- Research Center for Education Learning and Didactics (EA 3875), University of West Brittany, Brest, France
Introduction
The Naturalistic Decision-Making (NDM) approach (Klein, 2008, 2015; Hallé Petiot et al., 2021) is an alternative to the cognitivist approach (i.e., decision-making viewed as a rationalistic linear information process) and to the ecological-dynamic approach (i.e., decision-making viewed as behavioral adaptation to environmental constraints). NDM researchers have discovered that decision makers in natural settings rely heavily on intuition. Experts use their experiences to focus their perceptions onto salient features, to recognize situations as typical, and to choose the most appropriate option. The Recognition Primed-Decision (RPD) model (Klein et al., 2010) postulates that experts' decision-making is a recognition process and suggests three levels of experiencing a situation: (i) simple match (recognize a situation and associate the first adequate option), (ii) diagnose the situation (faced with a non-familiar situation and need time to adapt a typical action), (iii) evaluate a course of action (simulate the relevance of the first option through mental visualization).
The RPD model has been applied in sport settings as rapid decision-making is essential for sporting expertise (for a review see, Macquet, 2020). Numerous qualitative studies have investigated the recognition processes in Australian Football officials (Neville et al., 2017), soccer referees (Mascarenhas et al., 2009), badminton players (Macquet and Fleurance, 2007), volleyball players (Macquet, 2009; Fortin-Guichard et al., 2021), soccer players (Bossard, 2010; Kermarrec and Bossard, 2014), ice hockey players (Bossard et al., 2010; Mulligan et al., 2012), handball goalkeepers (Le Menn et al., 2019), basket-ball players (Macquet and Kragba, 2015), judo athletes (Macquet and Lacouchie, 2017), karate athlete (Milazzo and Fournier, 2015), rugby coaches (Collins et al., 2016; Ashford et al., 2020), and sport adventure coaches (Collins and Collins, 2016).
Thus, this consistent empirical evidence obtained through investigation during naturalistic games strengthen the assumption that experts' decision-making in sport is based on their intuition. Nevertheless, some paradoxes remain and call for future investigation: six key points could be promising avenues of research.
Interplay Between Intuitive and Analytic Decisions
According to Macquet (2020) literature review of the RPD model in sport, 60–81% of the experts' decisions were classified as intuitive, using simple matching, about 13–28% of the experts' decisions were related to diagnosis, and about 3–24% were related to evaluating a course of action. Time pressure in the game situation is invoked to explain the predominance of intuition. Nevertheless, some issues suggest that the role of the decision-maker and the context in which he is embedded could affect the recognition mechanism. Investigating a handball goalkeeper's decision-making (Le Menn et al., 2019) or soccer defenders' decision-making (Kermarrec and Bossard, 2014), researchers found that when athletes were far from the ball, they had more time for mental simulation and diagnosing the development of possible situations. Furthermore, rugby coaches' decision-making was less subject to less time pressure than athletes, so that deliberative or analytic decision were frequent (Collins et al., 2016).
Taken together, these outcomes suggest that decision-making in a sport setting may take place on a continuum between intuitive and analytical processes (Kahneman and Klein, 2009). In this sense, integrated theoretical models of decision-making have recently been proposed for athletes (Ashford et al., 2021a,b; Hallé Petiot et al., 2021), referees (Kittel et al., 2021a; Samuel et al., 2021) and coaches (Collins et al., 2016; Richards et al., 2017). There is a lack of consistent empirical evidence in a sport setting for establishing the continuum from intuitive to analytical decision-making. Furthermore, based on outcomes in other research fields (e.g., Calabretta et al., 2017), future studies in sport should examine the interplay between intuitive and analytic decision-making in athletes, coaches and referees.
