- School of Business, Central South University, Changsha, China
Hoarding behavior may not only interfere with hoarders’ daily lives but may also endanger the community. However, few studies have investigated the role of personality characteristics in hoarding behavior. We hypothesized that dispositional mindfulness would be negatively associated with hoarding behavior, and tested mechanisms and gender differences in this association. An online survey was conducted in a sample of 533 Chinese adults (262 women, Mage = 26.82; SD = 6.30). Regression-based analyses showed that mindfulness was associated with less hoarding behavior through higher self-esteem and lower emotion dysregulation. Moreover, gender moderated the mediating effect of emotion dysregulation in the association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior. Specifically, the indirect association was only significant for women. These findings provide a deeper understanding of how, why, and for whom dispositional mindfulness is negatively associated with hoarding behavior, they provide support for self-completion theory and the cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding, and they have heuristic value for future research.
Introduction
Hoarding behavior refers to acquiring but failing to use or discard numerous seemingly useless objects, what has been called “pathological collecting” (Frost and Gross, 1993; Frost et al., 2009). Hoarding behavior is relatively common in the population, with about 2.3∼5.8% of people showing significant problems (Samuels et al., 2008; Turna et al., 2018; Liu et al., 2021). Hoarding can interfere with daily life in the form of compulsive buying (Frost et al., 2011), social fears (Samuels et al., 2002), and depression symptoms (Raines et al., 2016). It may also endanger the surrounding community by increasing the risk of fires caused by clutter, injuries from falls, and the spread of bacteria (e.g., McGuire et al., 2013; Orr et al., 2019). Therefore, it is necessary to explore the antecedents of hoarding behavior, and develop effective methods to manage it.
Several studies have identified factors that may increase the risk of hoarding. For instance, anthropomorphism was found to be positively related to hoarding behavior (Burgess et al., 2018). Attachment anxiety was also positively associated with hoarding behavior via intolerance of uncertainty and experiential avoidance (Jia et al., 2019). Meanwhile, several studies have focused on the inhibitors of hoarding. Besides social support (Medard and Kellett, 2014) and social trust (Weiss et al., 2020), the attention facet of mindfulness skills has also been found to be negatively associated with compulsive hoarding symptoms for men who were on quarantine during the pandemic (Marazziti et al., 2021).
Mindfulness refers to an awareness and focus on the moment-to-moment experience in an open, non-reactive, and non-judgmental manner (Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Mindfulness can be enhanced through various interventions, such as meditation and mindfulness training (Kabat-Zinn, 2003). However, mindfulness can also be conceptualized as a trait or disposition. Dispositional mindfulness is an individual’s ability and tendency to maintain a state of mindfulness over time (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Brown et al., 2007). It has been suggested that almost all people have the ability to be mindful, but there are still individual differences in dispositional mindfulness (Pepping and Duvenage, 2016). Individuals with higher dispositional mindfulness have a clear awareness of what is actually happening in the moment, and therefore make conscious decisions and show less reflexive and impulsive behavior (Brown and Ryan, 2003). Although a recent study found the attention facet of mindfulness skills was negatively associated with compulsive hoarding symptoms (Marazziti et al., 2021), the relationship between individual differences in dispositional mindfulness and hoarding behavior, as well as the mechanisms underlying this relationship, have not yet been tested.
Moreover, many studies have always focused on gender differences in the mindfulness effects (e.g., Rojiani et al., 2017; Brem et al., 2019; Kingery et al., 2021). A neuroscience study demonstrated that men and women may benefit from different components of meditation to achieve similar effects (Luders et al., 2015). Other research also noted that different genders may benefit from mindfulness through various processes and at different levels (Katz and Toner, 2013). It is important to determine the effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior by gender and how it works among men and women in our study. Thus, our study tested whether, why, and for whom dispositional mindfulness and hoarding behavior might be negatively correlated.
Theoretical framework and hypotheses
Mindfulness and hoarding behavior
Hoarding is characterized by excessive acquisition and difficulty in discarding large numbers of seemingly useless objects (Greenberg et al., 1990; Frost and Gross, 1993). Previous research found that mindfulness was positively associated with self-control (e.g., Bowlin and Baer, 2012; Canby et al., 2015; Li et al., 2021), which was negatively linked with the tendency to save items, as well as difficulty in discarding them (Carbonella and Timpano, 2016).
Moreover, individuals with higher mindfulness focus not only on their internal processes but also on their surroundings (Shapiro et al., 2006). They live in the present moment so that they are not overwhelmed and guided by thoughts, emotions, and feelings in their actions and choices (Shapiro et al., 2006). Individuals with higher mindfulness may be more likely to make reasonable and non-emotional decisions (Shapiro et al., 2012), and therefore may be less likely to engage in hoarding behaviors.
Recent investigations have included mindfulness as a component of an intervention for obsessive-compulsive symptoms (e.g., Patel et al., 2007; Sugiura and Sugiura, 2015; Apaolaza et al., 2019), and one of the symptom dimensions is hoarding disorder. Moreover, previous research found that individuals with compulsive hoarding symptoms scored lower in the attention facet of mindfulness skills than those without such symptoms (Marazziti et al., 2021). These studies jointly suggest the negative association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior.
The mediating role of self-esteem
Self-esteem was expected to act as a mediator of the association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior in the current study. Self-esteem involves a positive or negative orientation toward the self (Rosenberg, 1965), reflecting a sense of self-acceptance and self-worth. Several studies have demonstrated a correlation between mindfulness and higher self-esteem (e.g., Park and Dhandra, 2017; Dhandra, 2020; Bharti et al., 2022). Mindfulness-based interventions have also been shown to significantly improve self-esteem (e.g., Ree and Craigie, 2007; Goldin et al., 2009). Mindfulness enables individuals to show non-judgment, make choices that are non-reactive, and be open to their experiences (Baer et al., 2006). In turn, mindful individuals are thought to be less judgmental and critical of themselves (Hildebrandt et al., 2017). Moreover, individuals with higher mindfulness are able to pay attention to current experiences rather than negative self-related thoughts, and let go of negative cognitions (Frewen et al., 2008). Thus, it is reasonable to propose a positive relationship between mindfulness and self-esteem.
Individuals with low self-esteem maintain a negative self-concept (Bharti et al., 2022). According to self-completion theory (Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1982), individuals with negative self-concepts may seek ways to build their self-worth (Banister and Hogg, 2004). Possessions are perceived to be extensions of the self and significant others (Claes et al., 2016; Kings et al., 2021), which has been found to be positively correlated with hoarding symptoms and beliefs (Kings et al., 2021). Hoarders may believe that owning objects is more important than using them, and discarding these objects makes them feel like they have lost part of their identity (Foster et al., 2007). That is, individuals may engage in hoarding behaviors to get a sense of self-identity and establish their self-worth (Kings et al., 2017, 2021). Thus, it is reasonable to infer the negative relationship between self-esteem and hoarding behavior.
