodological biases which result from indirect measures. Other studies were excluded when the condition was not an international relocation (i.e., repatriation and returnees or domestic relocation). We excluded studies focusing on different themes than predictors and adjustment outcomes, such as testing the effect of specific programs. We also excluded non-empirical studies, for example, case reports, gray literature, reviews, unpublished work, theses, and commentaries. Studies were also excluded where the TCK data analysis was not separated from non-TCK groups.
Screening and quality assessment
The online review management and screening tool Covidence was used to screen studies. Covidence is a web-based collaboration software platform that streamlines the production of systematic and other literature reviews (Covidence, 2021). The screening and selection of the papers based on title, abstract, full text, and quality control and extraction phases were conducted independently by 3 study team members (E.J., M.R. and Y.P.O.) and research assistants. For each paper, the quality of studies to extract was established independently by two study team members (E.J. and M.R. or E.J. and Y.P.O.) using Joanna Briggs Institute's critical appraisal tools (Critical-Appraisal-Tools, 2022). The 8-item checklist for analytical cross-sectional studies and the 10-item checklist for qualitative research was used1. Due to the small number of eligible studies, inclusion of each paper was based on consensus. Results from the process can be seen in the PRISMA chart presented in Figure 1 (Moher et al., 2009).
Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases and registers only. From Page et al. (2021). For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/.
Data abstraction and analysis
First, we defined a codebook that could be used to abstract findings in both quantitative and qualitative papers, and a content analysis of both quantitative and qualitative studies was conducted using Leximancer2 content analysis and concept mapping software. This automated analysis method offers an unbiased and objective data analysis (Smith and Humphreys, 2006; Angus et al., 2013). The software systematically extracts concepts from uploaded full-text studies and assembles the concepts into clusters according to their prominence and connectedness (Supplementary Image 1). Leximancer's yield was refined according to the researcher's knowledge of the selected studies. Next, we compared the clusters with the extracted theoretical references (Table 1) and deducted codes from these two abstractions. Last, the deducted codes were applied to Leximancer's ranked concept list (Table 2), allowing for details to be added to the codebook. This preliminary content analysis offers an overview of the higher-level themes and clusters of concepts explored in the selected research papers. The codebook was used as a grid to organize quantitative and qualitative study findings.
Subsequently, all 14 extracted studies were abstracted in Tables 3, 4 to the recommended strategy described in the Matrix Method (Garrard, 2020). Table 3 presents predictors of adjustment, extracted and organized into three categories using the predefined codes: psychological, academic, socio-cultural, family, and environmental. Then, following the Matrix Method, results from 10 quantitative (including one mixed methods) studies were abstracted to reveal significant findings. Only results reported as significant and with given correlation coefficients and p-values from each study were extracted (Table 4). Then, the four qualitative studies (including one mixed methods) were abstracted using a thematic synthesis approach, allowing recurring themes to be abstracted from qualitative data using thematic headings (Thomas and Harden, 2008).
Thematic and conceptual extraction
Theoretical frameworks and references were extracted from the included studies and organized into categories, as shown in Table 1. Concurrently, researchers extracted clusters from the Leximancer content analysis: the concept map (Supplementary Image 1) shows four clusters of themes where family, stress, and coping (labeled “psychological”); school and culture (labeled “'socio-cultural”); and engagement (labeled “environment”) stand out. The links within these clusters show the most frequently associated themes, allowing the authors to label each cluster accurately. We used the clusters and extracted theoretical references to deduct the following codes: environmental, family, socio-cultural and psychological. Table 2 shows the ranked concept list from Leximancer, where the above codes have been applied to each concept, allowing researchers to refine the labels. The final codebook is presented below.
Predictors:
• Demographic and environmental factors: age, gender, nationality, mobility, and parent work.
• Family factors: family support, family functioning, and parental stress.
• Psychological factors: cognitive, personality, attachment, emotion, behavior, social skills, and identity.
• Socio-cultural factors, friendships, home, and culture, including intercultural sensitivity, acculturation, language, and school.
Outcomes:
• Psychological adjustment includes wellbeing, stress, and coping.
• Socio-cultural adjustment includes culture shock and acculturative stress.
• Third culture identity includes place identity and specific traits.
A thematic synthesis of the qualitative studies was undertaken following three stages (Thomas and Harden, 2008): (1) line-by-line coding of study findings and direct quotations using the predefined codebook, (2) abstracting the themes and findings from the qualitative studies, then (3) grouping coded findings to generate analytical themes across studies. All interviews addressed child and adolescent TCKs; one study included images as an addition to the interviews, and one included parents in separate interviews. Results from family interviews were only considered when it was clear that the child respondent originated a comment or idea.
