Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 23 June 2022
Sec. Psychology of Language

Form-Focused Instruction, Technology, and Limitations: An Investigation of Past Research and Future Possibilities

  • Faculty of Sciences and Humanities, SUNY Korea, Seoul, South Korea

Modern studies that integrate technology with form-focused instruction (FFI) are useful, yet analysis of grammatical or learner differences is often limited. Without consistent examination of all factors that influence the efficacy of FFI, it is no surprise that results are often inconsistent. Recent synthesis of research concerning Korean, Persian, and Chinese learners suggests that grammatical difficulty (collectively defined by grammatical complexity, learner proficiency, and L1) determines when providing input is more effective, and when prompting a learner to produce output is more effective. Results suggest that input and explicit information may help learners produce language more accurately when grammatical difficulty is high (e.g., a grammatical feature is complex and learner proficiency is low). Conversely, compelling a learner to produce output appears to increase accuracy when difficulty of a grammatical feature is low (e.g., a grammatical feature is not complex and proficiency is high). More experimental research is needed to confirm results. Such research may provide a holistic perspective needed to tailor computer software to learner needs.

Introduction

Instead of human-to-human interaction, modern studies of corrective feedback (CF) tend to focus mainly on the use of technology. One recent study of written CF, for example, examined how computer-mediated collaborative groupwork could impact acquisition of the English definite and indefinite articles. Results of the study suggested that there was a significant relationship between CF and learner accuracy over time (Yamashita, 2021). In another study of the English article, the effects of digital game-based instruction vs. teacher instruction with corrective feedback were examined in more detail. Results suggested that only teacher instruction was more effective on the immediate posttest; the game-based instruction group, however, was significantly different for the delayed posttest (Reynolds and Kao, 2019). In a recent study of video chat recasts vs. face-to-face recasts, 57 Persian learners were found to use English articles more accurately in an oral production task for video chat recasts, suggesting superiority over face-to-face delivery (Rassaei, 2017).

While the use of computer-based media for grammar emphasis is indeed an important issue to explore, modern research appears to have some key limitations that reflect inherent weaknesses in past experimental designs. Studies by Yamashita (2021); Reynolds and Kao (2019), and Rassaei (2017), for example, all examine the same grammatical feature. A tendency to use the same grammatical feature in experimental studies appears to reflect a past misconception, which proposed that there are only two types of grammar. Systematic features like the past regular tense or English article were thought to be treatable, whereas lexical features (vocabulary words or collocations) were considered non-treatable (Wang and Jiang, 2015). Using an overly simplistic classification of either rule-governed or lexical features has dominated the field of second language acquisition, explaining why studies like that of Shintani (2012) test just vocabulary (nouns and adjectives) and one grammatical morpheme (plural -s). In addition to overly simple classification of features according to rule-based or lexical qualities, past research has used overly simplistic categorizations like simple or complex and early or late (Varnosfadrani and Basturkmen, 2009; Spada and Tomita, 2010; Van De Guchte et al., 2015). In reality, characteristics of a grammatical feature vary considerably based upon semantic, morphosyntactic, and phonological characteristics, which appear to collectively influence how a target feature is acquired (Goldschneider and DeKeyser, 2005).

In addition to oversimplistic categorization of grammar, research often lacks important information about learner background (Liu and Brown, 2015). Concerning the study by Yamashita (2021), 48 ESL learners from a United States university were examined. While information about proficiency was provided, levels varied and five could not be reported, since they lacked a score from a standardized exam like the TOEFL. The scores obtained led the researcher to conclude that “the sample was approximately upper-intermediate to advanced” (p. 80). These learners also came from various L1 backgrounds. While the participants were mostly Chinese (n = 20), other learners with Arabic, Indonesian, and Japanese L1s were represented. Concerning the study by Reynolds and Kao (2019), learners were recruited through a call for participants advertised at four schools of management in northern Taiwan with “ability to write at least 200 words in one sitting without major difficulties” (p. 466). No other information about participants was provided, leading to questions about L1 and English proficiency, both of which are known determinants of acquisition. As for the study by Rassaei (2017), all of the 57 learners had Persian L1s. Learners were described as “studying English at level 7 of an English program with level 12 as the highest level” (p. 138). Although this does give us some understanding of proficiency, more specific standardized assessment could allow for generalization to other learners of similar ability levels.