Together, self-completion theory (Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1982), along with these studies, suggests that mindfulness may be negatively associated with hoarding behavior via an increase in self-esteem. Specifically, individuals with higher mindfulness may be able to reduce their judgmental and critical attitudes toward themselves and therefore show higher self-esteem (e.g., Park and Dhandra, 2017; Dhandra, 2020; Bharti et al., 2022), with higher self-esteem, they become less likely to build their self-worth by hoarding.
The mediating role of emotion dysregulation
Emotion dysregulation may act as another mediator of the association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior. Emotion dysregulation involves an impaired ability to experience, express, and use emotions (Eisenberg et al., 2000). Previous research showed a correlation between mindfulness and lower emotion dysregulation (e.g., Luberto et al., 2014; Cheung et al., 2020; Powers et al., 2021). Moreover, after 6 weeks of training focused meditation (FM), healthy college students reported significantly lower scores in emotion dysregulation (Menezes and Bizarro, 2015). As mindfulness involves being open and accepting emotions, rather than ruminating or avoiding these experiences (Shapiro et al., 2006), individuals with higher mindfulness may have lower emotion dysregulation.
Emotions have been shown to be important in understanding hoarding. Hoarding is characterized by emotionally reinforced saving behaviors, as hoarders generally have strong emotional attachments to numerous objects (e.g., Mathes et al., 2020; Yap and Grisham, 2021). The cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding (Frost and Hartl, 1996) suggests that emotions play a significant role in hoarding onset and maintenance. Specifically, negative emotions are associated with avoidance (e.g., refusal to discard), whereas positive emotions are associated with approach behaviors (e.g., acquisition) (Frost and Hartl, 1996; Mathes et al., 2020). To avoid the negative emotions associated with discarding, hoarders will refuse to discard objects, and therefore saving is reinforced and maintained (Taylor et al., 2018). Meanwhile, to keep the positive emotions associated with acquiring new possessions and finding valuable possessions through clutter, hoarders may engage in more acquisitions and savings (e.g., Frost and Hartl, 1996; Steketee and Frost, 2003; Taylor et al., 2018).
As hoarders often acquire items to regulate both positive and negative emotions (Tolin, 2011), and difficulties in emotion regulation have been found to be associated with more hoarding symptoms (e.g., Fernandez de la Cruz et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2018; Tolin et al., 2018). The cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding (Frost and Hartl, 1996), along with these studies, suggests that individuals with higher levels of mindfulness may have less emotion dysregulation, which in turn may reduce the likelihood that they will engage in hoarding behaviors.
The sequential mediating role of self-esteem and emotion dysregulation
The relationship between mindfulness and hoarding behavior may be serially mediated by self-esteem and emotion dysregulation. Individuals with higher mindfulness are less judgmental and critical of themselves, and therefore show higher self-esteem (e.g., Park and Dhandra, 2017; Dhandra, 2020; Bharti et al., 2022). Moreover, individuals with higher self-esteem could be better able to cope with stress (Perlin et al., 1981), and to experience less distress and depression (Rosenberg et al., 1989). The negative association between self-esteem and emotion dysregulation has been established in previous empirical studies (e.g., Garofalo et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2017). Low self-esteem involves negative self-evaluation beliefs (Smith et al., 2006); strong negative emotional reactions to unfavorable self-evaluations may in turn disrupt individuals’ ability to regulate emotions (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2014). Thus, hoarding (e.g., saving possessions) may be an attempt to regulate emotions related to identity issues (e.g., Frost and Hartl, 1996; Morrison and Johnson, 2011; Tolin, 2011). This logic, along with previous findings, suggests the possibility of a serial mediation effect linking mindfulness and hoarding behavior.
In brief, individuals with higher mindfulness show higher self-esteem (e.g., Park and Dhandra, 2017; Dhandra, 2020; Bharti et al., 2022), with higher self-esteem, they may have lower emotion dysregulation (e.g., Garofalo et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2017) and ultimately, less hoarding behaviors (e.g., Fernandez de la Cruz et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2018; Tolin et al., 2018).
Gender differences: The moderating role of gender
It is possible that the mediation processes of interest differ for men and women. One consideration is that there appear to be gender differences in each of the key variables in our study. Table 1 lists studies that have found gender differences in mindfulness, self-esteem, emotion dysregulation, and hoarding. Each entry includes information about the sample and outcomes. With few exceptions, men had higher scores for mindfulness (e.g., Dhandra and Park, 2018; Amemiya and Sakairi, 2020; Webb et al., 2021) and self-esteem (e.g., Bleidorn et al., 2016; Rentzsch et al., 2016; Magee and Upenieks, 2019), and women had higher scores for emotion dysregulation (e.g., Barrett et al., 2000; Lafrance Robinson et al., 2014; Wieckowski et al., 2020) and hoarding (e.g., Neave et al., 2016; Tripathi et al., 2018; Turna et al., 2018).
Table 1. Summary of studies reviewed in the gender differences for mindfulness, self-esteem, emotion dysregulation, and hoarding.
In tests of mediation, the question is whether these gender differences affect the strength or direction of one or more of the paths in the model, constituting moderated mediation. In the current study, we tested whether gender moderated the first path of each mediated pathway. First, gender may moderate the effect of mindfulness on self-esteem. This focus is justified because gender differences have been identified both in regard to self-esteem (e.g., Bleidorn et al., 2016; Magee and Upenieks, 2019) and in regard to the effect of mindfulness on self-compassion (e.g., Rojiani et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2018). As a complimentary self-concept to self-esteem, self-compassion refers to being accepting and caring of oneself (Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Neff, 2003; Hwang et al., 2016), and individuals with higher self-compassion generally show higher self-esteem (Neff, 2011). The positive effect of mindfulness on self-compassion has been shown to be stronger in women than in men (e.g., Rojiani et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2018). These findings suggest that the effect of mindfulness on self-esteem may be stronger in women than in men. Moreover, individuals with higher self-esteem become less likely to build their self-worth by hoarding, which may in turn make the negative relationship between mindfulness and hoarding behavior different for men and women.
Second, gender may moderate the effect of mindfulness on the other mediator in our model, namely emotion dysregulation. The evidence here is based on the assumption that rumination in response to negative moods can be considered a form of emotion dysregulation (Zawadzki, 2015). Rumination has been found to have stronger relationships with psychological health, namely depression, than other emotion regulation strategies (Zawadzki, 2015). Previous research found that women ruminate more in response to depressive moods, thus exacerbating the moods (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999). Moreover, mindfulness was found in general to be associated with less rumination, and the association was stronger in women than in men (Kingery et al., 2021). Thus, it is reasonable to infer that the effect of mindfulness on decreasing emotion dysregulation may be stronger in women than in men. Furthermore, as for the positive correlation between emotion dysregulation and hoarding (e.g., Fernandez de la Cruz et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2018; Tolin et al., 2018), the gender difference of mindfulness in decreasing emotion dysregulation may then spill over into hoarding behavior.