Results
Preliminary analysis of studies
Table 5 presents studies ordered by continents, 5 year-periods, and journal types. The studies are evenly distributed over the past two decades and have been conducted primarily in Asia and Europe, whereas three were conducted across different continents. Studies were published in 13 psychology, development, education, society, intercultural, and human resources journals. Nine studies were quantitative, one used a mixed-methods design, and four were qualitative.
Factors of adjustment in quantitative studies
The 10 extracted quantitative studies' findings were abstracted and presented in Table 4 (Garrard, 2020). Significant results in each study are labeled according to the study number in Table 3 and the predefined codebook. Non-significant and null findings, correlations, and statistical weights can be found in Supplementary Datasheet 2. All 10 studies utilized surveys, out of which three were designed by the researchers (Pittman and Bowen, 1994; Miyamoto and Kuhlman, 2001; Straffon, 2003). One study used a mixed-methods approach. Comparison groups with local (non-international children/adolescents) were used in 4 out of the 10 studies (Gerner et al., 1992; Pittman and Bowen, 1994; Lam and Selmer, 2004; Morales, 2017).
Demographic variables
Ages ranged from 7 to 19 years, and samples included male and female participants of similar proportions. Sample sizes ranged from 39 to 272 in the TCK groups. Two studies found age to influence adjustment: notably, older adolescents were more likely to struggle with adjustment, and older teenagers used a more elaborate (approach vs. avoidance) coping strategy (Vercruysse and Chandler, 1992; McKeering et al., 2021) (n = 217). Gender was found to influence adjustment in two studies, with male students being less engaged at school and female TCK using a more elaborate (approach vs. avoidance) coping strategy (Vercruysse and Chandler, 1992; McKeering et al., 2021) (n = 217). Length of stay in the current setting positively predicted adjustment outcomes in 2 studies (Straffon, 2003; McKeering et al., 2021) (n = 692).
Family variables
The family was investigated in two studies, with TCK reportedly feeling closer to their families and family cohesion positively influencing adjustment (Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 62), (Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007) (n = 166).
Psychological variables
For personality traits, TCK were more open-minded, respectful, and flexible toward other cultures compared to their local counterparts (Gerner et al., 1992) (n = 147); (Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 62). Factors that improve adjustment outcomes are emotional stability (Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007) (n = 104) and self-efficacy (Ittel and Sisler, 2012) (n = 46). Factors that hinder adjustment outcomes are ambivalent attachment style (Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007) (n = 104) and repatriation anxiety (Miyamoto and Kuhlman, 2001) (n = 240).
Sociocultural variables
The perceived quality of social relationships with teachers, local friends (Ittel and Sisler, 2012) (n = 46), and those left behind (Miyamoto and Kuhlman, 2001) (n = 240) predict better adjustment. TCK were more interested in learning languages (Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 62), traveling (Gerner et al., 1992; Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 334), seeking a future abroad (Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 62) than their local peers. These findings are supported by measuring a distinct cultural identity (Lam and Selmer, 2004) (n = 62). Local language proficiency is shown to play a role in enhancing adjustment (Miyamoto and Kuhlman, 2001) (n = 240), whereas maintaining interest in “home language” reduces culture shock. TCK were generally more interested in language acquisition than their local counterparts (Gerner et al., 1992) (n = 272).
Orientation of outcomes
Psychological adjustment was explored through 12 findings, socio-cultural outcomes were explored through 22 findings, and the third culture was examined in 10 findings. In three cases, the same variable influenced socio-cultural and psychological adjustment. In one case, a psychological adjustment outcome was associated with a third culture trait. Three socio-cultural adjustment outcomes were associated with third culture traits.
Factors of adjustment in qualitative studies
Environmental factors
Context
Stability is an important protective factor to support adjustment when the context changes and can be found in immediate family rituals and maintained connections with extended family and friends (Mclachlan, 2007; Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017).
Time
Time spent abroad and in contact with diverse communities enhances an ethno-relative worldview and supports better acceptance of other cultures (Straffon, 2003).
Repatriation/high mobility
Fears of repatriation or frequent moves and lack of permanence may increase stress and hinder adjustment (Weeks et al., 2010).
Family factors
Child interviewees report increased family closeness through meetings, discussions, and meals, to supplement the lack of an extended family or other extensions (Mclachlan, 2007). Family closeness is a sensitive topic, bearing possibilities to support each other and the risk of a closeness that might raise tensions and limit autonomy. Being involved in the family's decision to move (communication) generally contributes to the child/teen's agreeableness with the move (Mclachlan, 2007). Family relationships contribute to a sense of safety, providing comfort and continuity (belonging and direction) during the initial adjustment phase and helping to reduce stress from situations when they arise. Family members and the rituals of family life and the objects associated with them provide a sense of continuity, replacing the physical concept of home. Connectedness with extended family and grandparents contributes to a sense of home and stability (Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017).
