Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 October 2022
Sec. Psychology of Aging

Joint developmental trajectories and temporal precedence of physical function decline and cognitive deterioration: A longitudinal population-based study

\r\nXiao Wei&#x;Xiao WeiHeng Liu&#x;Heng LiuLi Yang
Li Yang*Zihan GaoZihan GaoJinke KuangJinke KuangKexin ZhouKexin ZhouMengfan XuMengfan Xu
  • School of Nursing, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China

Objectives: Previous studies primarily explored the unidirectional impact of cognition on physical function. However, the interplay between physical function and cognition and the temporal precedence in their predictive relationships have not been elucidated. We explored the bidirectional mechanism between physical function and cognition in a longitudinal dataset.

Materials and methods: A total of 1,365 participants in the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey assessed physical function and cognition in 2011 (T1), 2014 (T2), and 2018 (T3) by the Katz scale and the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination scale, respectively. Changes in the trajectories of physical function and cognition were examined using the latent growth model. The correlational and reciprocal relationships between physical function and cognition were examined using the parallel process latent growth model and autoregressive cross-lagged (ARCL) models.

Results: Cognition and physical function decreased by an average of 0.096 and 0.017 points per year, respectively. Higher physical function was associated with better cognition at baseline (r = 0.237, p < 0.05), and longitudinal changes in physical function and cognition were positively correlated (r = 0.756, p < 0.05). ARCL analysis indicated that physical function at T1 positively predicted T2 cognitive function. However, this predictive relationship reversed between T2 and T3, whereby cognitive function at T2 predicted physical function at T3.

Conclusion: Both physical function and cognition declined over time. Early identification and intervention in physical dysfunction among older adults could be critical to prevent further cognitive impairment and maintain functional independence. Hence, regular functional assessment and individualized care plans are required to achieve healthy aging.

Introduction

The number of adults aged ≥ 60 years is expected to reach 2 billion by 2050, representing 22% of the entire population, almost double the 12% reported in 2015 (World Health Organization, 2018). Meeting the health needs of an aging population of this size poses a serious challenge to global health systems. It is accepted that prevention is more important than treatment in achieving healthy aging (Nivestam et al., 2020). Functional decline is a largely preventable feature of aging and is defined as a progressive weakening of functional autonomy, comprising mainly cognitive deterioration and decline in physical function (PF), impairing the ability of older adults to live safely and independently (Grimmer et al., 2013). Recognizing the characteristics of cognitive function (CF) and PF decline is crucial for supporting healthy aging and maintaining the quality of life of older adults.

Cognitive deterioration is placed on a continuum, including normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and severe cognitive impairment (dementia) (Chertkow et al., 2007) that are accompanied by different PF states, especially in the areas of feeding, dressing, toileting, and bathing (Ebly et al., 1995). Physical function in the dementia group was lower than that in the mild cognitive impairment group, specifically, regarding grooming, bathing, and bowel control domains (Lee et al., 2019). Cognitive impairment has been proven to be a risk factor for PF decline among older adults (Arias-Merino et al., 2012; Ha and Kim, 2014). Several studies on the trajectories of functional capacity decline have revealed that participants with worse CF at baseline exhibited larger PF decline (Dodge et al., 2005; Helvik et al., 2015), and older adults with lower levels of cognition are more likely to decline in functional capacity (Menezes et al., 2021).

Relatively few studies have focused on the effects of PF on cognition. However, available research has begun to propose that cognitive deterioration and PF decline may mutually influence and reinforce each other’s development (Black and Rush, 2002). One study reported that the onset of functional disability accelerated CF decline that progressed faster in individuals with more severe functional disabilities (Rajan et al., 2013). This echoes the findings of another study that the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in individuals with disabilities is higher than that in persons without disabilities (Chang et al., 2017).

In summary, many of the existing studies have focused on the unidirectional relationship between CF and PF. Meanwhile, evidence from cross-sectional studies has limited utility when considering temporal associations between variables. Moreover, existing longitudinal studies have focused more on the contributions of longitudinal changes in cognition to longitudinal changes in PF (Dodge et al., 2006; Cahn-Weiner et al., 2007), leading to the possibility that existing studies overlooked the impact on cognitive function of PF decline due to various causes, such as falls and fractures, when cognitive function is not impaired. Clarifying whether PF or cognitive ability changes first during the aging process and the subsequent impact on each other would better guide functional screening and interventions for older adults. Hence, longitudinal studies exploring the reciprocal relationship between PF and cognition are required.

In addition, previous studies have shown that changes in cognitive function varied significantly by sex, age, educational level, income, living arrangements, psychological status, social participation, and age-related health conditions such as hearing function and the number of chronic diseases (Zhang et al., 2019; Chen and Zhou, 2020). However, the association between lifestyle factors, such as smoking and alcohol consumption, and cognitive impairment is inconsistent across studies. Some studies reported that smoking status and excessive alcohol consumption are associated with cognitive decline in older adults (Hagger-Johnson et al., 2013), whereas other studies have disproven this association (Hou et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019). Similarly, studies demonstrated that sex, age, education level, depressive symptoms, and age-related health conditions such as hearing function and the number of chronic conditions are associated with PF decline in older adults (Diaz-Venegas et al., 2016; Chen and Zhou, 2020). Therefore, the current study includes all these variables as a covariate to identify protective or risk factors for CF and PF in older adults.

Hence, this study aimed to explore the longitudinal correlational and reciprocal relationships between PF and CF by utilizing large-scale data from the older adult Chinese population and advanced statistical methods, including the latent growth model (LGM) and an autoregressive cross-lagged (ARCL) model.

Materials and methods

Participants and settings

The data were obtained from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS), a longitudinal study investigating the health status of older adults in China, conducted by the Center for Research on Healthy Aging and Development at Peking University, which obtained data from eight face-to-face interviews with the cohort since 1998, utilizing internationally compatible questionnaires. Survivors were reinterviewed at each follow-up visit, and those deceased were substituted with new participants (Yang et al., 2016). Of 31 provinces in China, 23 were randomly selected, accounting for approximately half of the urban areas in each province (Qiu et al., 2020). The age of the respondents who voluntarily agreed to participate in the study covered the middle age group (34–64 years), the lower age group (65–80 years), and the higher age group (80 years and above). Individuals were excluded if they died, were lost to follow-up, or failed to respond to the items comprising the outcome variable (Ni et al., 2020). More detailed data information is available at https://doi.org/10.18170/DVN/WBO7LK.

We used data from three waves of the CLHLS: 2011 (T1), 2014 (T2), and 2018 (T3) as a result of follow-up loss and natural death of subjects, and the first measurement of health information including hearing function in 2011. The two inclusion criteria were (1) completion of three surveys and (2) no missing PF and CF data. There were 9,765 subjects in 2011. By 2018, 6,862 subjects were lost to follow-up or had died during the follow-up period. After excluding a further 1,538 subjects with missing PF and CF data, we ultimately analyzed 1,365 individuals who responded to the survey from 2011 to 2018.

We compared the baseline characteristics of included and excluded participants (see Supplementary Table 1). The results revealed no differences between included and excluded participants regarding income, living arrangements, and number of chronic conditions. Predictably, and consistent with previous studies (Yu et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021), these excluded participants tended to be older, female, with lower levels of education, who smoked less, drank less alcohol, and performed less well in terms of mental status, social engagement, and hearing function, but did not differ from included participants in terms of income, living arrangements, and number of chronic conditions.

Cognitive function

Cognitive function in the CLHLS was tested using the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (C-MMSE), which comprises six subscales with 24 items: five orienting, three registering, one naming, five noticing and counting, three recalling, and seven verbal items. The total C-MMSE rating ranged from 0 to 30 points, with larger scores indicating stronger cognition. The C-MMSE is a well-established instrument for CF assessment that has been verified in previous studies (Yang et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2019; Qiu et al., 2020).

Physical function

Physical function was evaluated by sum scores (ranging from 6 to 18 points) in six components of daily living: bathing, dressing, grooming, indoor transferring, toileting, and feeding according to the Katz scale (Katz et al., 1963). Older adults were scored as 1 (totally dependent on others), 2 (partially independent), or 3 (completely independent) points, depending on their ability to complete these actions without assistance, with larger scores indicating better daily living ability.

Covariates

The following covariates were evaluated in this study: (1) demographic characteristics, including age, sex (1 = man, 2 = woman), years of education, living arrangement (1 = living with family members, 0 = not living with family members), and income; (2) lifestyle factors, including smoking (1 = yes, 0 = no) and drinking (1 = yes, 0 = no); (3) social engagements, including doing housework, working in the garden, reading newspapers/books, raising domestic poultry/pets, playing poker/mahjong, watching television or listening to the radio, and participating in social activities. Respondents were asked about the frequency of participation in each activity: 5 = almost every day, 4 = at least once a week, 3 = at least once a month, 2 = sometimes, and 1 = never; (4) mental status, representing the extent of mental wellbeing, rated by the sum of seven questions scored on a scale from 7 to 35 points, including being optimistic, maintaining tidiness, not feeling afraid or restless, not feeling alone, making own decisions, not feeling useless with age, and being happier than in youth, with better scores indicating a more positive mental state; (5) health status, including hearing function and the numbers of chronic diseases, where the hearing function was assessed based on the interviewee’s ability to hear the interviewer’s question, with four possible responses: 4 = yes, without hearing aid; 3 = yes, but needs hearing aid; 2 = partly, despite hearing aid; 1 = no. Chronic diseases included hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, stroke, bronchitis, tuberculosis, cataracts, glaucoma, cancer, gastric or duodenal ulcer, Parkinson’s disease, bedsore, arthritis, and dementia. Sex, education, and income levels were time-invariant covariates, while the other variables were time-varying covariates.

Data analysis

The participants’ characteristics, including CF scores, PF scores, and covariate distributions or scores, were presented as descriptive statistics and analyzed using SPSS 25.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). Missing covariate data were substituted using multiple imputations based on Bayesian methods in SPSS 25.0.

Latent growth model

The LGM allows analysis of repeatedly measured temporal trajectories of a variable by establishing random intercepts and slopes (Sha et al., 2018), thereby describing individual trajectories and capturing individual differences in trajectory variation over time (Wang et al., 2017). See the Supplementary Data Sheet 1 for the specific rationale. Therefore, LGM analysis was performed to examine the changing trajectories of PF and CF. The change in trajectory was represented by two latent variables: a potential intercept growth factor, which reflects the initial state of variables, and a potential slope growth factor, which reflects the changing rate of variables. By considering the three waves of data, the trajectories of the variables were modeled using the specified linear LGM. The loading of the intercept factor was set for each measurement at a constant value of 1.0. For the slope factor, we fixed loadings sequentially at 0, 3.0, and 7.0, based on the survey time point.

Two unconditional LGMs were first modeled to mirror the growing trajectories of PF and CF without predictor variables (Figures 1A,B). The parallel process LGM (PP-LGM) enabled the estimation of interrelationships between growth factors (intercept and slope) by building a hypothetical model of two variables in parallel processes (Ni et al., 2020). Unconditional (without covariates or predictors, Figure 1C) and conditional (with covariates or predictors added) PP-LGMs were established to examine whether the growth parameters of one trajectory were related to those of the other.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Latent growth model. (A) Measurement of latent growth model for cognitive function. (B) Measurement of latent growth model for physical function. (C) Structural latent growth model to assess the relationships between changes in physical function and cognitive function. PF, physical function; CF, cognitive function; I, intercept; S, linear slope.

Autoregressive cross-lagged model

The ARCL model, comprising an autoregressive component and a cross-lagged component, adjusts for the stability of all variables over time and simultaneously explores the interaction of the two variables over time (Han and Kim, 2020). Thus, it is often applied to deduce temporal precedence of predictive relationships among variables in longitudinal studies. The cross-lagged parameters are typically interpreted as the between-person effect of Xtime1 on Ytime2, controlling for Ytime1 and vice versa (Gueron-Sela and Gordon-Hacker, 2020). Therefore, the ARCL model helped to distinguish between the effects of PF on CF and the effects of CF on PF in the current study, reflecting the reciprocal relationship between the two variables over an extended period.

All LGM and ARCL model analyses were conducted using Mplus version 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, United States). The following metrics were used to evaluate the model fit: chi-square (χ2) statistic, 1 < χ2/degrees of freedom (df) < 3, comparative fit index (CFI) of > 0.90, preferably of > 0.95; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of < 0.08; 90% confidence interval (CI) ≤ 0.08, preferably of < 0.06 (Melka et al., 2011); and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) of ≤ 0.08 (Brown and Weisman de Mamani, 2018).