The present study
Guided by self-completion theory (Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1982) and the cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding (Frost and Hartl, 1996), we administered online questionnaires to a community sample of adults to test two parallel mediation processes (self-esteem as the mediator; emotion dysregulation as the mediator) and a sequential mediation process (self-esteem and emotion dysregulation as sequential mediators). Gender was tested as a moderator of the mediating pathways. Together, these tests constitute a test of a moderated mediation model. See Figure 1. We made the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1. Mindfulness will be negatively associated with hoarding behavior.
Hypothesis 2. Mindfulness will be negatively associated with hoarding behavior via an increase in self-esteem.
Hypothesis 3. Mindfulness will be negatively associated with hoarding behavior via a decrease in emotion dysregulation.
Hypothesis 4. Mindfulness will be negatively associated with hoarding behavior via a sequence of increased self-esteem and decreased emotion dysregulation.
Hypothesis 5. Gender will moderate the mediating effect of self-esteem in the association between mindfulness and lower hoarding behavior. Specifically, the indirect association will be stronger in women than in men.
Hypothesis 6. Gender will moderate the mediating effect of emotion dysregulation in the association between mindfulness and lower hoarding behavior. Specifically, the indirect association will be stronger in women than in men.
Materials and methods
Participants
The study employed a cross-sectional design. The inclusion criteria were the age of 18 years old or older. To ensure the validity of our data, we remove the questionnaires where (a) duplicate IP addresses, (b) incorrect responses to the screening items, (c) answering the questions in less than 120 s, (d) marks of answers of items show regular shapes (such as zigzag patterns) (e.g., Wills et al., 2008; Bao et al., 2015, 2016). There were 533 Chinese adults (262 women, Mage = 26.82; SD = 6.30) who provided valid reports, resulting in a valid response rate of about 70%. These participants came from 29 provinces or municipalities in China, with the smallest number from Ningxia Province (N = 1, 0.19% of the total sample) and the largest number from Guangdong Province (N = 66, 12.38% of the total sample). Table 2 provides descriptive statistics, demographic information, and gender differences on the study variables. As shown in Table 2, most participants were pursuing or had a bachelor’s degree (59.47%). Their incomes were diverse, with 28.89% earning a monthly income from 5,001 to 8,000 RMB (about 792–1,266 USD), followed by 27.95% with a monthly income from 2,000 to 5,000 RMB (about 317–792 USD). There was no gender difference on age (t (531) = 0.50, p > 0.05), mindfulness (t (531) = 0.45, p > 0.05), self-esteem (t (531) = 1.23, p > 0.05), emotion dysregulation (t (517) = −1.79, p > 0.05), or hoarding behavior (t (498) = −1.95, p > 0.05). Women had a higher education level than men (Z = −3.34, p < 0.001), and men had a higher income level than women (Z = −2.98, p < 0.01).
Procedure
The study was approved by the research ethics committee at the corresponding author’s institution. We collected online data for adults in China from October 1 to 3, 2021. Specifically, we produced and distributed the questionnaire through the online survey tool Questionnaire Star. Potential participants were informed about the study through advertisements on the authors’ WeChat Moments and a professional questionnaire collection agency in China. Moreover, the participants were told that when they finished the questionnaires they could, if they wished, forward the questionnaire link to others. When participants clicked on the survey hyperlink, they saw an information sheet that clearly explained the purpose and procedures of the study, the fact that participation was voluntary and they had the right to withdraw at any time, and that no personally identifiable information was collected. Participants provided their informed consent before completing the questionnaires. Each participant was given a small payment for participating after the investigation to thank them for their time and effort.
Measures
All the instruments we used in our research were well-developed scales used in previously published studies. We used the translation and back-translation method to translate these scales into Chinese.
Dispositional mindfulness
Dispositional mindfulness was assessed with the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R) (Feldman et al., 2007). The scale includes 12 items on four dimensions: attention, present focus, awareness, and acceptance. A sample item is “I am able to focus on the present moment.” The 12 items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = rarely/not at all; 4 = almost always). After reverse-coding of negatively worded items, responses to all items were averaged, with higher scores indicating higher mindfulness. The Cronbach’s α of mindfulness for this sample was 0.72. After controlling the reverse wording effect, the results of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed acceptable structural validity: χ2/df = 2.64; CFI = 0.94; TLI = 0.92; IFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.06.
Self-esteem
Self-esteem was assessed with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) (Rosenberg, 1965), a ten-item unidimensional scale. A sample is “I take a positive attitude toward myself.” One reversed item “I wish I could have more respect for myself” was excluded for cultural differences (Tian, 2006). The nine remaining items are rated on a six-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 6 = strongly agree). After reverse-coding of negatively worded items, responses to all items were averaged, with higher scores indicating higher self-esteem. The Cronbach’s α of self-esteem for this sample was 0.91. After controlling the reverse wording effect, the results of CFA showed acceptable structural validity: χ2/df = 3.56; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.97; IFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.07.
Emotion dysregulation
Emotion dysregulation in the current study was measured with the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Bjureberg et al., 2016). The scale includes 16 items and is structured by five dimensions: lack of emotional clarity, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, limited access to effective emotion regulation strategies, and non-acceptance of emotional responses. A sample is “When I am upset, I have difficulty controlling my behaviors.” The 16 items are rated on a five-point Likert scale (1 = almost never; 5 = almost always). A higher average score of 16 ratings reflected higher emotion dysregulation. The Cronbach’s α of emotion dysregulation for this sample was 0.96. CFA of the DERS showed acceptable structural validity: χ2/df = 2.98; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.96; IFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.06.
Hoarding behavior
Hoarding behavior was measured with the Saving Inventory-Revised scale (SI-R) (Frost et al., 2004). The scale includes 23 items and is structured by three dimensions: clutter, difficulty discarding, and acquisition. A sample is “To what extent do you have difficulty throwing things away?” The 23 items are rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 = not at all; 4 = very often/extreme). Responses to all items were averaged, with higher scores indicating more hoarding behaviors. The Cronbach’s α of hoarding behavior for this sample was 0.94. CFA of the SI-R showed acceptable structural validity: χ2/df = 3.07; CFI = 0.94; TLI = 0.93; IFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.06.
Control variables
Control variables were age, education level, and income level. Previous research showed that younger people exhibited stronger hoarding cognition and behaviors (Neave et al., 2015). Education level has been found to be positively and significantly related to self-esteem (de Araujo and Lagos, 2013). Education level has also been found to be related to hoarding, with the participants who have a college or professional school degree scoring significantly higher than the other participants (Danet, 2018). Moreover, income level has been found to be positively related to self-esteem and hoarding (Samuels et al., 2008; de Araujo and Lagos, 2013). Therefore, we selected age, education level, and income level as statistical control variables.
Results
All the hypotheses were tested with the SPSS PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013), a computational tool for testing moderation and mediation effects as well as their combinations. Models 6 and 84 of the SPSS PROCESS macro were employed to test the hypothesized model. Moreover, the bootstrapping method was conducted to test the indirect effects (5,000 bootstrap samples). Indirect effects are considered significant when their confidence interval does not include 0.