Psychological factors
Personality
Child personality is raised as a determining factor, and agreeableness toward the move creates an opportunity to embrace change (Mclachlan, 2007). Open-mindedness is critical for making friends and adopting a worldview, including in international schools where students have diverse cultures and origins (Weeks et al., 2010).
Emotion
Grief from loss and longing can be related to places, memories, objects, perceived changes in family roles and responsibilities, or even a lost psychological state (Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017).
TCKs describe mixed emotions of excitement, disappointment, and anticipation as they repeatedly adjust to change.
Identity
Adolescence is a susceptible age for a move. Integrating the multiplicity of values of the various systems to which TCKs are exposed, as well as their differences in being multi-lingual, multicultural, and aware of the diversity of the world, creates an extra challenge in the identity formation process (Langinier and Gaspoz, 2015; Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017). Identification with a particular place, culture, and community call for a specific model to be defined for TCKs, which differs from identity construction and identification in non-TCKs (Langinier and Gaspoz, 2015; Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017). “TCK identity” becomes an entity within which TCKs are more inclined toward each other. Langinier and Gaspoz (2015) develop the idea of three expressions of identity (cosmopolitan, transnational, and anchor) dependent on identifications to national or international communities and where TCKs experience and social background influence the development of one or the other identity (Langinier and Gaspoz, 2015).
Socio-cultural factors
Friendships
Loss of friends in international settings is a commonly raised issue; TCKs must grieve friends from home and face the departures of friends and teachers in international schools (Weeks et al., 2010; Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017). TCKs report casual friendships rather than close ones, which could be their way of dealing with repeated loss or a bias in reporting and hiding underlying grief difficulties (Mclachlan, 2007). Difficulties entering already formed friend groups or communicating with peers can be a significant deterrent for adjustment and integration, whereas identifying and making friends they can identify with is raised by teens as the most important factor of overall adjustment (Weeks et al., 2010).
Home
Children maintain a bond with their passport country(ies) and the different places they have lived, which provides a sense of attachment. Positive feelings and memories during times spent in these places contribute to the sense of connectedness to a place (Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017). A challenge in adjustment arises when there is too big a gap between an idealized place and life challenges in that place.
Culture
Learning about a new culture can mean more freedom for adolescents, exploration, and easier access to drugs and alcohol in the host culture. These are mentioned as either contributing to autonomy and identity construction or creating a riskier environment and hindering the adjustment process (Weeks et al., 2010). Teenagers in international schools may feel at home in their host country without assimilating or integrating into their host country's culture. Friendships and the school environment majorly contribute to the sense of homeliness. Teenagers socializing within their international communities may preserve a surface-level interaction and understanding of their host culture (Weeks et al., 2010). Housing and comfort are positively related to adjustment and feeling at home.
Language
TCKs in international schools do not consider language a primary factor in their adjustment. Host language fluency is placed behind friendships and family relationships, as they are not dependent on the host culture to make friends or integrate. However, language acquisition has the potential to enhance the TCK's familiarity with their surroundings (Weeks et al., 2010). Home country language fluency is often maintained as a thread to home or to facilitate potential repatriation (Lijadi and Van Schalkwyk, 2017).
Discussion
This systematic review is the first to synthesize the available data on factors that influence adjustment in child and adolescent TCKs during their international experiences. It also offers the reader an organized overview of empirical evidence on factors influencing TCK adjustment. Only 14 studies met our eligibility criteria despite screening across eight electronic databases. This yield speaks for the limited empirical evidence on child and adolescent TCK adjustment. Findings from this systematic review point toward gaps in the knowledge about the particular needs and traits that define child and adolescent TCK.
Factors in TCK adjustment
Both quantitative and qualitative studies find specific variables contributing to TCK functioning and adjustment. Categories of factors that are shown to influence adjustment in TCK include demographics (age, gender, time/mobility, cultural background), family (demographics, functioning, support, and cohesion), environmental (expatriate work), psychological (cognitive and personality traits, attachment style, emotion, empathy, identity) and socio-cultural (relationships, friends, in particular, culture, language, school, and international mobility factors). Each factor contributes to or hinders psychological and socio-cultural adjustment or contributes to forming a specific third culture. Although studies have measured various factors and pinpointed the effects of these factors on TCK adjustment, there is a lack of cohesion between variables and outcomes. Only peer relationships on the outcome of socio-cultural adjustment and travel preference on the outcome of a third culture were tested twice. The interest in languages on the outcome of socio-cultural adjustment was tested only three times. This is in contrast to adult expatriate research showing that language plays a key role in adjustment (for example Selmer, 2006). This could be due to the limited number of studies in our review. However, it is also possible that the selected studies explore expatriate children in international schools who are not as exposed to the host culture and language as their adult counterparts, as the medium of teaching is often English. Clearly, more research on the role of language in TCK adjustment is needed.
In general, more research is needed to assert these findings, which remain scarce in number and sample size. Moreover, future models may include mediation and moderation factors. The coding categories deducted for this systematic review may continue to be used as a guide for future studies.