Results

The respondents’ characteristics are presented in Table 1. In the baseline survey, 54.9% of the participants were men, and 81.8% lived with their families. Most participants were non-smokers and non-drinkers; the corresponding proportions of participants increased during the follow-up period. During the 7-year period, the scores of covariates reflecting healthy status, including mental status, social engagements, and hearing function, declined with increasing age and the number of chronic diseases. The mean cognitive and physical functioning scores at each time point also decreased over time.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants.

Measurement model

In the LGM, the two trajectories of CF and PF change were described by a linear model with satisfactory fit indices (Table 2). Based on the mean intercept representing the baseline level, the initial scores for PF and CF were estimated to be 17.942 and 28.323 points, respectively. Based on the mean slope indicating the direction and magnitude of the trajectory change, the mean annual reductions in PF and CF scores were separately estimated at 0.017 and 0.096 points, respectively. Roughly, these results are consistent with the descriptive statistics on CF and PF as shown in Table 1, indicating that this LGM fits well to the data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of univariate latent growth model analyses.

Structural model

Unconditional parallel process-latent growth model modeling

The unconditional PP-LGM used to test the correlation between PF decline trajectory with the trajectory of CF deterioration showed a better model fit (Table 3). The PF intercept was significantly associated with the CF intercept in 2011 (r = 0.237, p < 0.05), which suggested that participants with better PF had higher cognitive scores at baseline. The PF slope was significantly associated with the CF slope (r = 0.756, p < 0.05), indicating that subjects with a greater rate of PF decline experienced greater decreases in CF over time.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized coefficients for parallel process LGM.

Conditional parallel process-latent growth model modeling

Compared with the unconditional PP-LGM, the conditional PP-LGM that controlled for predictors yielded better fit indices, but the results differed (Table 3). The correlation between initial PF levels and baseline CF scores was non-significant (r = 0.169, p > 0.05). Only the correlation between PF slope and CF slope remained significant, and the standardized coefficients were attenuated (r = 0.464, p < 0.05).

The variances of intercepts and slopes for PF and CF scores were significant (p < 0.05, Table 2), indicating strong individual differences between the trajectories of PF and CF. Therefore, time-invariant and time-varying covariates were included in the PP-LGM to determine whether individual characteristics were predictive of bivariate trajectories of CF and PF.

Sex, living arrangement, drinking status, and the number of chronic diseases were negatively associated with initial CF scores. Participants with higher education or lower incomes had a greater CF decline rate. Meanwhile, mental status, social engagement, and hearing function were positively related to CF scores at all three time points. However, the hearing function was positively correlated with PF in the last two waves. Living with family members and more chronic diseases was associated with lower PF scores in the third wave (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Standardized coefficients for covariates in the conditional parallel process latent growth model.

Bidirectional associations between physical function and cognition

We inspected the bidirectional association between PF and CF at three time points using the ARCL model. The model fits the data well [χ2/(4) = 33.78, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.937, RMSEA = 0.074, RMSEA 90% CI (0.052, 0.098), and SRMR = 0.026]. The significant autoregressive paths indicate the stability of PF and CF over time. PF in the first wave had a statistically significant effect on cognition in the second wave (β = 0.061, p = 0.019) and cognition in the second wave on PF in the third wave (β = 0.097, p < 0.001) (Figure 2). Compared with the result which controls for covariates (see Supplementary Figure 1), the results show the same predictive chain that between T1 and T2, PF positively predicted CF. However, from T2 to T3, the opposite effect was observed, whereby CF positively predicted PF.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Autoregressive cross-lagged model assessing bidirectional relationships between physical function and cognition over 3 years. Solid lines indicate significant associations, and dashed lines indicate non-significant associations. PF, physical function; CF, cognitive function, *p < 0.05.

Discussion

The present study investigated the trajectories of PF performance and CF scores and the longitudinal relationships between PF decline and cognitive deterioration in the Chinese older population based on longitudinal data collected over 7 years. The LGM revealed a declining trend in both CF and PF scores with a low change rate, consistent with a previous study that reported a minor decline in PF and cognition, with average annual decline rates of 0.03 (Diaz-Venegas and Wong, 2016) and 0.07 (Ku et al., 2012), respectively. The finding can be explained by the “benefits of success” concept (Fries, 1980); in other words, persons are living longer (success) and in better health at older ages than previously (benefits). The cognition and PF of older adults have benefited from these advances, with lower rates of functional disability, more effective disease treatment and healthier lifestyles, reduced disability rates for some major chronic diseases, such as stroke and cardiovascular disease, and improved living standards due to China’s rapid socio-economic development (Zeng et al., 2017). Overall, the decrease in PF and CF is insidious. Regular assessments of CF and PF are essential to achieving good outcomes and should ideally be included in routine medical examinations of middle-aged and older individuals.

The correlational relationship between CF and PF was explored using PP-LGM. Individuals with lower PF levels at baseline showed lower CF scores. However, the weak correlation (Cohen, 1992) between PF and CF at baseline was weakened to insignificance by the inclusion of control variables as competing explanatory variables (Lee Ray, 2016) during the initial CF, and PF of the study subjects was less impaired. Both initial cognitive functioning and PF performance were shown to be primarily influenced by age and social engagements in this study. Consistent with previous research, social engagement helps protect against functional decline in PF and CF among older adults, namely more social engagement is associated with better cognitive performance and less PF decline. The cognitive reserve, stress, and vascular hypothesis proposes that social activity can increase cognitive reserve to selectively improve CF and delay CF decline by increasing social contact with people to maintain a positive emotional state or reduce stress levels and consequently reduce the risk of cardiovascular disease (Fratiglioni et al., 2004). Other studies have reported the protective effect of social participation on PF (Ma et al., 2020; Dos Santos and Alberto Gobbo, 2021), even if older adults only do chores around the house that require strength and limb exercises in certain situations, such as carrying a bucket of water or sweeping, sufficiently to contribute to the maintenance of PF. Health policy initiatives should therefore encourage older adults to engage in social activities to delay their functional decline, including the provision of community activity centers for older adults.

However, the decreased rate of PF is positively correlated with the rate of CF deterioration, independently of adjustments for covariates, indicating that accelerated decline in function on either side triggers accelerated functional decline on the other side. It means the accelerated PF decline could trigger an accelerated decline in cognitive function and vice versa, which corroborates that PF and CF reciprocally influence each other’s development (Black and Rush, 2002) and may form a self-perpetuating malignant cycle in the absence of appropriate interventions. This finding deserves more attention from clinicians. Focusing on disability prevention and PF exercise could reduce the occurrence of cognitive decline. Equally, attention to the prevention and treatment of cognitive impairment will potentially prevent functional disability. Thus, attention to risk factors for cognitive or physical decline, such as specific chronic health conditions and mental status, would be helpful in preventing functional decline in older adults.

We further used the ARCL model to detect the sequence of predicted relationships between PF decline and CF deterioration. Consistent with a previous study (Farias et al., 2013) showing a gradual decline of daily functioning in individuals with normal CF and an accelerated decline as MCI progresses toward dementia, our results support that PF decline precedes CF decline and subsequently induces more PF decline. It could be explained by two potential mechanisms. First, neurofibrillary tangles in the substantia nigra related to gait may represent preclinical cognitive impairment in the brain pathology (Schneider et al., 2006). Second, stroke or diabetes can induce an initial reduction in PF, and disease-related vascular problems may subsequently exacerbate CF decline (Fauth et al., 2013).

The specific predictive chronology of functional decline identified in this study suggests that the health goals of older adults at different levels of functioning are distinct and that health management programs should focus on effectively preventing and delaying PF decline. Focusing on the etiology of unintentional injuries, such as falls, fractures, and cardiovascular disease, may help prevent or enable early intervention in PF decline, thereby helping to maintain a satisfactory quality of life.

This longitudinal study was based on a large national sample from the CLHLS database. Data quality was ensured by implementing strict quality control measures at each stage of the CLHLS survey. The use of LGM and ARCL analyses helped to identify the joint trajectory characteristics and sequential relationships between PF and CF decline, helping to determine the stage of functional decline among older adults, to select the timing of intervention, and to develop targeted health management programs that aim to delay functional decline and achieve healthy aging. Moreover, this study identified factors affecting both PF and cognition, which helped elucidate the association between PF and CF.

The present study has some limitations. Although the data quality was assured by stringent quality control at each stage of the CLHLS, the long duration of follow-up yielded missing data. The study performed multiple interpolations of data with missing confounding variables to achieve the best possible use of the available data. Furthermore, retained cohort members were younger and had better mental status and social engagement in general than excluded individuals, which may lead to the study being underpowered and biased toward the healthier segment of the aging population. Since we used a nationally representative database, confirmation bias could be limited to an extent. We are reminded to expand the sample in future studies by adding older individuals with different mental statuses and social engagement levels as subjects to improve the generalizability of this study’s findings. In addition, the research items, including PF assessment, social engagement, and mental status, were based on self-reported data, which may have been affected by recall and measurement biases. Nevertheless, the face-to-face interview method allowed investigators to get a more accurate picture of overall respondent profiles, which ensured the response quality to some extent. Furthermore, although this study revealed declines in cognitive and physical function among older adults with three measurements over a 7-year period, the two intervals may not be long enough to detect significant functional decline and to examine the non-linear relationship between PF and cognition. Therefore, future studies should consider selecting objective measurement instruments and extending the follow-up period to explore the long-term dynamic relationship between PF and cognition by constructing non-linear LGM.

Conclusion

This longitudinal study examined the temporal relationship between PF and CF in Chinese older adults, using survey data collected at three time points over 7 years. The results revealed that a decrease in PF and CF was characterized as implicit, with accelerated decline in one variable causing a faster decrease in the other variable. There is a clear prediction priority in the time between PF decrement and cognitive deterioration, whereby initial PF scores predicted CF, and CF predicted subsequent PF. Early identification and intervention in physical dysfunction among older adults would be critical to prevent further cognitive impairment and maintain functional independence. Regular functional assessment and individualized care plans are required to achieve healthy aging.

Data availability statement

The data used were obtained from the public database of the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey: https://opendata.pku.edu.cn/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.18170/DVN/WBO7LK.

Author contributions

XW and HL were responsible for the study conception, design, and drafting of the manuscript. ZG, JK, KZ, and MX collated data and conducted data analyses. LY was responsible for supervision. All authors made critical revisions to the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Shandong Provincial Natural Science Foundation (No. ZR2021MG031).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Center for Healthy Aging and Development Research at Peking University for access to the CLHLS data.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.933886/full#supplementary-material

References

Arias-Merino, E. D., Mendoza-Ruvalcaba, N. M., Ortiz, G. G., Velazquez-Brizuela, I. E., Meda-Lara, R. M., and Cueva-Contreras, J. (2012). Physical function and associated factors in community-dwelling elderly people in Jalisco, Mexico. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 54, e271–e278. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2012.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, S. A., and Rush, R. D. (2002). Cognitive and functional decline in adults aged 75 and older. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 50, 1978–1986. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50609.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. A., and Weisman de Mamani, A. (2018). The mediating effect of family cohesion in reducing patient symptoms and family distress in a culturally informed family therapy for schizophrenia: A parallel-process latent-growth model. J. Consult Clin. Psychol. 86, 1–14. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Farias, S. T., Julian, L., Harvey, D. J., Kramer, J. H., and Reed, B. R. (2007). Cognitive and neuroimaging predictors of instrumental activities of daily living. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 747–757. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. F., Yang, R. J., Chang, S. F., Chou, Y. H., and Huang, E. W. (2017). The effects of quality of life and ability to perform activities of daily living on mild cognitive impairment in older people living in publicly managed congregate housing. J. Nurs. Res. 25, 187–197. doi: 10.1097/JNR.0000000000000149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., and Zhou, R. (2020). Does self-reported hearing difficulty decrease older adults’ cognitive and physical functioning? The mediating role of social isolation. Maturitas 141, 53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2020.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chertkow, H., Nasreddine, Z., Joanette, Y., Drolet, V., Kirk, J., and Massoud, F. (2007). Mild cognitive impairment and cognitive impairment, no dementia: Part A, concept and diagnosis. Alzheimers Dement 3, 266–282. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz-Venegas, C., and Wong, R. (2016). Trajectories of limitations in activities of daily living among older adults in Mexico, 2001-2012. Disabil. Health J. 9, 524–532. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2016.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz-Venegas, C., Reistetter, T. A., Wang, C. Y., and Wong, R. (2016). The progression of disability among older adults in Mexico. Disabil. Rehabil. 38, 2016–2027. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2015.1111435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, H. H., Du, Y., Saxton, J. A., and Ganguli, M. (2006). Cognitive domains and trajectories of functional independence in non-demented elderly. J. Gerontol. Series A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 61, 1330–1337. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, H. H., Kadowaki, T., Hayakawa, T., Yamakawa, M., Sekikawa, A., and Ueshima, H. (2005). Cognitive impairment as a strong predictor of incident disability in specific ADL-IADL tasks among community-dwelling elders: The Azuchi Study. Gerontologist 45, 222–230. doi: 10.1093/geront/45.2.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dos Santos, V. R., and Alberto Gobbo, L. (2021). Occupational and leisure-time physical activity decreases the odds of disability in older adults: Prospective study. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 21, 927–934. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2020.1790669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebly, E. M., Hogan, D. B., and Parhad, I. M. (1995). Cognitive impairment in the nondemented elderly. results from the Canadian Study of health and aging. Arch. Neurol. 52, 612–619. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540300086018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farias, S. T., Chou, E., Harvey, D. J., Mungas, D., Reed, B., DeCarli, C., et al. (2013). Longitudinal trajectories of everyday function by diagnostic status. Psychol. Aging 28, 1070–1075. doi: 10.1037/a0034069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fauth, E. B., Schwartz, S., Tschanz, J. T., Ostbye, T., Corcoran, C., and Norton, M. C. (2013). Baseline disability in activities of daily living predicts dementia risk even after controlling for baseline global cognitive ability and depressive symptoms. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 597–606. doi: 10.1002/gps.3865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., and Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 3, 343–353. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(04)00767-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, J. F. (1980). Aging, natural death, and the compression of morbidity. N. Engl. J. Med. 303, 130–135. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198007173030304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimmer, K., Luker, J., Beaton, K., Kumar, S., Crockett, A., and Price, K. (2013). TRialing individualized interventions to prevent functional decline in at-risk older adults (TRIIFL): Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial nested in a longitudinal observational study. Trials 14:266. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-14-266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gueron-Sela, N., and Gordon-Hacker, A. (2020). Longitudinal links between media use and focused attention through toddlerhood: A cumulative risk approach. Front. Psychol. 11:569222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ha, E., and Kim, K. (2014). Factors that influence activities of daily living in the elderly with probable dementia. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 21, 447–454. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger-Johnson, G., Sabia, S., Brunner, E. J., Shipley, M., Bobak, M., Marmot, M., et al. (2013). Combined impact of smoking and heavy alcohol use on cognitive decline in early old age: Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry 203, 120–125. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.122960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J. W., and Kim, D. J. (2020). Longitudinal relationship study of depression and self-esteem in postnatal korean women using autoregressive cross-lagged modeling. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:3743. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helvik, A. S., Hogseth, L. D., Bergh, S., Saltyte-Benth, J., Kirkevold, O., and Selbaek, G. (2015). A 36-month follow-up of decline in activities of daily living in individuals receiving domiciliary care. BMC Geriatr. 15:47. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0047-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, G. Y., Li, G. M., Xie, J. Y., and Yang, D. (2018). Trajectories of cognitive function in older adults: An analysis based on a latent variable growth model [in Chinese]. J. Psychol. Sci. 41, 835–841.

Google Scholar

Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., and Jaffe, M. W. (1963). Studies of illness in the aged. the index of adl: A standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA 185, 914–919. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ku, P. W., Stevinson, C., and Chen, L. J. (2012). Prospective associations between leisure-time physical activity and cognitive performance among older adults across an 11-year period. J. Epidemiol. 22, 230–237. doi: 10.2188/jea.je20110084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee Ray, J. (2016). Explaining interstate conflict and war: What should be controlled for?. Confl. Manag. Peace Sci. 20, 1–31. doi: 10.1177/073889420302000201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. T., Jang, Y., and Chang, W. Y. (2019). How do impairments in cognitive functions affect activities of daily living functions in older adults? PLoS One 14:e0218112. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Jiang, H., Jin, X., Wang, H., Ji, J. S., and Yan, L. L. (2021). Cognitive impairment and all-cause mortality among Chinese adults aged 80 years or older. Brain Behav. 11:e2325. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., Piao, X., and Oshio, T. (2020). Impact of social participation on health among middle-aged and elderly adults: Evidence from longitudinal survey data in China. BMC Public Health 20:502. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-08650-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melka, S. E., Lancaster, S. L., Bryant, A. R., and Rodriguez, B. F. (2011). Confirmatory factor and measurement invariance analyses of the emotion regulation questionnaire. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 1283–1293. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menezes, K., Auger, C., Barbosa, J. F. S., Gomes, C. S., Menezes, W. R. S., and Guerra, R. O. (2021). Trajectories and predictors of functional capacity decline in older adults from a Brazilian Northeastern Hospital. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 44, 82–87. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, Y., Tein, J. Y., Zhang, M., Zhen, F., Huang, F., and Huang, Y. (2020). The need to belong: A parallel process latent growth curve model of late life negative affect and cognitive function. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 89:104049. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2020.104049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nivestam, A., Westergren, A., Petersson, P., and Haak, M. (2020). Factors associated with good health among older persons who received a preventive home visit: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 20:688. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-08775-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, P., Zeng, M., Kuang, W., Meng, S. S., Cai, Y., Wang, H., et al. (2020). Heterogeneity in the dynamic change of cognitive function among older Chinese people: A growth mixture model. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 35, 1123–1133. doi: 10.1002/gps.5334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajan, K. B., Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Mendes de Leon, C. F., and Evans, D. A. (2013). Disability in basic and instrumental activities of daily living is associated with faster rate of decline in cognitive function of older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 624–630. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J. A., Li, J. L., Li, Y., Wilson, R. S., Kordower, J. H., and Bennett, D. A. (2006). Substantia nigra tangles are related to gait impairment in older persons. Ann. Neurol. 59, 166–173. doi: 10.1002/ana.20723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sha, T., Cheng, W., and Yan, Y. (2018). Prospective associations between pulse pressure and cognitive performance in Chinese middle-aged and older population across a 5-year study period. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10:29. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0355-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P. W., Lin, H. C., Yang, Y. C., Hsu, C. Y., Chung, K. S., Wu, H. C., et al. (2017). Gender and age effects on the trajectory of depression in opioid users during methadone maintenance treatment. Front. Psychiatry 8:288. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization, (2018). Ageing and health. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health (accessed June 8, 2021).

Google Scholar

Yang, L., Martikainen, P., Silventoinen, K., and Konttinen, H. (2016). Association of socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning change among elderly Chinese people. Age Ageing 45, 674–680. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afw107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J., Feng, Q., Yu, J., Zeng, Y., and Feng, L. (2020). Late-Life cognitive trajectories and their associated lifestyle factors. J. Alzheimers Dis. 73, 1555–1563. doi: 10.3233/JAD-191058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, Y., Feng, Q., Hesketh, T., Christensen, K., and Vaupel, J. W. (2017). Survival, disabilities in activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning among the oldest-old in China: A cohort study. Lancet 389, 1619–1629. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(17)30548-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Q., Wu, Y., Han, T., and Liu, E. (2019). Changes in cognitive function and risk factors for cognitive impairment of the elderly in China: 2005-2014. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:2847. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16162847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive function, physical function, aging, latent growth model, autoregressive cross-lagged model

Citation: Wei X, Liu H, Yang L, Gao Z, Kuang J, Zhou K and Xu M (2022) Joint developmental trajectories and temporal precedence of physical function decline and cognitive deterioration: A longitudinal population-based study. Front. Psychol. 13:933886. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.933886

Received: 16 May 2022; Accepted: 09 September 2022;
Published: 12 October 2022.

Edited by:

Ioannis Tarnanas, Altoida Inc., United States

Reviewed by:

Bohan Zhang, Harvard University, United States
Jennifer Pierce, University of Michigan, United States

Copyright © 2022 Wei, Liu, Yang, Gao, Kuang, Zhou and Xu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Li Yang, yangli81@qdu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.