Preliminary analysis
Pearson correlations among mindfulness, self-esteem, emotion dysregulation, and hoarding behavior are presented in Table 3 for men and women, respectively. There were significant correlations among all study variables.
Testing for the main effect and mediation effects
The analysis results of SPSS PROCESS macro are presented in Figure 2 and Table 4. Mindfulness was negatively associated with hoarding behavior (B = −0.77, p < 0.001), supporting Hypothesis 1. Moreover, the total effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior (effect = −0.77, 95% CI = [−0.93, −0.61]) was negative and significant, but the direct effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior (effect = 0.08, 95% CI = [−0.08, 0.23]) was not significant.
Figure 2. Regression results of the moderated multi-mediation model. The model is assessed using PROCESS macro (MODEL6 and MODEL84). Dotted line represents the direct effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior; the effects of age, education level, and income level are controlled; ns, non-significant; **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Table 4 (Equations 2 and 4) shows that mindfulness was positively associated with self-esteem (B = 1.63, p < 0.001), and self-esteem was negatively associated with hoarding behavior (B = −0.12, p < 0.001). Moreover, Table 4 (Equations 3 and 4) shows that mindfulness was negatively associated with emotion dysregulation (B = −0.36, p < 0.001), and emotion dysregulation was positively associated with hoarding behavior (B = 0.57, p < 0.001). Meanwhile, Table 4 (Equations 3) shows that self-esteem was negatively associated with emotion dysregulation (B = −0.46, p < 0.001).
In addition, Table 5 shows that the simple and serial mediation effects of self-esteem and emotion dysregulation were statistically significant (mindfulness→self-esteem→hoarding behavior: effect = −0.20, 95% CI = [−0.33, −0.07]; mindfulness→emotion dysregulation→hoarding behavior: effect = −0.22, 95% CI = [−0.34, −0.10]; mindfulness→self-esteem→emotion dysregulation→hoarding behavior: effect = −0.43, 95% CI = [−0.55, −0.32]). Thus, two parallel mediation paths and the sequential mediation path of self-esteem and emotion dysregulation were identified, supporting Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4.
Testing for the moderated mediation effect
Table 4 shows the results of tests of moderated mediation. Table 4 (Equation 2) shows that the interaction of mindfulness and gender was not significantly related to self-esteem (B = −0.04, p > 0.05). Hence, Hypothesis 5 was not supported.
As for Hypothesis 6, Table 4 (Equation 3) shows that the interaction of mindfulness and gender was positively associated with emotion dysregulation (B = 0.45, p < 0.01). Figure 3 illustrates the moderating effect of gender on the relationship between mindfulness and emotion dysregulation. Simple slopes tests revealed that the effect of mindfulness on emotion dysregulation was significant for women (Bsimple = −0.59, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.84, −0.35]), but not significant for men (Bsimple = −0.14, p > 0.05, 95% CI = [−0.40, 0.13]). Meanwhile, the mediation effect of emotion dysregulation was significant for women (effect = −0.34, 95% CI = [−0.48, −0.19]), but not significant for men (effect = −0.08, 95% CI = [−0.24, 0.08]). Taken together, the results support Hypothesis 6.
Figure 3. Moderating effect of gender on the relationship between mindfulness and emotion dysregulation.
Discussion
Our results showed that individuals with higher dispositional mindfulness had reported less hoarding behaviors. Specifically, individuals with higher mindfulness had higher self-esteem, which in turn was associated with lower emotion dysregulation, and ultimately less hoarding. Moreover, tests of gender differences showed that the effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior through decreased emotion dysregulation was only significant in women, but there was no gender difference in the effect of mindfulness on hoarding behavior through self-esteem.
Sociocultural factors might partly explain why gender did not moderate the mediating effect of self-esteem in our sample. A sociocultural perspective holds that gender differences in self-esteem are largely governed by social influences that vary by context and culture (Kling et al., 1999; Orth and Robins, 2014). In many Asian countries, such as China, there are small gender differences in self-esteem, as people are more likely to engage in within-gender social comparisons that reduce self-stereotyping processes (Guimond et al., 2007; Bleidorn et al., 2016). Chinese culture is described as more collectivistic, and gender differences in self-esteem have been found relatively less in this cultural context (Bleidorn et al., 2016). Moreover, Buddhism, a religion that has existed in China for thousands of years, advocates reacting mindfully to maladaptive feelings, and being kinder and more accepting of oneself (David et al., 2013). It appears that the philosophy of Buddhism, and particularly the emphasis placed on mindfulness (Kuan, 2008; Jones-Smith, 2012), may influence Chinese people in a subtle way. Recent research showed that there was no significant gender difference in the extent to which mindfulness was associated with self-esteem in a Chinese sample (Wang and Kong, 2019). These studies might help to explain why there was no gender difference in the positive association between mindfulness and self-esteem in our sample. Mindfulness may be an important source of self-esteem for both men and women.
Theoretical implications
The theoretical implications of the current study’s results include three aspects. First, the finding that mindfulness was negatively associated with hoarding behavior is in accordance with another recent study. Marazziti et al. (2021) found that the ability to regulate attention can protect from hoarding and play the role of a psychological factor associated with lower hoarding. Unlike the socio-psychological antecedents of hoarding that have received a lot of attention from researchers (e.g., Wheaton et al., 2011; Jia et al., 2019), the current study enriches and extended prior literature on the role of dispositional mindfulness in hoarding behavior.
The second notable contribution of the present study is that it sheds light on why dispositional mindfulness could be negatively associated with hoarding behavior, employing self-completion theory (Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1982) and the cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding (Frost and Hartl, 1996). The results demonstrated the parallel and serial mediation effects of self-esteem and emotion dysregulation in the relationship between mindfulness and hoarding behavior. These findings suggest that mindfulness can help achieve a “double dividend” in decreasing hoarding behavior. Although previous studies showed the potential negative influence of mindfulness on hoarding behavior (Marazziti et al., 2021), our findings identify the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and hoarding behavior from a new perspective, and reveal the “black-box” of how this relationship works.
Finally, the present study provides a better understanding of the association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior by revealing its gender differences. The relationship between mindfulness and hoarding behavior through decreased emotion dysregulation was only significant in women. This finding is a response to the call for more research on gender differences in the mindfulness effects (Luders et al., 2015), provides new support to previous studies showing that the mindfulness effect is stronger in women than in men (e.g., Rojiani et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2018), and enriches research on gender differences in mindfulness and hoarding (e.g., Turna et al., 2018; Amemiya and Sakairi, 2020).