Demographics and environmental factors
This systematic review shows that demographic and mobility factors have been considered across four studies in total. Only one study compared two international locations but found mobility overrides the actual location (Gerner et al., 1992). Another single study compared TCK with local peers. Efforts must be made to refine sample characteristics using demographic variables (Aderi et al., 2013). Samples of various age categories and family structures will further define the contribution of these demographic variables. More research is needed where comparison groups could help understand the influence of cultures and nationalities on adjustment.
Family factors
Qualitative studies have expanded upon the family factors involved in adjustment, including cohesion, parenting, and family rituals. Only two studies measured family characteristics, parent relationships, and family demographic variables in quantitative designs (Pittman and Bowen, 1994; Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007). More quantitative studies, including measures of family functioning, family cohesion, parenting, and family demographics, will assert these findings, as suggested by Sterle et al. (2018).
Psychological factors
Psychological factors are particularly under-investigated, although shown to largely contribute to wellbeing and adjustment (Arslan, 2019). Potential mediation and moderation effects, particularly the interaction between third culture and psychosocial adjustment, as well as family functioning and psychosocial adjustment, need to be investigated (Zeng et al., 2022).
Toward a broader model of adjustment
Future research may refine our understanding of TCK adjustment by devising and testing more inclusive models and multiple trajectories in adjustment (Haslberger et al., 2014; Hirai et al., 2015; Mesidor and Sly, 2016). The classification proposed in this review includes categories of environmental, family, psychological and socio-cultural factors as a general frame for understanding the interactions between factors and outcomes of TCK adjustment and may serve as a guide for future studies and the foundation for a model of TCK adjustment.
Defining and measuring adjustment
Extracted studies are scattered across the areas of psychology, development, education, human resources, and intercultural sciences. There is also diversity in the scope of theoretical references used to frame the research. Psychological adjustment may be linked to attachment theory, coping, identity, social identity, place identity concepts, and notions of stress and wellbeing. Socio-cultural adjustment may refer to Berry's acculturation theory, Bennett's intercultural sensitivity model, or notions of culture shock (Berry, 1980; Berry et al., 2006; Bennett and Hammer, 2017).
In some cases, adult adjustment models are used as models of child adjustment. Two studies also referenced family models (family stress and family functioning) (Pittman and Bowen, 1994; Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007). Theories used to frame research on TCK primarily target a specific model and explore either family, culture, identity, or psychological traits. The diverse theories and research found in this systematic review suggest that distinctive models may not reflect the entire process of TCK adjustment. More likely, adjustment at a point in time but also over time and identity outcomes are interconnected with psychological, socio-cultural, and environmental factors. As proposed for adult expatriates, a model reflecting these interrelations is needed for TCK (Haslberger et al., 2014).
Defining the TCK sample
The theoretical complexity continues with diverse samples falling under the generic understanding of the meaning of TCK: a reflection of this diversity can be read through the multiple terms (e.g., military, internationally mobile, TCK, expatriate) used across studies to refer to the particular population. Half of the studies in this review referred to Pollok and Van Reken's or Useem's definition of TCK (Useem and Useem, 1967; Pollock et al., 2010). The lack of cohesion in the definition of the sample itself is an insight into the diversity of the specific experiences associated with particular reasons underlying the international relocation. Another fundamental challenge for researching this population lies in the diverse nationalities of origin and relocation, age groups, duration of stay, types of schools, and family structures contributing to the variation in adjustment. One example of sampling difficulty can be found in comparing the following studies: the case of exploring culture shock in Japanese students adjusting to the U.S. and the other studying intercultural adjustment in TCK from 21 different home countries living in 37 different host countries (Miyamoto and Kuhlman, 2001; Van Oudenhoven et al., 2007). As the populations are so diverse, each study may only apply to a particular cultural sample and may not be generalizable to other TCK groups. To conclude, we suggest that the ecological complexity reflected in this systematic review may be better approached through the lens of complex systems, which can account for individual, contextual and cultural interactions (Brown and Goetz, 1987; Schwartz et al., 2010).
Study designs and measures
Studies included in this review have used a variety of measures, some designed for the study by the researchers, some based on pre-existing scales, and some using validated scales with normative information for a general population. Normative studies using validated scales could help create a standard for TCK, which would contribute to a better understanding of the outcomes of future quantitative studies. Reproducing studies using a particular scale would help assert the findings from an ecological standpoint and increase the consistency of results. Lastly, no study used a longitudinal design despite the specific sensitivity of time measured (as a predictor of mobility) in two of the presented studies (Fisher and Shaw, 1994; Straffon, 2003; Pritchard et al., 2007; McKeering et al., 2021). Future cohort studies, particularly those using a longitudinal design, as has been done with adult and college student expatriate samples, would reinforce findings from the cross-sectional studies available this far (Fisher and Shaw, 1994; Pritchard et al., 2007).