Visual Search Strategies and Intuition
Moreover, NDM studies also brought information on the relevant cues decision-makers take into account when embedded in the course of an action. As pointed by Macquet (2020), relevant cues were context-specific and were mostly concerned with visual perception. Today the field of visual search strategies is fertile among athletes (Vater et al., 2019; Krabben et al., 2022), referees (Luis del Campo et al., 2018; Moore et al., 2019) or coaches (Robertson et al., 2018; Mitchell et al., 2020). New methodological designs need to be proposed in order to study the visual information that is prioritized by experts in situation recognition and intuitive decisions in more ecological conditions (i.e., naturalistic conditions). Research designs that immerse participants in environments close to real sports settings with video filmed with a 360° camera (Musculus et al., 2021) or with virtual reality (Mascret et al., 2022) seem to be a very promising methodological avenue. Thus, an original contribution from RPD for sports may consist of studying decision-making in naturalistic sports games settings, providing empirical evidence on the way experts make intuitive decision and use very few cues to support ongoing decisions and actions.
Intuitive Decision-Making and Creativity
Although it is common to associate intuition and creativity, a clear link between them has not been adequately established in sports sciences. Quite the contrary: previous empirical research on IDM in sports settings showed that expert athletes choose their preferred “first option” most of time (in 80% of the decisions made), so they could be considered as non-creative. Nevertheless, the role of intuition in creativity should not be neglected as it is often reported to be a core component of the idea generation process (Pétervári et al., 2016). Sports researchers recently took into consideration this assumption when experimenting team sports programs aiming to develop players' creativity and adaptive behaviors (e.g., Coutinho et al., 2016). Furthermore, the relation between creativity and perceptual and cognitive processes was recently examined in experimental decision-making studies. Firstly, creative decision-making and visual search behavior were investigated in skilled soccer players (Roca et al., 2018; Roca and Ford, 2020). The results showed that most-creative players employed a broader attentional focus including more fixations of shorter duration and toward more informative locations of the display compared with least-creative players. Secondly, Klatt et al. (2019) examined the relation between creativity and IDM in two studies—one involving coaches and one involving soccer players—using video footage of real soccer matches. Results indicate a positive correlation between a player's creativity score and the quality of the first generated option for the whole sample. Even if the topics of those studies were not the recognition primed-decision processes, the results confirm insights about the relationship between IDM and creativity. If more creative players and coaches tend to maintain a broader attentional focus in a realistic sport setting, they should also be more sensitive to recognizing many salient cues.
IDM and the Role of Emotions
In recent decades, interest in the links between decision-making processes and emotions has grown (George and Dane, 2016). Emotions have often been considered as biases or hindrances to decision-making, especially in cognitive approaches studying athletes, referees and coaches (Tenenbaum et al., 2013). More recently, emotions have begun to emerge as resources for decision-making in a variety of models (e.g., Affect as information approaches, Appraisal tendency framework, Affect infusion model). The NDM approach also considers emotion as a potential resource. Empirical studies on this issue have focused on managers in companies (Sayegh et al., 2004), bank agents (Lipshitz and Shulimovitz, 2007), emergency doctors (Coget and Keller, 2010), film directors (Coget et al., 2011) or aircraft maintenance engineers (Naweed and Kingshott, 2019). These studies highlight the diverse roles of emotions in intuitive processes involved in complex, uncertain and time-pressured situations, supporting the theoretical assumptions of the NDM approach (Mosier and Fischer, 2010). To our knowledge, no study has shown the role of emotions in IDM in the context of sport. According to several empirical studies cited above and conducted through the NDM framework, situationally-induced emotions in a real-world context, namely “integral affects” (Mosier and Fischer, 2010) could play different roles in sports decision-making. Taken together these issues suggest further investigation of the role of emotion in the RPD model in a naturalistic decision-making perspective. Three hypotheses concerning these roles can thus be proposed: (1) expert pattern matching and intuitive decisions often could be guided by an affective appraisal of the situation, (2) emotional responses such as discomfort could motivate individuals to look for more sources of information to make a more deliberative decision, and (3) if the mental simulation of the potential evolution of the situation induces discomfort, individuals might be tempted to opt for another option.
The Development of IDM
The development of sports experts' IDM underlies two types of research questions corresponding to two distinct promising research avenues: (1) How does the development of IDM contribute to the development of sports expertise? (2) Which training programs could enhance IDM in sport?