Practical implications
The results also have potential applied value. Our results suggest that living and behaving more mindfully may benefit individuals in having less hoarding behaviors. Promoting mindfulness broadly may reduce hoarding behavior in non-clinical samples. Given the limitations of the study, any therapeutic implications we can offer are tentative, we await further clinical replication before strong treatment recommendations can be made. Our findings suggest decreasing hoarding by fostering mindfulness may be a beneficial focus for future intervention. Randomized controlled trials would provide the strongest evidence of the effectiveness of mindfulness-based interventions to decrease hoarding. It is possible that this type of intervention could be effective not just in reducing hoarding, but also in influencing the contributing factors of self-esteem and low emotion dysregulation. Moreover, we found an important gender difference that can be taken into account in future interventions. Our findings showed that the indirect association between mindfulness and hoarding behavior via a decrease in emotion dysregulation was only significant in women. Interventions to reduce emotion dysregulation and hoarding could be tailored separately for men and women.
Limitations and future directions
Our study has several limitations that can be addressed in future research. First, limited by the cross-sectional design, we cannot make causal inferences about the relations among mindfulness, self-esteem, emotion dysregulation, and hoarding behavior. In our research, prior experimental/longitudinal studies (e.g., Ree and Craigie, 2007; Goldin et al., 2009; Menezes and Bizarro, 2015; Cheung et al., 2020), the self-completion theory (Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1982), and the cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding (Frost and Hartl, 1996) are helpful in conceptualizing possible causal links. Still, it is problematic to use a cross-sectional design to study mediation effects as estimates may be biased and may not be replicated in longitudinal research (Maxwell et al., 2011). Longitudinal or experimental designs should be conducted in the future to verify causality and demonstrate the predictive power of the mediation model.
Second, the present study was not conducted with a clinical sample, so the findings should be applied with caution to clinically hoarding adults. Although a community sample might provide useful information about hoarding in a natural context as hoarders do not necessarily come to the attention of mental health providers (Büscher et al., 2013), the study should be repeated in a clinical sample to assess the clinical generalizability of the relationships and to explore if more severely hoarding adults would benefit from mindfulness.
Third, we did not measure whether participants have attention problems, a factor that has been shown to influence hoarding (e.g., Hartl et al., 2005; Hallion et al., 2015). Neither did we measure participants’ levels of self-compassion, a factor that was related to participants’ mindfulness (e.g., Rojiani et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2018). Future research should measure these factors as control variables and rule out these factors as alternative explanations.
Fourth, we were interested in hoarding generally and therefore did not analyze the relationship between mindfulness and each aspect of hoarding (clutter, difficulty discarding, and acquisition). Although this total score has been often used (e.g., Neave et al., 2015, 2016), some studies suggest that it is more necessary to look at each aspect separately (e.g., Timpano et al., 2011; Ayers et al., 2014). It is possible that mindfulness is more strongly associated with compulsive acquisition than other aspects of hoarding, as the beneficial effects of mindfulness in reducing impulsive or compulsive buying have been well established (e.g., Park and Dhandra, 2017; Dhandra, 2020; Brunelle and Grossman, 2022). Individuals with higher levels of mindfulness are more satisfied with the possessions they already have, they have less interest in consumerist messages and aspirations for more (Brown et al., 2009). Thus, further investigation is needed to comprehensively understand how mindfulness relates to various aspects of hoarding and whether there are significant differences in the relationship between mindfulness and different aspects of hoarding.
Although not a limitation, it should be noted that the study was carried out in China and so the generalizability of the results needs to be tested. Buddhism is a part of Chinese culture, and its devotees emphasize mindfulness meditation (Xu et al., 2014). Moreover, the self-control associated with hoarding could be affected by the individualistic or collectivistic cultural orientation. Greater importance is placed on self-control in more collectivistic societies (e.g., China) than in individualistic societies (e.g., the United States) (Timpano et al., 2015). Thus, future research could consider studying the links between mindfulness and hoarding behavior in different cultural contexts.
Conclusion
The current study examined “how” and “why” dispositional mindfulness and hoarding were negatively correlated. Through an online questionnaire survey to a community sample of 533 adults in China, we explored the effect of dispositional mindfulness on hoarding behavior, and tested mechanisms and gender differences in this association. Specifically, mindfulness was negatively associated with hoarding behavior through the parallel and sequential mediating pathways of increased self-esteem and decreased emotion dysregulation. Moreover, the indirect effect of mindfulness and hoarding behavior through decreased emotion dysregulation was only significant in women. The results of the study shed light on how self-esteem and emotion dysregulation are associated with the process of the mindfulness-hoarding relationship, contribute to the existing work on gender differences in mindfulness and hoarding behavior, and have heuristic value for future research.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Central South University Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
YG contributed to conception and design of the study, assisted with the execution of the study and data collection, and provided critical revisions. YT contributed to conception and design of the study, executed the study, analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript. RH assisted with the data collection and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript reversion, read and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The present research was supported by the Project of the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos: 72072185 and 71672195).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Amemiya, R., and Sakairi, Y. (2020). The role of self-compassion in athlete mindfulness and burnout: Examination of the effects of gender differences. Pers. Individ. Differ. 166:110167. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110167
Apaolaza, V., Hartmann, P., D’Souza, C., and Gilsanz, A. (2019). Mindfulness, compulsive mobile social media use, and derived stress: The mediating roles of self-esteem and social anxiety. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 22, 388–396. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0681
Ayers, C. R., Castriotta, N., Dozier, M. E., Espejo, E. P., and Porter, B. (2014). Behavioral and experiential avoidance in patients with hoarding disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 45, 408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.04.005
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504
Banister, E. N., and Hogg, M. K. (2004). Negative symbolic consumption and consumers’ drive for self-esteem: The case of the fashion industry. Eur. J. Mark. 38, 850–868. doi: 10.1108/03090560410539285
Bao, G., Xu, B., and Zhang, Z. (2016). Employees’ trust and their knowledge sharing and integration: The mediating roles of organizational identification and organization-based self-esteem. Knowl. Manage. Res. Pract. 14, 362–375. doi: 10.1057/kmrp.2015.1
Bao, G., Zhang, Z., and Chen, J. (2015). The mediation of in-group identification between collectivism and knowledge sharing. Innovation 17, 341–363. doi: 10.1080/14479338.2015.1054605
Barrett, L. F., Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., and Schwartz, G. E. (2000). Sex differences in emotional awareness. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1027–1035. doi: 10.1177/01461672002611001
Bharti, M., Suneja, V., and Bharti, M. (2022). Mindfulness as an antidote to conspicuous consumption: The mediating roles of self-esteem, self-concept clarity and normative influence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 184:111215. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111215
Bjureberg, J., Ljótsson, B., Tull, M. T., Hedman, E., Sahlin, H., Lundh, L. G., et al. (2016). Development and validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: The DERS-16. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 38, 284–296. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9514-x
Bleidorn, W., Arslan, R. C., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Gebauer, J. E., Potter, J., et al. (2016). Age and gender differences in self-esteem—a cross-cultural window. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 396–410. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000078
Bowlin, S. L., and Baer, R. A. (2012). Relationships between mindfulness, self-control, and psychological functioning. Pers. Individ. Differ. 52, 411–415. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.050
Brem, M. J., Shorey, R. C., Anderson, S., and Stuart, G. L. (2019). Exploring gender differences in the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and compulsive sexual behavior among adults in residential substance use treatment. Mindfulness 10, 1592–1602. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01117-7
Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Linley, P. A., and Orzech, K. (2009). When what one has is enough: Mindfulness, financial desire discrepancy, and subjective well-being. J. Res. Pers. 43, 727–736. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.07.002
Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., and Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychol. Inq. 18, 211–237. doi: 10.1080/10478400701598298
Brunelle, C., and Grossman, H. (2022). Predictors of online compulsive buying: The role of personality and mindfulness. Pers. Individ. Differ. 185:111237. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111237
Burgess, A. M., Graves, L. M., and Frost, R. O. (2018). My possessions need me: Anthropomorphism and hoarding. Scand. J. Psychol. 59, 340–348. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12441
Büscher, T. P., Dyson, J., and Cowdell, F. (2013). The effects of hoarding disorder on families: An integrative review. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 21, 491–498. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12098
Canby, N. K., Cameron, I. M., Calhoun, A. T., and Buchanan, G. M. (2015). A brief mindfulness intervention for healthy college students and its effects on psychological distress, self-control, meta-mood, and subjective vitality. Mindfulness 6, 1071–1081. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0356-5
Carbonella, J. Y., and Timpano, K. R. (2016). Examining the link between hoarding symptoms and cognitive flexibility deficits. Behav. Ther. 47, 262–273. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.003
Cheung, R. Y., Ke, Z., and Ng, M. C. (2020). Dispositional mindfulness and mental health in Chinese emerging adults: A multilevel model with emotion dysregulation as a mediator. PLoS One 15:e0239575. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239575
Claes, L., Müller, A., and Luyckx, K. (2016). Compulsive buying and hoarding as identity substitutes: The role of materialistic value endorsement and depression. Compr. Psychiatry 68, 65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.04.005
Danet, D. M. (2018). Insecure attachment as a factor in hoarding behaviors in a non-clinical sample of women. Psychiatry Res. 270, 286–292. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.053
David, D., Lynn, S. J., and Das, L. S. (2013). “Self-acceptance in Buddhism and psychotherapy,” in The strength of self-acceptance, ed. M. Bernard (New York, NY: Springer), 19–38.
de Araujo, P., and Lagos, S. (2013). Self-esteem, education, and wages revisited. J. Econ. Psychol. 34, 120–132. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2012.12.001
Dhandra, T. K. (2020). Does self-esteem matter? A framework depicting role of self-esteem between dispositional mindfulness and impulsive buying. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 55:102135. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102135
Dhandra, T. K., and Park, H. J. (2018). Mindfulness and gender differences in ethical beliefs. Soc. Responsibil. J. 14, 274–286. doi: 10.1108/SRJ-05-2016-0067
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., and Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 136–157. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.136
Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., and Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 29, 177–190. doi: 10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8
Fernandez de la Cruz, L., Landau, D., Iervolino, A. C., Santo, S., Pertusa, A., Singh, S., et al. (2013). Experiential avoidance and emotion regulation difficulties in hoarding disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 27, 204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.01.004
Foster, J. D., Kernis, M. H., and Goldman, B. M. (2007). Linking adult attachment to self-esteem stability. Self Identity 6, 64–73. doi: 10.1080/15298860600832139
Frewen, P. A., Evans, E. M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D. J., and Partridge, K. (2008). Letting go: Mindfulness and negative automatic thinking. Cogn. Ther. Res. 32, 758–774. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9142-1
Frost, R. O., and Gross, R. C. (1993). The hoarding of possessions. Behav. Res. Ther. 31, 367–381. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90094-B
Frost, R. O., and Hartl, T. L. (1996). A cognitive-behavioral model of compulsive hoarding. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 341–350. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00071-2
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., and Grisham, J. (2004). Measurement of compulsive hoarding: Saving inventory-revised. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 1163–1182. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.07.006
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., and Tolin, D. F. (2011). Comorbidity in hoarding disorder. Depress. Anxiety 28, 876–884. doi: 10.1176/appi.focus.130218
Frost, R. O., Tolin, D. F., Steketee, G., Fitch, K. E., and Selbo-Bruns, A. (2009). Excessive acquisition in hoarding. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 632–639. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.013
Garofalo, C., Holden, C. J., Zeigler-Hill, V., and Velotti, P. (2016). Understanding the connection between self-esteem and aggression: The mediating role of emotion dysregulation. Aggress. Behav. 42, 3–15. doi: 10.1002/ab.21601
Gilbert, D., and Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors. Mindfulness 1, 227–234. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0032-3
Goldin, P., Ramel, W., and Gross, J. (2009). Mindfulness meditation training and self-referential processing in social anxiety disorder: Behavioral and neural effects. J. Cogn. Psychother. 23, 242–256. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.3.242
Gratz, K. L., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
Greenberg, D., Witzum, E., and Levy, A. (1990). Hoarding as a psychiatric symptom. J. Clin. Psychiatry 51, 417–421.
Guimond, S., Branscombe, N. R., Brunot, S., Buunk, A. P., Chatard, A., Désert, M., et al. (2007). Culture, gender, and the self: Variations and impact of social comparison processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 1118–1134. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1118
Hallion, L. S., Diefenbach, G. J., and Tolin, D. F. (2015). Poor memory confidence mediates the association between inattention symptoms and hoarding severity and impairment. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 7, 43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.10.003
Hartl, T. L., Duffany, S. R., Allen, G. J., Steketee, G., and Frost, R. O. (2005). Relationships among compulsive hoarding, trauma, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.002
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hildebrandt, L. K., Mccall, C., and Singer, T. (2017). Differential effects of attention-, compassion-, and socio-cognitively based mental practices on self-reports of mindfulness and compassion. Mindfulness 8, 1488–1512. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0716-z
Hwang, S., Kim, G., Yang, J. W., and Yang, E. (2016). The moderating effects of age on the relationships of self-compassion, self-esteem, and mental health. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 58, 194–205. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12109
Jia, Y.-R., Zhang, S.-C., Jiin, Y.-L., Zhang, L., Zhao, S.-Q., and Li, Q. (2019). Effect of attachment anxiety on hoarding behavior of college students: Mediating role of intolerance of uncertainty and experiential avoidence. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 35, 393–400. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.04.02
Jones-Smith, E. (2012). Theories of counseling and psychotherapy: An integrative approach. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Josefsson, T., Larsman, P., Broberg, A. G., and Lundh, L. G. (2011). Self-reported mindfulness mediates the relation between meditation experience and psychological well-being. Mindfulness 2, 49–58. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0042-9
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 144–156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Kang, Y., Rahrig, H., Eichel, K., Niles, H. F., Rocha, T., Lepp, N. E., et al. (2018). Gender differences in response to a school-based mindfulness training intervention for early adolescents. J. Sch. Psychol. 68, 163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2018.03.004
Katz, D., and Toner, B. (2013). A systematic review of gender differences in the effectiveness of mindfulness-based treatments for substance use disorders. Mindfulness 4, 318–331. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0132-3
Kingery, J. N., Bodenlos, J. S., Schneider, T. I., Peltz, J. S., and Sindoni, M. W. (2021). Dispositional mindfulness predicting psychological adjustment among college students: The role of rumination and gender. J. Am. Coll. Health 1–12. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2021.1943411
Kings, C., Moulding, R., Yap, K., Gazzola, R., and Knight, T. (2021). Measuring possessions as extensions of self and links to significant others in hoarding: The possessions as others and self inventory. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 43, 441–453. doi: 10.1007/s10862-020-09858-9
Kings, C. A., Moulding, R., and Knight, T. (2017). You are what you own: Reviewing the link between possessions, emotional attachment, and the self-concept in hoarding disorder. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 14, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.05.005
Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., and Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 125, 470–500. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.470
Kuan, T. (2008). Mindfulness in early Buddhism: New approaches through psychology and textual analysis of Pali, Chinese and Sanskrit sources. New York, NY: Routledge.