Limitations
Although this study has the merit of synthesizing available data on a clearly defined ecological sample, it has several limitations. First, the restrictive criteria for inclusion meant that only a small number of papers were included and studies with multiple informants, such as parents and teachers, were excluded. Other unpublished or pilot studies may contribute to TCK adjustment but were not included in this study to ensure the strong validity of our findings. Further, the abstracted results from quantitative studies were not included in a meta-analysis due to the heterogeneity of predictors and outcomes and the variety of analyses used and reported.
Conclusions
This review highlights the complexity of defining the TCK sample, the diversity of internal and external factors contributing to TCK adjustment, and the formation of a “third culture.” Because of this, the network of selected studies stands out as heterogeneous and difficult to analyze. To better assess the needs and characteristics of TCK, efforts can be made to improve the ecological validity of study samples and to consider adjustment within an inclusive multi-faceted model or through the lens of complex adaptive systems (Arrow et al., 2000; Nettle et al., 2013; Haslberger et al., 2014; Theodore and Bracken, 2020). More research is needed on TCKs at the time of the relocation, and over time and more effort can be made to improve the methodological quality of measures.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author contributions
EJ conceived the structure of the manuscript. EJ, MR, and YO reviewed the papers. EJ and MR drafted the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript and read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
Funding for this project was provided by the Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Dr. Robin Segerer and Dr. Andreas Ledl for their contributions to the search string and library search and the Home Abroad research assistants, specifically Cara Dopke, and Berfin Bakis, for contributing to the project.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.939044/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Aderi, M., Jdaitawi, M., Ishak, N., and Jdaitawi, F. (2013). The Influence of Demographic Variables on University Students' Adjustment in North Jordan. Int. Educ. Stud. 6, p172. doi: 10.5539/ies.v6n2p172
Ali, A., Van der Zee, K., and Sanders, G. (2003). Determinants of intercultural adjustment among expatriate spouses. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 27, 563–580. doi: 10.1016/s0147-1767(03)00054-3
Andreason, A. W. (2008). Expatriate adjustment of spouses and expatriate managers: an integrative research review. Int. J. Manag. 25, 382.
Angus, D., Rintel, S., and Wiles, J. (2013). Making sense of big text: a visual-first approach for analysing text data using Leximancer and Discursis. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 16, 261–267. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2013.774186
Arrow, H., McGrath, J. E., and Berdahl, J. L. (2000). Small Groups as Complex Systems: Formation, Coordination, Development, and Adaptation. Thousand oaks, CA; London; New Dehli: SAGE Publications. doi: 10.4135/9781452204666
Arslan, G. (2019). Exploring the effects of positive psychological strengths on psychological adjustment in adolescents. Child. Ind. Res. 12, 1449–1464. doi: 10.1007/s12187-018-9589-5
Bennett, M. J. (1986). A developmental approach to training for intercultural sensitivity. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 10, 179–196. doi: 10.1016/0147-1767(86)90005-2
Bennett, M. J. (1993). “Towards ethnorelativism: a developmental model of intercultural sensitivity,” in Education for the Inntercultural Experience, 2nd Edn, ed R. M. Paige (Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press), 21–71.
Bennett, M. J., and Hammer, M. (2017). A developmental model of intercultural sensitivity. The international encyclopedia of intercultural communication 1. doi: 10.1002/9781118783665.ieicc0182
Berry, J. W. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. Accult. Theory Models Some New Findings 9, 25.
Berry, J. W. (1990). “Psychology of acculturation: understanding individuals moving between cultures,” in Applied Cross-Cultural Psychology (London: Sage), 232–253. doi: 10.4135/9781483325392.n11
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Appl. Psychol. 46, 5–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., and Vedder, P. (2006). Immigrant youth: acculturation, identity, and adaptation. Appl. Psychol. 55, 303–332. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00256.x
Bhawuk, D. P. S., and Brislin, R. (1992). The measurement of intercultural sensitivity using the concepts of individualism and collectivism. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 16, 413–436. doi: 10.1016/0147-1767(92)90031-o
Black, J. S. (1988). Work role transitions: A study of american expatriate managers in Japan. J. Int Bus. Stud. 19, 277–294. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8490383
Black, J. S., and Stephens, G. K. (1989). The influence of the spouse on american expatriate adjustment and intent to stay in pacific rim overseas assignments. J. Manag. 15, 529–544. doi: 10.1177/014920638901500403
Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. Br. J. Psychiatr. 130, 201–210. doi: 10.1192/bjp.130.3.201
Brown, L. A., and Goetz, A. R. (1987). Development-related contextual effects and individual attributes in third world migration processes: a Venezuelan example. Demography 24, 497–516. doi: 10.2307/2061388
Bruner, J. (2015). Car la culture donne forme à l'esprit: de la révolution cognitive à la psychologie culturelle. Retz.