NDM researchers in sport science have typically focused on IDM in experts, but the development of intuition and its contribution to the development of expertise still lacks empirical evidence (Martindale and Collins, 2013). Researchers from other fields have provided insights by considering the interaction between intuition and deliberation and how their use might lead to “skilled intuition” (Kahneman and Klein, 2009). Baylor (2001) argued for the possibility that the development of intuition follows a U-shaped progression where immature intuitions start at a relatively high level and then decrease when decisions become more analytic, and later increases with available intuitions. Bangert et al. (2014) hypothesized that musicians deliberately explore and test interpretative options, perhaps guided in their search by intuitive pattern recognition. Future studies in a sports context should examine the development of intuition to understand how athletes, coaches and referees become experts.
Proposals for training decision-making in sport are mainly based on a perceptual-cognitive approach to decision-making both for athletes (Hadlow et al., 2018), referees (Kittel et al., 2021b) and coaches (Richards et al., 2017). Proposals for training IDM based on NDM approach are less common and still lack empirical evidence. The research work of Kittel et al. (2021b) and the recommendations for introducing reflective practices with referees could be an interesting way to improve IDM. Future interventional studies could be set up based on programs dedicated to improving IDM in order to test their effects.
IDM in Teams
Previous sections in this paper argued to revisit the RPD model (Macquet, 2020). Several studies presented below are in sports contexts and especially in team sports. Thus, in naturalistic approaches there is a need to connect decision-making and team cognition research in both athletes (Steiner et al., 2017; Bourbousson et al., 2019), referees (Boyer et al., 2020) and coaches (Richards et al., 2017). Three perspectives could be investigated. Firstly, Kermarrec and Bossard (2014) showed that decision-making in defensive phases in soccer need to be coordinated. Results support the fact that decision-making is connected to shared cues in the situation. Secondly, as this paper argued for the investigation of the development of IDM to understand how athletes become experts, there is a need to understand the development of collective intuition at team level (Kérivel et al., 2021). Future studies could use recent theoretical advances (Akinci and Sadler-Smith, 2019) to understand links between collective intuition and decision-making. Thirdly, this paper suggests that IDM could be influenced by emotions. From a team perspective, shared emotion could also influence team naturalistic decision-making and the interpersonal coordination between team members (Van Hoorebeke, 2006; Tamminen and Crocker, 2013). Future studies may examine the influence of emotions on sharing and decision-making processes in team sports, coaching teams, and referee teams.
Conclusion
The use of the NDM approach, and more specifically of the RPD model is relatively recent in the field of sport. The growing number of empirical studies has confirmed its relevance for the understanding and enrichment of decision-making in sports settings. Previous results also invite researchers to question the model and its relations with six other determinants of expertise (analytical decisions, visual search strategies, creativity, emotions, development and team coordination), usually studied separately from IDM in both athletes, referees and coaches. In conclusion, the six avenues of research to revisit the RPD model, as suggested in this article, require innovative research methods to capture the complexity of IDM in naturalistic sports contexts.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Akinci, C., and Sadler-Smith, E. (2019). Collective intuition: implications for improved decision making and organizational learning: collective intuition: implications for organizations. Br. J. Manag. 30, 558–577. doi: 10.1111/1467-8551.12269
Ashford, M., Abraham, A., and Poolton, J. (2020). A communal language for decision making in team invasion sports. Int. Sport Coaching J. 8, 62. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2019-0062
Ashford, M., Abraham, A., and Poolton, J. (2021a). Understanding a player's decision-making process in team sports: a systematic review of empirical evidence. Sports 9, 65. doi: 10.3390/sports9050065
Ashford, M., Abraham, A., and Poolton, J. (2021b). What cognitive mechanism, when, where, and why? Exploring the decision making of university and professional rugby union players during competitive matches. Front. Psychol. 12, 609127. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609127
Bangert, D., Schubert, E., and Fabian, D. (2014). A spiral model of musical decision-making. Front. Psychol. 5, 320. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00320
Baylor, A. L. (2001). A U-shaped model for the development of intuition by level of expertise. New Ideas Psychol. 19, 237–244. doi: 10.1016/S0732-118X(01)00005-8
Bossard, C., Keukelaere, C. D., Pasco, D., and Kermarrec, G. (2010). L'activité décisionnelle en phase de contre-attaque en Hockey sur glace. Activites 7, 2341. doi: 10.4000/activites.2341
Bossard, C. (2010). L'activité décisionnelle en situation dynamique et collaborative: Application à la contre-attaque au football. Brest: Editions Universitaires Européennes.
Bourbousson, J., Feigean, M., and Seiler, R. (2019). Team cognition in sport: how current insights into how teamwork is achieved in naturalistic settings can lead to simulation studies. Front. Psychol. 10, 2082. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02082
Boyer, S., MacMahon, C., Récopé, M., and Rix-Lièvre, G. (2020). The assistant referees' activity in refereeing elite football: preoccupations when not judging offside. Psychol. Sport Exer. 48, 101662. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101662
Calabretta, G., Gemser, G., and Wijnberg, N. M. (2017). The interplay between intuition and rationality in strategic decision making: a paradox perspective. Org. Stud. 38, 365–401. doi: 10.1177/0170840616655483
Coget, J.-F., Haag, C., and Gibson, D. E. (2011). Anger and fear in decision-making: the case of film directors on set. Eur. Manag. J. 29, 476–490. doi: 10.1016/j.emj.2011.06.003
Coget, J.-F., and Keller, E. (2010). The critical decision vortex: lessons from the emergency room. J. Manag. Inquiry 19, 56–67. doi: 10.1177/1056492609357009
Collins, D., Collins, L., and Carson, H. J. (2016). “If it feels right, do it”: intuitive decision making in a sample of high-level sport coaches. Front. Psychol. 7, 504. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00504
Collins, L., and Collins, D. (2016). Professional judgement and decision-making in adventure sports coaching: the role of interaction. J. Sports Sci. 34, 1231–1239. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2015.1105379
Coutinho, P., Mesquita, I., Davids, K., Fonseca, A. M., and Côté, J. (2016). How structured and unstructured sport activities aid the development of expertise in volleyball players. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 25, 51-59. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.04.004
Fortin-Guichard, D., Thibault, N., Tétreault, É., Trottier, C., and Grondin, S. (2021). Applying the recognition-primed decision model to differentiate players' role in volleyball. J. Exper. 4, 248–269.
George, J. M., and Dane, E. (2016). Affect, emotion, and decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 136, 47-55. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.06.004
Hadlow, S. M., Panchuk, D., Mann, D. L., Portus, M. R., and Abernethy, B. (2018). Modified perceptual training in sport: a new classification framework. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 950–958. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.01.011
Hallé Petiot, G., Aquino, R., Silva, D. C., da Barreira, D. V., and Raab, M. (2021). Contrasting learning psychology theories applied to the teaching-learning-training process of tactics in soccer. Front. Psychol. 12, 1274. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637085
Kahneman, D., and Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. Am. Psychol. 64, 515-526. doi: 10.1037/a0016755
Kérivel, T., Bossard, C., and Kermarrec, G. (2021). Applying the input-process-outcome model to team learning in sport sciences: an exploratory qualitative study on twenty soccer players. J. Hum. Kinetics 78, 251–262. doi: 10.2478/hukin-2021-0040
Kermarrec, G., and Bossard, C. (2014). Defensive soccer players' decision making: a naturalistic study. J. Cogn. Eng. Decision Mak. 8, 187–199. doi: 10.1177/1555343414527968
Kittel, A., Cunningham, I., Larkin, P., Hawkey, M., and Rix-Lièvre, G. (2021a). Decision-making training in sporting officials: past, present and future. Psychol. Sport Exer. 56, 102003. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2021.102003
Kittel, A., Elsworthy, N., and Spittle, M. (2021b). The effectiveness of above real time training for developing decision-making accuracy in australian football umpires. Res. Quart. Exerc. Sport. 92, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2021.1939843
Klatt, S., Noël, B., Musculus, L., Werner, K., Laborde, S., Lopes, M., et al. (2019). Creative and intuitive decision-making processes: a comparison of brazilian and german soccer coaches and players. Res. Quart. Exerc. Sport. 90, 651–665. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2019.1642994
Klein, G. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Hum. Fact. 50, 456–460. doi: 10.1518/001872008X288385
Klein, G. (2015). A naturalistic decision making perspective on studying intuitive decision making. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 4, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.07.001
Klein, G., Calderwood, R., and Clinton-Cirocco, A. (2010). Rapid decision making on the fire ground: the original study plus a postscript. J. Cogn. Eng. Decision Mak. 4, 186-209. doi: 10.1518/155534310X12844000801203
Krabben, K., Mann, D., Lojanica, M., Mueller, D., Dominici, N., van der Kamp, J., et al. (2022). How wide should you view to fight? Establishing the size of the visual field necessary for grip fighting in judo. J. Sports Sci. 40, 236–247. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2021.1987721
Le Menn, M., Bossard, C., Travassos, B., Duarte, R., and Kermarrec, G. (2019). Handball goalkeeper intuitive decision-making: a naturalistic case study. J. Hum. Kinet. 70, 297–308. doi: 10.2478/hukin-2019-0042
Lipshitz, R., and Shulimovitz, N. (2007). Intuition and emotion in bank loan officers' credit decisions. J. Cogn. Eng. Dec. Mak. 1, 212–233. doi: 10.1518/155534307X232857
Luis del Campo, V., Canelo Fariñas, A., Domínguez Márquez, F. J., and Morenas Martín, J. (2018). The influence of refereeing experiences judging offside actions in football. Psychol. Sport Exer. 37, 139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.03.004
Macquet, A.-C. (2009). Recognition within the decision-making process: a case study of expert volleyball players. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 21, 64–79. doi: 10.1080/10413200802575759
Macquet, A.-C. (2020). “Decision-making in sport: looking at and beyond the recognition-primed decision model,” in Human Factors and Ergonomics in Sport, 1st Edn (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 135–154.
Macquet, A.-C., and Fleurance, P. (2007). Naturalistic decision-making in expert badminton players. Ergonomics 50, 1433–1450. doi: 10.1080/00140130701393452
Macquet, A.-C., and Kragba, K. (2015). What makes basketball players continue with the planned play or change it? A case study of the relationships between sense-making and decision-making. Cogn. Technol. Work 17, 345–353. doi: 10.1007/s10111-015-0332-4
Macquet, A.-C., and Lacouchie, H. (2017). Two case studies of decision-making under stress. J. Sports Sci. 35(Suppl. 1), 1–119. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2017.1378421
Martindale, A., and Collins, D. (2013). The development of professional judgment and decision making expertise in applied sport psychology. Sport Psychol. 27, 390–399. doi: 10.1123/tsp.27.4.390
Mascarenhas, D. R., Button, C., Hara, D., and Dicks, M. (2009). Physical performance and decision making in association football referees: a naturalistic study. Open Sports Sci. J. 2, 1–9. doi: 10.2174/1875399X00902010001
Mascret, N., Montagne, G., Devrièse-Sence, A., Vu, A., and Kulpa, R. (2022). Acceptance by athletes of a virtual reality head-mounted display intended to enhance sport performance. Psychol. Sport Exer. 61, 102201. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102201
Milazzo, N., and Fournier, J. (2015). Effect of individual implicit video-based perceptual training program on high-skilled karatekas' decision making. Mov. Sport Sci. 88, 13-19. doi: 10.3917/sm.088.0013
Mitchell, J., Maratos, F. A., Giles, D., Taylor, N., Butterworth, A., and Sheffield, D. (2020). The visual search strategies underpinning effective observational analysis in the coaching of climbing movement. Front. Psychol. 11, 1025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01025
Moore, L. J., Harris, D. J., Vine, S. J., Wilson, M. R., and Sharpe, B. T. (2019). Perceptual-cognitive expertise when refereeing the scrum in rugby union. J. Sports Sci. 37, 1778–1786. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2019.1594568
Mosier, K. L., and Fischer, U. (2010). The role of affect in naturalistic decision making. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Mak. 4, 240–255. doi: 10.1518/155534310X12844000801122
Mulligan, D., McCracken, J., and Hodges, N. J. (2012). Situational familiarity and its relation to decision quality in ice-hockey. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 10, 198–210. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2012.672009
Musculus, L., Bäder, J., Sander, L., and Vogt, T. (2021). The influence of environmental constraints in 360° videos on decision making in soccer. J. Sport Exer. Psychol. 43, 365–374. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2020-0166
Naweed, A., and Kingshott, K. (2019). Flying off the handle: affective influences on decision making and action tendencies in real-world aircraft maintenance engineering scenarios. J. Cogn. Eng. Dec. Mak. 13, 81–101. doi: 10.1177/1555343418821507
Neville, T. J., Salmon, P. M., and Read, G. J. (2017). Analysis of in-game communication as an indicator of recognition primed decision making in elite Australian rules football umpires. J. Cogn. Eng. Dec. Mak. 11, 81–96. doi: 10.1177/1555343416672557
Pétervári, J., Osman, M., and Bhattacharya, J. (2016). The role of intuition in the generation and evaluation stages of creativity. Front. Psychol. 7, 1420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01420
Richards, P., Collins, D., and Mascarenhas, D. R. (2017). Developing team decision-making: a holistic framework integrating both on-field and off-field pedagogical coaching processes. Sports Coach. Rev. 6, 57–75. doi: 10.1080/21640629.2016.1200819
Robertson, P. J., Callan, M., Nevison, C., and Timmis, M. A. (2018). Is visual search strategy different between level of judo coach when acquiring visual information from the preparation phase of judo contests? Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 13, 186–200. doi: 10.1177/1747954117737275
Roca, A., and Ford, P. R. (2020). Decision-making practice during coaching sessions in elite youth football across European countries. Sci. Med. Football 4, 263–268. doi: 10.1080/24733938.2020.1755051
Roca, A., Ford, P. R., and Memmert, D. (2018). Creative decision making and visual search behavior in skilled soccer players. PLoS ONE 13, e0199381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199381
Samuel, R. D., Tenenbaum, G., and Galily, Y. (2021). An integrated conceptual framework of decision-making in soccer refereeing. Int. J. Sport Exer. Psychol. 19, 738–760. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2020.1766539
Sayegh, L., Anthony, W. P., and Perrewé, P. L. (2004). Managerial decision-making under crisis: the role of emotion in an intuitive decision process. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 14, 179–199. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2004.05.002
Steiner, S., Macquet, A.-C., and Seiler, R. (2017). An integrative perspective on interpersonal coordination in interactive team sports. Front. Psychol. 8, 01440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01440
Tamminen, K. A., and Crocker, P. R. (2013). “I control my own emotions for the sake of the team”: emotional self-regulation and interpersonal emotion regulation among female high-performance curlers. Psychol. Sport Exer. 14, 737–747. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.05.002
Tenenbaum, G., Basevitch, I., Gershgoren, L., and Filho, E. (2013). Emotions–decision-making in sport: theoretical conceptualization and experimental evidence. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 11, 151–168. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2013.773687
Van Hoorebeke, D. (2006). La contagion émotionnelle: Problème ou ressource pour les relations interpersonnelles dans l'organisation? Human. Entrepr. 279, 1–18.
Keywords: decision-making, intuition, recognition, expertise, sports
Citation: Bossard C, Kérivel T, Dugény S, Bagot P, Fontaine T and Kermarrec G (2022) Naturalistic Decision-Making in Sport: How Current Advances Into Recognition Primed Decision Model Offer Insights for Future Research in Sport Settings? Front. Psychol. 13:936140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.936140
Received: 04 May 2022; Accepted: 31 May 2022;
Published: 20 June 2022.
Edited by:
Sergio Machado, Federal University of Santa Maria, BrazilReviewed by:
Giancarlo Condello, University of Parma, ItalyCopyright © 2022 Bossard, Kérivel, Dugény, Bagot, Fontaine and Kermarrec. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Cyril Bossard, cyril.bossard@univ-brest.fr