Lafrance Robinson, A., Kosmerly, S., Mansfield-Green, S., and Lafrance, G. (2014). Disordered eating behaviours in an undergraduate sample: Associations among gender, body mass index, and difficulties in emotion regulation. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 46, 320–326. doi: 10.1037/a0031123
Li, Y., Wei, L., Zeng, X., and Zhu, J. (2021). Mindfulness in ethical consumption: The mediating roles of connectedness to nature and self-control. Int. Mark. Rev. 38, 756–779. doi: 10.1108/IMR-01-2019-0023
Liu, Y., Ma, D.-H., Du, Y.-L., Liu, X.-R., Zhao, J.-F., and Zhang, S.-C. (2021). Hoarding behavior on depression in college students: The multiple chain mediating effects of social exclution, thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 29, 1050–1059. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2021.05.032
Luberto, C. M., Cotton, S., Mcleish, A. C., Mingione, C. J., and O’Bryan, E. M. (2014). Mindfulness skills and emotion regulation: The mediating role of coping self-efficacy. Mindfulness 5, 373–380. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0190-6
Luders, E., Thompson, P. M., and Kurth, F. (2015). Larger hippocampal dimensions in meditation practitioners: Differential effects in women and men. Front. Psychol. 6:186. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00186
Magee, W., and Upenieks, L. (2019). Gender differences in self-esteem, unvarnished self-evaluation, future orientation, self-enhancement and self-derogation in a US national sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 149, 66–77. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.016
Marazziti, D., Pozza, A., Mucci, F., and Dettore, D. (2021). Compulsive hoarding symptoms and the role of mindfulness skills during social distancing for the COVID-19 pandemic: An exploratory survey. Front. Psychiatry 12:634454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.634454
Mathes, B. M., Timpano, K. R., Raines, A. M., and Schmidt, N. B. (2020). Attachment theory and hoarding disorder: A review and theoretical integration. Behav. Res. Ther. 125:103549. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103549
Maxwell, S. E., Cole, D. A., and Mitchell, M. A. (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behav. Res. 46, 816–841. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.606716
McGuire, J. F., Kaercher, L., Park, J. M., and Storch, E. A. (2013). Hoarding in the community: A code enforcement and social service perspective. J. Soc. Serv. Res. 39, 335–344. doi: 10.1080/01488376.2013.770813
McMullin, J. A., and Cairney, J. (2004). Self-esteem and the intersection of age, class, and gender. J. Aging Stud. 18, 75–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2003.09.006
Medard, E., and Kellett, S. (2014). The role of adult attachment and social support in hoarding disorder. Behav. Cognit. Psychther. 42, 629–633. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813000659
Menezes, C. B., and Bizarro, L. (2015). Effects of focused meditation on difficulties in emotion regulation and trait anxiety. Psychol. Neurosci. 8, 350–365. doi: 10.1037/pne0000015
Morrison, K. R., and Johnson, C. S. (2011). When what you have is who you are: Self-uncertainty leads individualists to see themselves in their possessions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 639–651. doi: 10.1177/0146167211403158
Neave, N., Jackson, R., Saxton, T., and Hoenekopp, J. (2015). The influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviours. Pers. Individ. Differ. 72, 214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.041
Neave, N., Tyson, H., Mcinnes, L., and Hamilton, C. (2016). The role of attachment style and anthropomorphism in predicting hoarding behaviours in a non-clinical sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 99, 33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.067
Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self Identity 2, 223–250. doi: 10.1080/15298860309027
Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion, self-esteem, and well-being. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 5, 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00330.x
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., and Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 1061–1072. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061
Orr, D. M. R., Preston-Shoot, M., and Braye, S. (2019). Meaning in hoarding: Perspectives of people who hoard on clutter, culture and agency. Anthropol. Med. 26, 263–279. doi: 10.1080/13648470.2017.1391171
Orth, U., and Robins, R. W. (2014). The development of self-esteem. Curr. Dir. Psychol. 23, 381–387. doi: 10.1177/0963721414547414
Orth, U., Trzesniewski, K. H., and Robins, R. W. (2010). Self-esteem development from young adulthood to old age: A cohort-sequential longitudinal study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 645–658. doi: 10.1037/a0018769
Park, H. J., and Dhandra, T. K. (2017). The effect of trait emotional intelligence on the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and self-esteem. Mindfulness 8, 1206–1211. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0693-2
Patel, S. R., Carmody, J., and Simpson, H. B. (2007). Adapting mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A case report. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 14, 375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2006.08.006
Pepping, C. A., and Duvenage, M. (2016). The origins of individual differences in dispositional mindfulness. Pers. Individ. Differ. 93, 130–136. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.027
Perlin, L. I., Lieberman, M. A., Menaghan, E., and Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress process. J. Health Soc. Behav. 22, 337–356. doi: 10.2307/2136676
Powers, A., Dixon, H. D., Guelfo, A., Mekawi, Y., Bradley, B., Kaslow, N., et al. (2021). The mediating role of emotion dysregulation in the association between trait mindfulness and PTSD symptoms among trauma-exposed adults. Mindfulness 12, 2229–2240. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01684-8
Raines, A. M., Short, N. A., Fuller, K. L., Allan, N. P., Oglesby, M. E., and Schmidt, N. B. (2016). Hoarding and depression: The mediating role of perceived burdensomeness. J. Psychiatr. Res. 83, 24–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.003
Ree, M. J., and Craigie, M. A. (2007). Outcomes following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a heterogeneous sample of adult outpatients. Behav. Change 24, 70–86. doi: 10.1375/bech.24.2.70
Rentzsch, K., Wenzler, M. P., and Schütz, A. (2016). The structure of multidimensional self-esteem across age and gender. Pers. Individ. Differ. 88, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.012
Rojiani, R., Santoyo, J. F., Rahrig, H., Roth, H. D., and Britton, W. B. (2017). Women benefit more than men in response to college-based meditation training. Front. Psychol. 8:551. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00551
Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., and Schoenbach, C. (1989). Self-esteem and adolescent problems: Modeling reciprocal effects. Am. Sociol. Rev. 54, 1004–1018. doi: 10.2307/2095720
Samuels, J., Bienvenu, O. J., Riddle, M. A., Cullen, B. A. M., Grados, M. A., Liang, K.-Y., et al. (2002). Hoarding in obsessive compulsive disorder: Results from a case-control study. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 517–528. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00026-2