Caligiuri, P., and Bonache, J. (2016). Evolving and enduring challenges in global mobility. J. World Bus. 51, 127–141. doi: 10.1016/j.jwb.2015.10.001
Chandler, L. A., and Shermis, M. D. (1990). “A paradigm for the study of childhood stress,” in Human Stress: Current Selected Research Vol. 4, ed J. H. Humphrey (New York, NY: AMS Press, Inc).
Covidence. (2021). Covidence Systematic Review Software. Veritas Health Innovation. Melbourne, Australia. Available online at: www.covidence.org (accessed April 30, 2022).
Critical-Appraisal-Tools. (2022). Critical Appraisal Tools | JBI. Available online at: https://jbi.global/critical-appraisal-tools (accessed March 31, 2022).
Dawis, R. V., and Lofquist, L. H. (1984). A Psychological Theory of Work Adjustment: An Individual-Differences Model and Its Applications. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
De Leon, C. T., and McPartlin, D. (1995). “Adjustment of expatriate children,” in Expatriate Management: New Ideas for International Business, ed J. Selmer (Westport, CT: Quorum Books).
Decuyper, I. I., Van Damme, P., Soentjens, P., and Wojciechowski, M. (2019). Low adherence with national travel medicine recommendations in Belgian expatriate children: a retrospective analysis. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 32, 101424. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2019.05.010
Finaccord. (2018). Global Expatriates: Size, Segmentation and Forecast for the Worldwide Market. Available online at: https://www.finaccord.com/Home/About-Us/Press-Releases/Global-Expatriates-Size,-Segmentation-and-Forecas (accessed April 30, 2022).
Fisher, C. D., and Shaw, J. B. (1994). Relocation attitudes and adjustment: a longitudinal study. J. Organ. Behav. 15, 209–224. doi: 10.1002/job.4030150304
Garrard, J. (2020). Health Sciences Literature Review Made Easy. Jones and Bartlett Learning. Available online at: https://books.google.ch/books?id=eOcLEAAAQBAJ.
Gerner, M., Perry, F., Moselle, M. A., and Archbold, M. (1992). Characteristics of internationally mobile adolescents. J. Sch. Psychol. 30, 197–214. doi: 10.1016/0022-4405(92)90031-Y
Haslberger, A. (2005). Facets and dimensions of cross-cultural adaptation:refining the tools. Pers. Rev. 34, 85–109. doi: 10.1108/00483480510571897
Haslberger, A., and Brewster, C. (2009). Capital gains: expatriate adjustment and the psychological contract in international careers. Hum. Resour. Manage. 48, 379–397. doi: 10.1002/hrm.20286
Haslberger, A., Brewster, C., and Hippler, T. (2014). Managing Performance Abroad. 0 ed. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203111468
Hayden, M., and Thompson, J. (2000). International education: Flying flags or raising standards. Int. School J. 19, 48–56.
Hirai, R., Frazier, P., and Syed, M. (2015). Psychological and socio-cultural adjustment of first-year international students: trajectories and predictors. J. Couns. Psychol. 62, 438–452. doi: 10.1037/cou0000085
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. (2003). Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organisations Across Nations. London; Thousand Oaks, CA; New Delhi: Sage.
Ittel, A., and Sisler, A. (2012). Third culture kids: adjusting to a changing world. Diskurs Kindheits-und Jugendforschung/Discourse. J. Childh. Adolesc. Res. 7, 21–22. doi: 10.3224/diskurs.v7i4.11
Izumi, M., and Gullón-Rivera, A. L. (2018). Family resilience among sojourning japanese mothers: links to marital satisfaction and children's behavioral adjustment. Fam. Consum. Sci. Res. J. 46, 282–296. doi: 10.1111/fcsr.12247
Kemph, J. P. (1969). Erik H. Erikson. Identity, youth and crisis. New York, NY: W. W. Norton Company, 1968. Behav. Sci. 14, 154–159. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830140209
Lam, H., and Selmer, J. (2004). Are former “third-culture kids” the ideal business expatriates? Career Dev. Int. 9, 109–122. doi: 10.1108/13620430410526166
Langinier, H., and Gaspoz, D. G. (2015). Expatriates' and teenagers' nomadic identities: an intersectional analysis. Equality Diver. Inclusion Int. J. 34, 308–324. doi: 10.1108/EDI-09-2012-0078
Lazarus, R. S., and Opton, E. M. (1966). “The study of psychological stress: A summary of theoretical formulations and experimental findings,” in Anxiety and Behaviour (Academic Press), 225–262. doi: 10.1016/b978-1-4832-3131-0.50015-9
Lijadi, A. A., and Van Schalkwyk, G. J. (2017). Place identity construction of Third Culture Kids: eliciting voices of children with high mobility lifestyle. Geoforum 100, 120–128. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2017.02.015
McKeering, P., Hwang, Y.-S., and Ng, C. (2021). A study into well-being, student engagement and resilience in early-adolescent international school students. J. Res. Int. Educ. 20, 69–92. doi: 10.1177/14752409211006650
Mclachlan, D. A. (2007). Global nomads in an international school: families in transition. J. Res. Int. Educ. 6, 233–249. doi: 10.1177/1475240907078615
Mesidor, J. K., and Sly, K. F. (2016). Factors that contribute to the adjustment of international students. J. Int. Students 6, 262–282. doi: 10.32674/jis.v6i1.569
Miller, S. T., Wiggins, G. M., and Feather, K. A. (2020). Growing up globally: third culture kids' experience with transition, identity, and well-being. In.t J. Adv. Counsel. 42, 414–423. doi: 10.1007/s10447-020-09412-y
Miyamoto, Y., and Kuhlman, N. (2001). Ameliorating culture shock in Japanese expatriate children in the US. Int. J. Intercul. Relat. 25, 21–40. doi: 10.1016/S0147-1767(00)00040-7
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 151, 264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135
Moos, R. H. (1984). Context and coping: Toward a unifying conceptual framework. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 12, 5–36. doi: 10.1007/bf00896933
Moos, R. H. (1990). Coping Responses Inventory-Youth Form. Center for Health Care Evaluation, Stanford University.
Morales, A. (2017). Intercultural sensitivity, gender, and nationality of third culture kids attending an international high school. J. Int. Educ. Res. 13, 35–44. doi: 10.19030/jier.v13i1.9969
Nelson, E. C., Landgraf, J. M., Hays, R. D., Kirk, J. W., Wasson, J. H., Keller, A., et al. (1990). “The COOP function charts: A system to measure patient function in physicians' offices,” in Functional Status Measurement in Primary Care (New York, NY: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-8977-4_8
Nettle, D., Gibson, M. A., Lawson, D. W., and Sear, R. (2013). Human behavioral ecology: current research and future prospects. Behav. Ecol. 24, 1031–1040. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ars222
Oberg, K. (1960). Cultural shock: Adjustment to new cultural environments. Pract. Anthropol. 7, 177–182. doi: 10.1177/009182966000700405
Olson, D. H., McCubbin, H. I., Barnes, H., Larsen, A., Muxen, M., and Wilson, M. (1986). Family Inventories. St Paul, MN: Family Social Science, University of Minnesota.
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M/., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372, n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71
Patterson, J. M., and McCubbin, H. I. (1987). Adolescent coping style and behaviors: conceptualization and measurement. J. Adolesc. 10, 163–186. doi: 10.1016/s0140-1971(87)80086-6
Piers, E. V., and Harris, D. B. (1984). Revised Manual for the Piers-Harris Children's Self-Concept Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Pittman, J. F., and Bowen, G. L. (1994). Adolescents on the move: adjustment to family relocation. Youth Soc. 26, 69–91. doi: 10.1177/0044118X94026001004
Pollard, E. L., and Lee, P. D. (2003). Child well-being: a systematic review of the literature. Soc. Indicat. Res. 61, 59–78. doi: 10.1023/A:1021284215801
Pollock, D. C., Van Reken, R. E., and Pollock, M. V. (2010). Third Culture Kids: The Experience of Growing up Among Worlds: The original, Classic Book on TCKs. Hachette UK.
Pritchard, M. E., Wilson, G. S., and Yamnitz, B. (2007). What predicts adjustment among college students? A longitudinal panel study. J. Am. Coll. Health 56, 15–22. doi: 10.3200/JACH.56.1.15-22
Proshansky, H. M., Fabian, A. K., and Kaminoff, R. (1983). Place-identity: Physical world socialization of the self. J. Environ. Psychol. 3, 57–83. doi: 10.1016/s0272-4944(83)80021-8
Reinicke, M. J. (1986). Cultural adjustment of international students in the U.S.: A reevaluation using reformulated learned helplessness. Doctor of Psychology Research Paper, Rosemead School of Psychology, La Mirada, California, United States.
Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Zamboanga, B. L., and Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation: implications for theory and research. Am. Psychol. 65, 237. doi: 10.1037/a0019330
Searle, W., and Ward, C. (1990). The prediction of psychological and socio-cultural adjustment during cross-cultural transitions. Int. J. Intercul. Relat. 14, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/0147-1767(90)90030-Z
Seligman MEP. (2011). Flourish: A Visionary New Understanding of Happiness and Well-Being. New York, NY: Free Press.
Selmer, J. (2006). Language ability and adjustment: western expatriates in China. Thunderbird Int. Business Rev. 48, 347–368. doi: 10.1002/tie.20099
Shaffer, M. A., and Harrison, D. A. (2001). Forgotten partners of international assignments: Development and test of a model of spouse adjustment. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 238–254. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.2.238
Shaffer, M. A., Harrison, D. A., and Gilley, K. M. (1999). Dimensions, determinants, and differences in the expatriate adjustment process. J. Int. Bus. Stud 30, 557–581. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8490083
Smith, A. E., and Humphreys, M. S. (2006). Evaluation of unsupervised semantic mapping of natural language with Leximancer concept mapping. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 262–279. doi: 10.3758/BF03192778
Sterle, M. F., Fontaine, J. R. J., De Mol, J., and Verhofstadt, L. L. (2018). Expatriate family adjustment: an overview of empirical evidence on challenges and resources. Front. Psychol. 9, 1207. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01207
Straffon, D. A. (2003). Assessing the intercultural sensitivity of high school students attending an international school. Int. J. Intercul. Relat. 27, 487–501. doi: 10.1016/S0147-1767(03)00035-X
Taft, R. (1988). “The psychological adaptation of soviet immigrants in Australia,” in Cross-Cultural Adaptation – Current Approaches, eds Y. Y. Kim and W. B. Gudykunst (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 150–167.
Takeuchi, R. (2010). A critical review of expatriate adjustment research through a multiple stakeholder view: progress, emerging trends, and prospects. J. Manag. 36, 1040–1064. doi: 10.1177/0149206309349308
Tan, E. C., Wang, K. T., and Cottrell, A. B. (2021). A systematic review of third culture kids empirical research. Int. J. Intercul. Relat. 82, 81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.03.002
Theodore, L. A., and Bracken, B. A. (2020). “Positive psychology and multidimensional adjustment,” in Promoting mind–Body Health In Schools: Interventions for Mental Health Professionals Applying psychology in the schools (Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association), 131–143. doi: 10.1037/0000157-009
Thomas, J., and Harden, A. (2008). Methods for the thematic synthesis of qualitative research in systematic reviews. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 8, 45. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-8-45
Tofthagen, C. (2012). Threats to validity in retrospective studies. J. Adv. Pract. Oncol. 3, 181–183. doi: 10.6004/jadpro.2012.3.3.7
Twigger-Ross, C. L., and Uzzell, D. L. (1996). Place and identity processes. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 205–220. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0017
Useem, J., and Useem, R. (1967). The interfaces of a binational third culture: a study of the American community in India. Soc. Issues 23, 130–143. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1967.tb00567.x
Useem, R. H. (2001). TCK ‘Mother' pens history of field. NewsLinks: The Newspaper of International School Services. Available online at: http://iss.edu/pages/kids.html
Valsinier, J. (2007). Culture in Minds and Societies: Foundations of Cultural Psychology. London; Thousand Oaks, CA; New Delhi: Sage.
van der Zee, K. I., and van Oudenhoven, J. P. (2000). The multicultural personality questionnaire: a multidimensional instrument of multicultural effectiveness. Euro. J. Pers. 14, 291–309. doi: 10.1002/1099-0984(200007/08)14:4<291::aid-per377>3.0.co;2-6
Van Oudenhoven, J. P., Timmerman, M. E., and Van der Zee, K. I. (2007). Cross-cultural equivalence and validity of the Multicultural Personality Questionnaire in an intercultural context. J. Int. Commun. 13, 51–65. doi: 10.1080/13216597.2007.9674714
Vercruysse, N. J., and Chandler, L. A. (1992). Coping strategies used by adolescents in dealing with family relocation overseas. J. Adolesc. 15, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/0140-1971(92)90066-E
Ward, C., and Kennedy, A. (1999). The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 23, 659–677. doi: 10.1016/s0147-1767(99)00014-0
Ward, C., and Rana-Deuba, A. (1999). Acculturation and adaptation revisited. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 30, 422–442. doi: 10.1177/0022022199030004003
Weeks, K. P., Weeks, M., and Willis-Muller, K. (2010). The adjustment of expatriate teenagers. Pers. Rev. 39, 24–43. doi: 10.1108/00483481011007841
Keywords: TCK, child, adolescent, adjustment, systematic review, factors
Citation: Jones EM, Reed M, Gaab J and Ooi YP (2022) Adjustment in third culture kids: A systematic review of literature. Front. Psychol. 13:939044. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.939044
Received: 08 May 2022; Accepted: 20 September 2022;
Published: 28 November 2022.
Edited by:
Yasuhiro Kotera, University of Nottingham, United KingdomReviewed by:
Chris Brewster, University of Reading, United KingdomMichał Wilczewski, University of Warsaw, Poland
Ahmed Rageh Ismail, Universiti Utara Malaysia, Malaysia
Copyright © 2022 Jones, Reed, Gaab and Ooi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Emma Marchal Jones, e.jones@unibas.ch
†These authors share first authorship