Examining the Multiple Factors of Acquisition

Before the significance of CF studies can be accurately assessed, multiple influences of acquisition must be examined and understood. First, grammar is more diverse than simple binary dichotomies would suggest, impacting the process of acquisition. According to the Processability Theory, for example, grammar varies based upon whether it modifies a single phrase (verb, noun, or adjective), multiple phrases, or larger clauses. More simplistic grammar that modifies a single phrase tends to be acquired first (e.g., plural -s, verb tenses, etc.). Next, grammar that expresses a relationship between multiple phrases begins to emerge (e.g., do support, subject/verb inversion, or phrasal verbs). Finally, larger clauses are used and manipulated at the sentence level (e.g., conditional sentences, relative clauses, or cancel inversion) (Pienemann and Lenzing, 2015).

Since some grammatical features are more difficult to use, learners must have a certain level of proficiency to acquire a grammatical feature. Research supports this assertion, suggesting that grammar given just above the level of an ESOL learner makes promotes acquisition (Dyson and Håkansson, 2017; Dyson, 2018). Other characteristics of the learner like L1 may influence the acquisition process, either serving to promote or hinder acquisition according to morphosyntactic similarities (Luk and Shirai, 2009; Shin, 2015; Yang et al., 2017). L1 processing routines have even been shown to be effective in promoting the acquisition of L2 grammar (McManus and Marsden, 2019).

A final influence on acquisition is the CF itself. Type of CF may influence how a grammatical feature is acquired. Reformulations, for example, provide a scaffold, since a correction is given. Prompts, in contrast, push the learner to correct their own error, making the process of acquisition more cognitively demanding. Explicit forms of CF, which overtly direct attention to a language form (e.g., all forms of written feedback) may also influence the efficacy of CF (Li and Vuono, 2019). Despite potential influences, past studies have not systematically examined each type of CF with a variety of grammatical features. Research by Lyster et al. (2013) goes further to explore different types of grammatical features with CF, yet it still conceptualizes target features with an overly simplistic dichotomy based on grammatical and lexical characteristics. Furthermore, discussion of treatment efficacy and grammatical feature type is based primarily on the target feature itself, lacking related information on learner background variables like proficiency, which may ultimately impact the difficulty level of a grammatical feature (and subsequent effectiveness of a treatment).

A Need for a Holistic Perspective in Examination of Corrective Feedback

Without consistent examination of all factors that influence the efficacy of CF (grammatical complexity, proficiency, and L1), it is no surprise that results are often inconsistent. Some researchers, for example, argue that written CF is effective (Bitchener et al., 2005; Chen et al., 2016), whereas others suggest that it is largely unnecessary (Truscott, 1996, 1999). Some scholars contend that oral recasts are very useful (Sakai, 2011; Goo and Mackey, 2013; Banaruee et al., 2018), while others argue that “The acquisitional value of recasts in comparison to other forms of corrective feedback might have been overestimated” (Ellis and Sheen, 2006, p. 575). Past experimental studies have failed to provide a comprehensive understanding of the impact of CF, which is ultimately needed to adapt theory to practice.

To provide a more holistic perspective of multiple variables influencing the acquisition process, studies of CF were synthesized for learners with Korean, Persian, and Chinese L1s (Schenck, 2020, 2021, 2022b). Results suggest a potential pedagogical application of theory to practice. The effectiveness of different types of CF on accuracy of production (implicit knowledge in assessments of either writing or speaking) appeared to be impacted by grammatical difficulty, which was defined by the type of grammatical feature, along with learner characteristics of proficiency and L1. Concerning the past regular tense, for example, Korean learners who tend to be at lower levels of proficiency had average effect sizes that were larger for implicit reformulations, in the form of oral recasts (Schenck, 2020). While this information is insightful, differences in research methodology between the two studies averaged for analysis (Cho, 2012; Kim and Cho, 2017) need to be considered. In the study by Kim and Cho (2017), for example, only two thirds of the learners examined were at the beginner level, with the remainder being labeled intermediate. Furthermore, only the study by Cho (2012) contained treatments with both recasts and prompts; from this study, results revealed that improvements in accuracy due to recasts were either higher (for the past irregular tense) or nearly the same as the prompt (for the past regular -ed). Overall, average effect sizes for both regular and irregular past tenses were higher for recasts, which appears to suggest that such recasts are more effective when emphasizing the past tense at lower proficiency levels. While insightful, variability in research methodology of past experimental studies also reveals a need for further, more holistic experimental examination of FFI.

Both Chinese and Persian learners, all of whom were at intermediate levels of proficiency, benefited more from implicit oral prompts (Yang and Lyster, 2010; Khezrlou, 2019). These studies suggest that more simplistic, intra-phrasal features like the past tense require a scaffold (a correction) at beginner levels, whereas more cognitively demanding prompts are required at intermediate levels. In addition to this finding, research synthesis also suggests that lower-level learners benefit more from explicit CF, which promotes conscious consideration of the target feature (Schenck, 2022b). Overall, findings appear to suggest that recasts and explicit techniques serve as a form of scaffold earlier in the process of acquisition, helping lower-level learners produce the target feature more accurately. Prompts, in contrast, tend to be more effective with learners at higher levels, who may have sufficient knowledge of the target feature and may not need a scaffold (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Type of assistance provided by each form of corrected feedback.

It appears that prompts are more effective when proficiency and competence with a feature increase, while scaffolded reformulations are more effective when proficiency is low and grammatical difficulty is high. Explicit FFI also appears to be more effective than implicit FFI, suggesting that this characteristic of CF also serves as a scaffold.

Similar Patterns in Input-Based and Output-Based Instruction

As in the case of CF, results of other FFI techniques that emphasize either input or output have been relatively inconsistent. Some studies suggest that modification of input increases grammatical accuracy and noticing (Jourdenais et al., 1995; Lee, 2007), whereas other studies reveal that such modification is rather inconsequential (Leow et al., 2003; Lee and Huang, 2008). Several studies suggest that facilitating output helps learners to acquire grammatical features and vocabulary (Izumi, 2002; Shintani, 2011; Rassaei, 2012), yet other research implies that output-based activities do not adequately engage the learner (Izumi and Bigelow, 2000; Izumi and Izumi, 2004). As past studies reveal, the true efficacy of FFI techniques that emphasize either input or output has yet to be realized. As in the case of CF, more holistic examination of grammatical difficulty (as defined by the combination of grammatical complexity, learner proficiency, and L1) and type of instruction (input vs. output) could lead to new insights, which allow educators to choose the best educational technique at the right time.

As in the case of CF, research synthesis has provided new insights concerning other forms of FFI. Effects of FFI techniques appear to be influenced by the following characteristics: input-providing, output-prompting, or degree of explicitness. Recent synthesis of 37 experimental studies (19 studies having learners with the Korean L1 and 18 studies having learners with the Persian L1), revealed some parallels to results from the collation of CF studies, with input serving as a type of scaffold for more difficult grammar at lower levels of proficiency (Schenck, 2022a). Studies which included grammatical features of a higher complexity with lower-level learners tended to benefit more from input. This perspective is supported by Modirkhamene et al. (2018), who found that Persian learners at beginner proficiency had higher effect sizes for explicit input via Processing Instruction (PI) (d = 0.70) and lower effect sizes for output (d = 0.52). The finding appears to parallel results found concerning CF. When the past tense is emphasized with beginner learners, input appears to be more effective. In addition to the past -ed, other features appear to reveal a relationship between grammatical feature, proficiency, and the benefits of input. In a study by Yang (2008), for example, the present perfect progressive, an inter-phrasal feature, was used with beginning level learners with TOEIC scores of approximately 140–185. Once again, low level learners appear to benefit from input, which may prime the learner, providing essential information to assist with processing and production of a grammatical feature.

Studies that revealed a larger benefit for output-based instruction may also be explained by examining characteristics of the target feature and learner background. In studies of relatively simple, intra-phrasal features like the participial adjective (boring/bored), low intermediate or intermediate learners benefited more from output (Kim, 2002; Yeo, 2002). It appears that low complexity and high proficiency result in larger effects for output, which may push learners to correctly use grammatical features that they are already familiar with. This pattern is repeated in other studies. In a study of high intermediate Korean learners by Jeong and Lee (2018), for example, intra-phrasal features (vocabulary) were emphasized with high school students of “high ability.” In another study of high intermediate and advanced learners by Kim and Nam (2017), more simplistic inter-phrasal collocations like “shake a leg” or “hit the sack” were emphasized. In both cases, output was more effective, further suggesting that low complexity and high proficiency influenced the findings.

Research synthesis of FFI research appears to explain when either input or output-based instruction will be more effective for a specific target feature. As in the case of CF, input associated with other forms of FFI appear most effective when proficiency is low and grammatical difficulty is high. Explicit forms of FFI (PI, explicit grammar instruction, and input enhancement) also tend to have larger effect sizes (Schenck, 2022a), which suggests that the explicit attribute may serve as a scaffold. Overall, results from synthesis of FFI studies may suggest a key link between grammatical difficulty and instruction, which parallels CF (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Relationship between grammatical difficulty and instruction.

Review of Table 2 links grammatical difficulty (grammatical complexity, learner proficiency, and L1) with instruction (type of FFI). While insightful, more experimental research is needed. Although research synthesis appears to show relationships between these variables, differences in research methodologies, which ultimately use different treatments and assessments of production, may yield variable results. Experimental studies are required that more holistically explore relationships between variables. In addition, while the effects of different pedagogical techniques that use explicit input (e.g., textual enhancement and PI) appear to be related, little past research has explored such potential relationships, further highlighting a need for additional research. Finally, past studies concerning both CF and other forms of FFI used the same grammatical features repeatedly (e.g., past tense, article, and conditional). By choosing a target feature based upon either intuition or experimental tradition, our understanding of acquisition has remained limited.

Future Research: Establishing a “Corpus” of Experimental Studies

As revealed by recent synthesis of past research, problems with using the right teaching technique at the right time appears to rest in an incomplete understanding of how grammar is acquired. By looking more holistically at grammatical difficulty (defined collectively as grammatical complexity, learner proficiency, and L1), selection of instructional techniques that maximize acquisition becomes an attainable goal. This also has implications for the design of computer software, which can rapidly modify instruction based upon a learner’s proficiency and L1.

It is important to remember that, while research synthesis provides useful insights, understanding of FFI remains limited. Although some tendencies for FFI effectiveness may be gleaned through collation of past research, studies used for examination contain methodological differences that may lead to variability in results. Thus, a more comprehensive and systematic experimental method is needed to gain a holistic understanding, which can allow computer software to provide emphasis at the right time. Rather than envisioning one research study that incorporates all grammatical features, diverse learners, and instructional style, the collection and collation of a large number of mini-studies may be the answer. Data collection at the individual level may be the key to success. For example, one learner may be presented with a reading and input enhancement for the past tense regular feature. Following a production task, this information can be carefully recorded into a corpus of FFI studies according to the following variables:

1. Amount of input (duration)

2. Kind of input (word count, lexical density, source, etc.)

3. Amount of output (duration)

4. Kind of output (e.g., dictogloss)

5. Target Feature (Complexity level and characteristics)

6. Kind of enhancement (input vs. output-based, degree of explicitness)

7. Proficiency level and L1 of the learner

After meticulously recording characteristics of one learner in a specific circumstance, results can be collated to provide the necessary holistic perspective. In addition to standardization of learner variables, different characteristics of instruction will need to be carefully scrutinized before being recorded into a corpus.

Although reform of research methods is clearly needed, the difficulty involved in creating one multivariate research study is daunting. If multiple variables involved in acquisition can be systematically considered in small-scale studies, they can be collated, thereby providing the holistic perspective needed adapt theory to practice. Computer programs may then be developed to tailor input and output to learner needs.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Banaruee, H., Khatin-Zadeh, O., and Ruegg, R. (2018). Recasts vs. direct corrective feedback on writing performance of high school EFL learners. Cogent Educ. 5, 1–23. doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2018.1455333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bitchener, J., Young, S., and Cameron, D. (2005). The effect of different types of corrective feedback on ESL student writing. J. Second Lang. Writ. 14, 191–205. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2005.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., Nassaji, H., and Liu, Q. (2016). EFL learners’ perceptions and preferences of written corrective feedback: a case study of university students from Mainland China. Asian Pac. J. Second Foreign Lang. Educ. 1:5. doi: 10.1186/s40862-016-0010-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, H. (2012). Relative effects of prompts and recasts on the development of implicit and explicit L2 knowledge. Engl. Teach. 67, 57–79. doi: 10.15858/engtea.67.4.201212.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyson, B. (2018). “Developmental sequences,” in The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching, ed. J. I. Liontas (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 1–8. doi: 10.1002/9781118784235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyson, B. P., and Håkansson, G. (2017). Understanding Second Language Processing: A Focus on Processability Theory, Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/bpa.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, R., and Sheen, Y. (2006). Reexamining the role of recasts in second language acquisition. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 28, 575–600. doi: 10.1017/S027226310606027X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschneider, J. M., and DeKeyser, R. M. (2005). Explaining the “natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition” in English: a meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Lang. Learn. 51, 1–50. doi: 10.1111/1467-9922.00147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goo, J., and Mackey, A. (2013). The case against the case against recasts. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 35, 127–165. doi: 10.1017/S0272263112000708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izumi, S. (2002). Output, input enhancement, and the noticing hypothesis: an experimental study on ESL relativization. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 24, 541–577.

Google Scholar

Izumi, S., and Bigelow, M. (2000). Does output promote noticing and second language acquisition? TESOL Q. 34, 239–278. doi: 10.2307/3587952

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izumi, Y., and Izumi, S. (2004). Investigating the effects of oral output on the learning of relative clauses in English: Issues in the psycholinguistic requirements for effective output tasks. Can. Mod. Lang. Rev. 60, 587–609. doi: 10.3138/cmlr.60.5.587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, H., and Lee, H. (2018). The effects of two types of written output tasks on noticing and learning of vocabulary. Korean J. Appl. Linguis. 34, 75–102. doi: 10.17154/kjal.2018.12.34.4.75

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jourdenais, R., Ota, M., Stauffer, S., Boyson, B., and Doughty, C. (1995). “Does textual enhancement promote noticing? A think-aloud protocol analysis,” in Attention and Awareness in Foreign Language Learning, ed. R. Schmidt (Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai’i Press), 183–216.

Google Scholar

Khezrlou, S. (2019). Task repetition and corrective feedback: the role of feedback types and structure saliency. Engl. Teach. Learn. 43, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s42321-019-00025-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, B. (2002). The role of focus on form in the EFL classroom setting: looking into recast and CR tasks. Engl. Teach. 57, 267–296.

Google Scholar

Kim, J. E., and Nam, H. (2017). The pedagogical relevance of processing instruction in second language idiom acquisition. Int. Rev. Appl. Linguis. Lang. Teach. 55, 93–132. doi: 10.1515/iral-2015-0027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., and Cho, Y. W. (2017). The effects of recasts and working memory on Korean EFL learners’ past tense accuracy. Engl. Teach. 72, 105–132. doi: 10.15858/engtea.72.4.201712.105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. K. (2007). Effects of textual enhancement and topic familiarity on Korean EFL students’ reading comprehension and learning of passive form. Lang. Learn. 57, 87–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00400.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. K., and Huang, H. T. (2008). Visual input enhancement and grammar learning: a meta-analytic review. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 30, 307–331. doi: 10.1017/S0272263108080479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leow, R. P., Egi, T., Nuevo, A. M., and Tsai, Y. C. (2003). The roles of textual enhancement and type of linguistic item in adult L2 learners’ comprehension and intake. Appl. Lang. Learn. 13, 1–16.

Google Scholar

Li, S., and Vuono, A. (2019). Twenty-five years of research on oral and written corrective feedback in System. System 84, 93–109. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2019.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q., and Brown, D. (2015). Methodological synthesis of research on the effectiveness of corrective feedback in L2 writing. J. Second Lang. Writ. 30, 66–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2015.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luk, Z. P. S., and Shirai, Y. (2009). Is the acquisition order of grammatical morphemes impervious to L1 knowledge? Evidence from the acquisition of plural -s, articles, and possessive’s. Lang. Learn. 59, 721–754. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00524.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyster, R., Saito, K., and Sato, M. (2013). Oral corrective feedback in second language classrooms. Lang. Teach. 46, 1–40. doi: 10.1017/S0261444812000365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McManus, K., and Marsden, E. (2019). Signatures of automaticity during practice: explicit instruction about L1 processing routines can improve L2 grammatical processing. Appl. Psycholinguist. 40, 205–234. doi: 10.1017/S0142716418000553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modirkhamene, S., Pouyan, A., and Alavinia, P. (2018). Processing instruction: learning complex grammar and writing accuracy through structured input activities. Indones. J. Appl. Linguist. 8, 177–188. doi: 10.17509/ijal.v8i1.11479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pienemann, M., and Lenzing, A. (2015). “Processability theory,” in Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction, 2nd Edn, eds B. VanPatten and J. Williams (New York, NY: Routledge), 159–179. doi: 10.4324/9780203628942

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rassaei, E. (2012). The effects of input-based and output-based instruction on L2 development. TESL EJ 16:3.

Google Scholar

Rassaei, E. (2017). Video chat vs. face-to-face recasts, learners’ interpretations and L2 development: a case of Persian EFL learners. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 30, 133–148. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2016.1275702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, B. L., and Kao, C. W. (2019). The effects of digital game-based instruction, teacher instruction, and direct focused written corrective feedback on the grammatical accuracy of English articles. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 34, 462–482. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2019.1617747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakai, H. (2011). Do recasts promote noticing the gap in L2 learning? Asian EFL J. Q. 13, 357–385.

Google Scholar

Schenck, A. (2020). Using meta-analysis of technique and timing to optimize corrective feedback for specific grammatical features. Asian Pac. J. Second Foreign Lang. Educ. 5:16. doi: 10.1186/s40862-020-00097-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenck, A. (2021). Defining grammatical difficulty to make better choices about corrective feedback: a meta-analysis of Persian EFL learners. SAGE Open 11, 1–13. doi: 10.1177/21582440211047558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenck, A. (2022b). How selection of a target feature impacts corrective feedback: finding answers through meta-analysis of Chinese EFL learners. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 45, 274–293. doi: 10.1515/CJAL-2022-0208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenck, A. (2022a). Examining the Impact of Form-Focused Instruction on L2 Acquisition of Diverse Grammatical Features of English (UCI No. I804:23006-200000603204). Ph.D. thesis. Incheon: Incheon National University.

Google Scholar

Shin, M. K. (2015). The resetting of the head direction parameter. Stud. Foreign Lang. Educ. 18, 17–35.

Google Scholar

Shintani, N. (2011). A comparative study of the effects of input-based and production-based instruction on vocabulary acquisition by young EFL learners. Lang. Teach. Res. 15, 137–158. doi: 10.1177/1362168810388692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shintani, N. (2012). Input-based tasks and the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar: a process-product study. Lang. Teach. Res. 16, 253–279. doi: 10.1177/1362168811431378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spada, N., and Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: a meta-analysis. Lang. Learn. 60, 263–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00562.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Lang. Learn. 46, 327–369. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1996.tb01238.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truscott, J. (1999). The case for “the case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes”: a response to Ferris. J. Second Lang. Writ. 8, 111–122. doi: 10.1016/S1060-3743(99)80124-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Guchte, M., Braaksma, M., Rijlaarsdam, G., and Bimmel, P. (2015). Learning new grammatical structures in task-based language learning: the effects of recasts and prompts. Mod. Lang. J. 99, 246–262. doi: 10.1111/modl.12211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varnosfadrani, A. D., and Basturkmen, H. (2009). The effectiveness of implicit and explicit error correction on learners’ performance. System 37, 82–98. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2008.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, T., and Jiang, L. (2015). The effects of written corrective feedback on Chinese EFL learners’ acquisition English collocations. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 38, 319–338. doi: 10.1515/cjal-2015-0020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamashita, T. (2021). Corrective feedback in computer-mediated collaborative writing and revision contributions. Lang. Learn. Technol. 25, 75–93.

Google Scholar

Yang, M. (2008). The effects of task difficulty on learners’ attention to meaning and form during focus-on-form instruction. Foreign Lang. Educ. 15, 27–52.

Google Scholar

Yang, M., Cooc, N., and Sheng, L. (2017). An investigation of cross-linguistic transfer between Chinese and English: a meta-analysis. Asian Pac. J. Second Foreign Lang. Educ. 2:15. doi: 10.1186/s40862-017-0036-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., and Lyster, R. (2010). Effects of form-focused practice and feedback on Chinese EFL students’ acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 32, 235–263. doi: 10.1017/S0272263109990519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, K. (2002). The effects of dictogloss: a technique of focus on form. Engl. Teach. 57, 149–167.

Google Scholar

Keywords: form-focused instruction, processability theory, L1, grammar, corrective feedback (CF)

Citation: Schenck A (2022) Form-Focused Instruction, Technology, and Limitations: An Investigation of Past Research and Future Possibilities. Front. Psychol. 13:920818. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.920818

Received: 15 April 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 23 June 2022.

Edited by:

Ehsan Rassaei, Majan University College, Oman

Reviewed by:

Hedayat Sarandi, Istanbul Sabahattin Zaim University, Turkey

Copyright © 2022 Schenck. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrew Schenck, Schenck@hotmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.