Samuels, J. F., Bienvenu, O. J., Grados, M. A., Cullen, B., Riddle, M. A., Liang, K.-Y., et al. (2008). Prevalence and correlates of hoarding behavior in a community-based sample. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 836–844. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.004
Šeibokaitė, L., Endriulaitienė, A., Sullman, M. J. M., Markšaitytė, R., and Žardeckaitė-Matulaitienė, K. (2017). Difficulties in emotion regulation and risky driving among Lithuanian drivers. Traffic Inj. Prev. 18, 688–693. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2017.1315109
Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., and Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 373–386. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237
Shapiro, S. L., Jazaieri, H., and Goldin, P. R. (2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction effects on moral reasoning and decision making. J. Posit. Psychol. 7, 504–515. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.723732
Smith, B., Fowler, D. G., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P., Bashforth, H., Garety, P., et al. (2006). Emotion and psychosis: Links between depression, self-esteem, negative schematic beliefs and delusions and hallucinations. Schizophr. Res. 86, 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.018
Steketee, G., and Frost, R. (2003). Compulsive hoarding: Current status of the research. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23, 905–927. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2003.08.002
Steketee, G., Kelley, A. A., Wernick, J. A., Muroff, J., Frost, R. O., and Tolin, D. F. (2015). Familial patterns of hoarding symptoms. Depress. Anxiety 32, 728–736. doi: 10.1002/da.22393
Sugiura, T., and Sugiura, Y. (2015). Common factors of meditation, focusing, and cognitive behavioral therapy: Longitudinal relation of self-report measures to worry, depressive, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms among nonclinical students. Mindfulness 6, 610–623. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0296-0
Taylor, J. K., Moulding, R., and Nedeljkovic, M. (2018). Emotion regulation and hoarding symptoms. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 18, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.03.003
Tian, L. (2006). Shortcoming and merits of Chinese version of Rosenberg (1965) self-esteem scale. Psychol. Explor. 26, 88–91.
Timpano, K. R., Çek, D., Fu, Z. F., Tang, T., Wang, J. P., and Chasson, G. S. (2015). A consideration of hoarding disorder symptoms in China. Compr. Psychiatry 57, 36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.11.006
Timpano, K. R., Keough, M. E., Traeger, L., and Schmidt, N. B. (2011). General life stress and hoarding: Examining the role of emotional tolerance. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 4, 263–279. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2011.4.3.263
Tolin, D. F. (2011). Understanding and treating hoarding: A biopsychosocial perspective. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 517–526. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20795
Tolin, D. F., Levy, H. C., Wootton, B. M., Hallion, L. S., and Stevens, M. C. (2018). Hoarding disorder and difficulties in emotion regulation. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 16, 98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.01.006
Tripathi, A., Avasthi, A., Grover, S., Sharma, E., Lakdawala, B. M., Thirunavukarasu, M., et al. (2018). Gender differences in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Findings from a multicentric study from India. Asian J. Psychiatr. 37, 3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2018.07.022
Turna, J., Patterson, B., Simpson, W., Pullia, K., Khalesi, Z., Kaplan, K. G., et al. (2018). Prevalence of hoarding behaviours and excessive acquisition in users of online classified advertisements. Psychiatry Res. 270, 194–197. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.022
Wang, K., and Kong, F. (2019). Linking trait mindfulness to life satisfaction in adolescents: The mediating role of resilience and self-esteem. Child Indic. Res. 13, 321–335. doi: 10.1007/s12187-019-09698-4
Webb, L., Sibinga, E., Musci, R., Clary, L. K., and Mendelson, T. (2021). Confirming profiles of comorbid psychological symptoms in urban youth: Exploring gender differences and trait mindfulness. J. Youth Adolesc. 50, 2249–2261. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01509-w
Weiss, E. R., Landers, A., Todman, M., and Roane, D. M. (2020). Treatment outcomes in older adults with hoarding disorder: The impact of self-control, boredom and social support. Australas. J. Ageing 39, 375–380. doi: 10.1111/ajag.12817
Wheaton, M., Timpano, K. R., Lasalle-Ricci, V. H., and Murphy, D. (2008). Characterizing the hoarding phenotype in individuals with OCD: Associations with comorbidity, severity and gender. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 243–252. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.01.015
Wheaton, M. G., Abramowitz, J. S., Franklin, J. C., Berman, N. C., and Fabricant, L. E. (2011). Experiential avoidance and saving cognitions in the prediction of hoarding symptoms. Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 511–516. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9338-7
Wieckowski, A. T., Luallin, S., Pan, Z., Righi, G., Gabriels, R. L., and Mazefsky, C. (2020). Gender differences in emotion dysregulation in an autism inpatient psychiatric sample. Autism Res. 13, 1343–1348. doi: 10.1002/aur.2295
Wills, T. A., Ainette, M. G., Stoolmiller, M., Gibbons, F. X., and Shinar, O. (2008). Good self-control as a buffering agent for adolescent substance use: An investigation in early adolescence with time-varying covariates. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 22, 459–471. doi: 10.1037/a0012965
Xu, W., Rodriguez, M. A., Zhang, Q., and Liu, X. (2014). The mediating effect of self-acceptance in the relationship between mindfulness and peace of mind. Mindfulness 6, 797–802. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0319-x
Yap, K., and Grisham, J. R. (2021). Object attachment in hoarding disorder and its role in a compensatory process. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 39, 76–81.
Zawadzki, M. J. (2015). Rumination is independently associated with poor psychological health: Comparing emotion regulation strategies. Psychol. Health 30, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2015.1026904
Zeigler-Hill, V., Enjaian, B., Holden, C. J., and Southard, A. C. (2014). Using self-esteem instability to disentangle the connection between self-esteem level and perceived aggression. J. Res. Pers. 49, 47–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.01.003
Keywords: mindfulness, hoarding, self-esteem, emotion dysregulation, gender differences
Citation: Gong Y, Tan Y and Huang R (2022) Exploring the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and hoarding behavior: A moderated multi-mediation model. Front. Psychol. 13:935897. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.935897
Received: 04 May 2022; Accepted: 29 July 2022;
Published: 12 August 2022.
Edited by:
Petra Hanson, University of Warwick, United KingdomReviewed by:
David Tolin, The Institute of Living, United StatesJessica Stubbing, The Institute of Living, United States, in collaboration with reviewer DT
Keong Yap, Australian Catholic University, Australia
Zita Sousa, Instituto Politécnico do porto, Portugal
Copyright © 2022 Gong, Tan and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yuxuan Tan, a2F0aHl0YW45M0AxNjMuY29t
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship