Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 June 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

The Eudemonic Wellbeing of Volunteers in a Public Health Emergency: COVID-19 in China

  • 1Faculty of Hospitality and Tourism Management, Macau University of Science and Technology, Macau, Macau SAR, China
  • 2School of Innovation and Entrepreneurship, Guangdong Polytechnic Normal University, Guangzhou, China

With improvements in the public awareness regarding volunteer opportunities, more people are participating in social work, particularly during emergency events. The mental health of volunteers has been attracting more academic attention due to its increasing social significance. Drawing on the Theory of Planned Behavior, a qualitative interview was conducted to identify important attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived control factors guiding people’s volunteering behaviors in an emergency context. Then, a sequential quantitative survey was implemented based on the results of the qualitative study to explore the impact of the aforementioned factors and job involvement on eudemonic well-being. The moderating role of empathy in these relationships was also investigated in this nested design. The results indicate that behavioral attitudes, perceived control, and job involvement have significant positive effects on volunteers’ eudemonic well-being. A high perspective taking (cognitive empathy) of volunteers positively moderates the relationship between job involvement and eudemonic well-being, while high personal distress (affective empathy) buffers this relationship. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed in relation to emergency volunteer activities.

Introduction

At the beginning of 2020, COVID-19 began to spread rapidly around the world and has been identified as a public health emergency by the World Health Organization (2020) (WHO), leading to the first-level response in many countries. With the rising public awareness regarding volunteer opportunities around the world (Crane, 2019), many volunteers offered their time and effort in this emergency response. In addition to the frontline doctors and nurses, volunteer teams were also deployed to perform emergency rescue services immediately. Being under the constant fear of exposure and uncertainty regarding the possibility of infection, the mental state of volunteers was intensively challenged in this severe public health event.

During and after the service, return of the volunteers back to their homes was identified as “not easy,” as some were required to be in “isolation” before re-entry (Curtin and Brown, 2019, p. 192). As such, pathological issues and psychological well-being (PWB) of volunteers were extensively investigated (Keyes, 2005), and many pathological outcomes were found. With the advent of positive psychology in the past few decades, a shifted focus on well-being or overall happiness was more embraced in the hope of mobilizing an individual’s internal capacity or strength to achieve people’s best performance. Or rather, well-being is regarded as an integral human pursuit (Tang and Wang, 2020). Eudemonic well-being (EWB) is one of the main concepts accounting for happiness with a focus on the meaning of life and self-development (Dolan and Metcalfe, 2012). An understanding of their EWB is crucial, particularly after their recent volunteer experience. This is not only for the good of their own psychological balance, but also for more sustainable development of volunteer organizations, which normally suffer from a shortage of talents, wherein “it is generally much easier for volunteers to quit their volunteer employer than it is for salaried workers to quit theirs” (Alfes et al., 2016, p. 596).

Well-being and “authentic happiness” can be achieved through being absorbed into activities and meaning-making tasks, as posited in Seligman (2011) PERMA block building theory (Seligman, 2011). In other words, proper involvement in meaningful cause is believed to enhance people’s well-being. Controversially, it was identified that improper involvement, such as over-involvement, was found to be detrimental, thus leading to many negative outcomes in the workplace and personal life (Claxton et al., 2017; Edmondson et al., 2019). Risky volunteer service can elevate some mental health issues (Milligan-Saville et al., 2018). Regretfully, rare empirical studies could be found so far about whether the meaning-making task in the context of volunteering in general, or public emergency crisis in particular, can contribute to volunteers’ EWB. To fight against possible adverse circumstances, empathy was identified to benefit people by allowing them to forgo some unpleasant experiences and offset the challenging stress (Muralidharan and Kim, 2020), thus mitigating some psychological reactance (Shen, 2010). As a valuable human quality in volunteerism (Butler and Tomazos, 2011), empathy was tested as a moderator that can regulate some behavioral variables and emotional responses or valuable orientation in different contexts (e.g., Lindsey et al., 2007). Despite the urgency and the importance of better understanding the ways volunteers reenergize or invigorate themselves into a challenging work environment through involvement, prior academic research remains sparse about its exerting influence on their EWB moderated by empathy.

To address these gaps, this study aims to measure volunteers’ EWB after they finish their recent volunteer job in a public health emergency, soliciting volunteers’ responses after their volunteering services in Wuhan of China during the first phase of the pandemic outbreak. We further seek to identify volunteers’ behavioral attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived control drawing on the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1991) and investigate their respective relationships with EWB. What is more, the moderating role of empathy between volunteers’ job involvement and EWB in public health emergencies got substantiated.

Theoretical Background and Hypothesis

Eudemonic Well-Being

Eudemonia was conceptualized as personal expressiveness to represent happiness along with hedonic enjoyment. When individuals strive to live based on the notion of their “true” selves, and when they realize their potential (self-actualization), “personal expressiveness” will occur. In other words, happiness occurs when people engage in activities that best match their deep-seated values and in which they could invest wholeheartedly (Waterman, 1993).

Although the measurement of EWB was described to be “less strictly defined” (Vanhoutte, 2014, p. 5) compared to other well-being concepts, such as hedonic, still, as summarized by Vanhoutte (2014), there are mainly three different measurement approaches. One is to conceptualize it into six dimensions by Ryff and Keyes (1995), which include autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. The second is underpinned by self-determination theory with a focus on three dimensions: autonomy, competence, and relatedness (Ryan and Deci, 2000). The third is often short for CASP-19 (Hyde et al., 2003), which consists of control, autonomy, self-realization, and pleasure. Among them, the first approach is deemed relatively more suitable in the context of the volunteer domain given the following reasons.

First, although some common or similar dimensions were shared, a distinction can still be spotted. The dimension of pleasure included in the third approach seems not to be appropriate in a public health emergency like the COVID-19 pandemic. Furthermore, life purpose and environmental mastery are only presented in the first approach, which fit right into the mechanism of enhancing well-being in volunteerism. The feeling of making a difference by doing altruistic things triggers positive well-being (Piliavin and Siegl, 2007). Herein, life purpose is a vital dimension for this study. Environmental mastery was defined as an individual’s ability to “control and manage one’s environment” (Hill and Allemand, 2010, p. 246). Confronting the challenging and critical environment on a voluntary basis brings up rare and valuable qualities. Besides, the first approach of six-dimension measurement was previously adopted and tested empirically in volunteer-related studies (e.g., Son and Wilson, 2012). Based on the aforementioned findings, the first approach of dimensionality is deemed appropriate for this study.

Theory of Planned Behavior and Well-Being

Ajzen’s (1991) TPB is an established behavioral theory for exploring determinants accounting for an individual’s behavior intentions based on a premise that decision-making is rational (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). The theory introduces three variables, including behavioral attitudes (AT), subjective norms (SN), and perceived control (PC). AT is related to a person’s evaluation of the performance of certain behavior. SN refer to the perceived or actual social pressure exerted by others, normally coming from people with less social distance known as “significant others,” such as family members, teachers, industrial managers, and other important reference figures in the decision-making process (Ajzen, 1991; Goh and Lee, 2018). PC is associated with the perception of the difficulty or ease of performing certain behavior (Ajzen, 1991).

Theory of Planned Behavior is widely adopted in examining various behaviors in the social science domain, including volunteerism, and is empirically identified as a more viable tool in explaining volunteers’ participation with better predictive validity than other approaches (Greenslade and White, 2005). People’s attitude toward a particular event predicts their personal growth and purpose in life (Lo et al., 2016). To comply with important referents is a normative belief. For volunteers, being seen as a “hero” (Tomazos and Butler, 2010, p. 363) in many situations represents an honor, which may further strengthen their EWB. As a cognitive psychological resource (Bandura, 2001), an individual’s perceived control can decrease personal stress (Cohen, 1980), so as to avoid harm to mental health. It was found to buffer the negative effects of accumulative stress by transforming to subjective well-being (SWB) (Jibeen, 2019).

Unlike many previous studies that focus on participants’ intentions as the outcome variables in TPB theory, we argue for the value of their EWB after their recent volunteer experience in the emergency event. We seek to understand the antecedent factors that induced them to volunteer in a public health emergency. Given this, the following hypotheses were proposed for volunteers who participated in a public health emergency:

H1a: Volunteers’ attitude has a significant and positive effect on their EWB after their completion of volunteer participation.

H1b: Volunteers’ subjective norms have a significant and positive effect on their EWB after their completion of volunteer participation.

H1c: Volunteers’ perceived control has a significant and positive effect on their EWB after their completion of volunteer participation.

Job Involvement and Well-Being

Involvement is defined as the extent to which an individual can internalize values of goodness or the importance of work (Lodahl and Kejnar, 1965), which is manifested by the amount of physical and psychological energy devoted to the job (Wolf-Wendel et al., 2009). Job involvement was first proposed by Allport (1943) as a type of job attitude, but later on, Kanungo (1982) definition dominated the discourse of the literature (Salessi and Omar, 2019), with a focus on the degree to which individual values and identifies his job. This term is sometimes used interchangeably with engagement (Hallberg and Schaufeli, 2006), with a common emphasis on the level of absorption and engrossment. In this study, it refers to the degree to which volunteer participants value and identify their volunteer tasks during the emergency event.

The positive relationship between job involvement and well-being has been extensively substantiated by researchers (e.g., Srivastava and Krishna, 1992; Srivastava, 2001). Besides, it receives solid support from Seligman (2011) PERMA block building theory, which asserts that well-being and “authentic happiness” can be achieved through being absorbed into activities and meaning-making tasks. However, the relationship between involvement and enhanced well-being remains ambiguous, with very few studies rendering direct empirical support in the context of volunteerism. On one hand, the adversities and challenging difficulties in public health emergencies might confound or complicate this relationship. On the other hand, the degree of involvement in this context may further add complications to this relationship. For instance, over-involvement in the workplace was found to suppress the mental health in an elementary school (Gechman and Wiener, 1975). This finding was somewhat different from that of Riipinen (1997), who stated that job involvement and well-being are conditioned by need fulfillment. Similarly, an indirect and positive relationship was also identified between job involvement and SWB through the experience of flow in work (Zhou et al., 2019).

Lodahl and Kejnar (1965) job involvement scale has been widely acknowledged. Subsequent adaptations were made afterward (Kanungo, 1982; Reeve and Smith, 2001). A 10-item Job Involvement Questionnaire (JIQ) was developed and validated as a simple version (Kanungo, 1982), exhibiting high internal consistency, validity, and reliability (Paterson and O’ Driscoll, 1990). Moreover, this JIQ is suggested to be suitable to measure psychological identification at the individual level (Igbaria et al., 1994). Thus this measurement was adopted by our study.

Based on the above-mentioned discussion, we propose the following hypothesis:

H2: Volunteer’s job involvement has a significant and positive effect on their EWB after their completion of volunteer participation in a public health emergency.

Empathy

Empathy plays a vital role in contributing to ethical life, moral judgment, and prosocial behavior (Eisenberg, 2000). The interdisciplinary academic effort, ranging from neuroscience, psychotherapy, psychology, and philosophy, has been made, viewing empathy as a kind of phenomenon, emotional reaction, trait, or capability. Empathy was normally seen as a vicarious response to another person’s emotional state or condition (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1990). It was characterized by being able to resonate with another person’s mind of state. Many researchers believe that empathy is an interaction between both facets, that is, emotional and cognitive (e.g., Coke et al., 1978). In a similar vein, Stueber (2013) contends that there are two striking features of empathy: ability to recognize other people’s feelings and ability to engage with others.

Stemming from the very nature of caring for other people in a social relationship, empathy has been employed to understand various helping behaviors to maintain social justice, promote well-being, and facilitate altruistic behavior. In addition, it has often been entangled and interchangeably used with other similar terms, such as sympathy or compassion (e.g., Eisenberg and Miller, 1987). Nevertheless, it is pointed out that sympathy was tapped by one of the empathy’s sub-scales, namely, emphatic concern (Davis, 1983). When filled with empathy, people’s empathic reaction will lead to two actions: one is compassion which is categorized by other related good feelings such as warmth and love, and the other is empathic distress which taps self-related negative feelings such as personal stress (Singer and Klimecki, 2014). Given this, empathy is employed as an umbrella term in this study.

In recognizing its full-fledged theoretical implication, a multi-dimensional approach is mainly embraced by studies in philosophy and behavioral science instead of a unipolar one. Among many options, Davis (1983) Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was identified as a valid and reliable instrument (e.g., Pulos et al., 2004), which taps the two important facets in a comprehensive fashion with four sub-scales, including perspective taking (PT), fantasy (FS), empathic concern (EC), and personal distress (PD). This IRI instrument remains rigorous when getting tested in a cross-cultural context (Karyagina and Kukhtova, 2016). As suggested, each sub-scale of IRI is a separate facet of empathy and can be singled out to be independently used (Thomas et al., 2007). This study adopts PT and PD to capture volunteers’ cognitive and emotional facets. As the only cognitive facet, PT is seen as a core component of empathy (Davis, 2018), which plays a positive role in enhancing well-being (Gleichgerrcht and Decety, 2013). Another study confirmed the interaction effect of PT with human responsibility in predicting the positive helping of a victim (Marjanovic et al., 2012). As a component of emotional empathy, PD taps the negative effect of the exposure to disaster (Heath et al., 2020). This is deemed essential to unpack the mental state after the completion of a stressful emergency task. The moderating role of PD was confirmed among volunteers in a prior study, revealing that low PD is beneficial for volunteers’ emotional relief in disaster aftermath (Cristea et al., 2014). Based on this finding, this study proposes the following hypotheses:

H3a: Volunteers’ PT significantly and positively moderates the effect of job involvement on their EWB. The effect is stronger when PT is high compared to when PT is low.

H3b: Volunteers’ PD significantly and negatively moderates the effect of job involvement on their EWB. The effect is weaker when PD is high compared to when PD is low.

The research model is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research model. Source: Authors.

Method

A sequential exploratory strategy of mixed methodology was employed with a research design of qualitative study followed by a quantitative one. The qualitative study aims to identify volunteers’ attitudes, social norms, and perceived control drawing upon the TPB from an in-depth interview. The subsequent quantitative study further contributes to testing the influence of the above-mentioned variables on the EWB of volunteers. This strategy can help identify the “elements of a theory” and extend the horizon by investigating the relative importance of these elements in improving the EWB of volunteers’, with authors investing great effort in overcoming the strategy’s “very time-consuming” nature (Terrell, 2012, p. 264). Furthermore, a nested method was adopted, with the identified attitudes, social norms, and perceived control embedded in the proposed normative framework, and further exploring their relationship with EWB. In this nested design, job involvement was integrated, accentuating its vital significance in internalizing the value of morality in expressing themselves in volunteering. Both the interview and survey were conducted within 1 month of the completion of their volunteer work in COVID-19, to ensure the recollection of their recent memory remained relatively fresh and clear.

Study 1: Qualitative Data from Interviews

Sampling and Interview Questions

A snowball sampling method (SSM) was used in our quantitative study. As a non-probability convenience technique, it is used when potential subjects are not easily identifiable (Petersen and Valdez, 2005) or when the research timeline needs to be efficient. In the initial stage, we reached five volunteers who were, respectively, from Shandong Province and Jiangsu Province medical assistance teams, the Wuhan hospitality industry, and the Sichuan volunteer group in China. Each of these organizations was established long enough and comprised experienced volunteers. They introduced the other 20 qualified volunteers to this study. Totally, we selected 25 experienced volunteers. As can be seen from Table 1, the respondents included 25 volunteers, covering job positions ranging from frontline health workers (5), transportation workers (2), vehicle convoy drivers (2), isolation area staff (2), catering and hospitality staff (7), community workers (4), to online psychological assistants (3), and all of them volunteered during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Most of them were between 21 and 35 years of age (68%). The total number of 25 volunteers met the sample size recommended by many TPB-related studies (N ≥ 25) (e.g., Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980; Godin and Kok, 1996; Goh and Lee, 2018).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Personal profiles of interviewees in study 1 (N = 25).

The interviews were conducted after obtaining informed consent. All interviews lasted for an average of 30 min. Online audio interviews were recorded with all the respondents to avoid direct human contact, as a precautionary measure due to the epidemic situation. All authors made joint efforts in transcribing and coding jobs to ensure accuracy and reliability. All interviews occurred from 10 April 2020 to 10 May 2020. Three interview questions were developed based on TPB (details of the interview questions included in study 1 are given in Appendix). Respondents were asked to identify their attitudes, list the influential social group, and outline any possible perceived difficulties in doing their volunteer jobs during the public health emergency, and how they dealt with those difficulties. Content analysis was conducted to extract possible themes with inductive techniques (Boyatzis, 1998). An initial pool consisting of 13 themes was generated under these three variables. We further invited two subject experts to go through the script and themes to ensure content validity. The finalized versions were adjusted to 12 themes, by combining “being away from family or friends” and “keeping secrets from family or friends” into one theme of “shunning of family/friends.”

Findings

Eight AT, eight SN, and nine PC themes were identified from the 25 respondents. The top four items for AT were altruism (72%), fulfilling one’s ambition (64%), mentally and emotionally exhausting (56%), and a sense of helplessness (52%). The top four items for SM were frontline health workers (80%), family and friends (52%), other volunteers (52%), and government/community/volunteer organizations (52%). The top four items for PC were high risk of infection (80%), shortage of supplies for epidemic prevention (72%), lack of effective medicine/therapeutic schedule (56%), and shunning of family/friends (56%). The findings of study 1 are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The results of study 1, antecedent factors of TPB.

Study 2: Quantitative Analysis

Sampling and Measures

Interviewees in study 1 were further asked to share the questionnaire link with their volunteer organizations, asking for their cooperation to distribute the link to as many as possible volunteers who participated in a recent public health emergency. Interviewees in study 1 were not included in study 2. The snowball sampling method was used, which was deemed appropriate to ensure efficient access to representative samples during the COVID-19 epidemic while social distancing was strictly enforced. The questionnaires were distributed and collected from 13 May 2020 to 20 June 2020. According to the normal distribution:

n = [ e r f - 1 ( C ) 2 × w ] 2 (1)

From WolframAlpha (2022)1, erf−1(0.95)was 1.38590, when confidence level (C) was 95% and confidence interval (w) was 5%. A minimal size (n) of about 384 respondents was needed for our research. Of the 411 questionnaires collected in study 2, 392 were found to be valid; thus, the effective rate was 95%.

The questionnaire consisted of five sections: TPB antecedent factors, JI, empathy, EWB, and demographic information, such as gender, age, and type of volunteering. The original question items were developed in English and translated into Chinese following the back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1980). The four main parts of the questionnaire were measured on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Negative items were scored in a reverse pattern. SPSS 23.0 and PROCESS macro v3.3 were used for data analysis. PROCESS macro can process traditional regression functions with interaction going “automatically and painless,” while yielding “substantively identical” results (Hayes et al., 2017, pp. 80–81). Furthermore, it is labeled as a “versatile modeling tool,” as it is capable of integrating multiple functions (Hayes, 2018). Besides, the PROCESS method determines the significance of interaction terms and calculates the degree of the moderating effect in high-level and low-level groups with a simple slope in a more readable manner. Template Model 1 in PROCESS, which illustrates a moderation relationship template, was adopted for this study.

The high-frequency (more than half) items that can present the opinion of the majority of participants of study 1 (Goh, 2009; Goh and Ritchie, 2011; Goh and Lee, 2018) were used to design questionnaire items in study 2. Regarding the attitude, typical statements were adopted, such as “overall my experience, this volunteering for this public health emergency was an altruistic behavior.” For the SN item, the typical statement was, “most people (frontline health workers, family and friends, etc.) who are important to me would encourage me to be volunteers in this public health emergency.” A typical statement for the measurement of PC was “I’ve control over being away or keeping secret from family and friends.” The JIQ (Kanungo, 1982) was adopted to measure the respondents’ volunteer job involvement. Six dimensions, including autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance, were used to evaluate the respondents’ EWB (Ryff, 1989; Ryff and Singer, 2008; Son and Wilson, 2012). The cognitive and emotional dimensions of the IRI, namely PT and PD, were used to measure empathy (Davis, 1983).

Personal Profile, Reliability, and Validity

Of all the respondents, 53.3% were men and 64.8% were young people (aged 18 to 30 years). As seen in Table 3, the Cronbach’s alpha of all the included constructs ranges between 0.776 and 0.973, exhibiting satisfactory reliability. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity were used to test construct validity. In this study, most KMO values were between 0.601 and 0.955, except for PT and PD which had a value of 0.500, which is probably due to the fact that both these two variables included only two items. The results show that the proposed scales indicated acceptable construct validity (Field, 2005). The above-mentioned relevant information is detailed in Tables 3, 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics of study 2 (N = 392).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Questionnaire items, reliability, validity, mean scores, and standard deviations of study 2.

Hypothesis Testing

Regression Analysis

We used linear regression analysis to identify the influence and extent of the influence of TPB antecedents and JI on EWB. Gender and age were computed as control variables.

Three regression models (Models 1, 2, and 3) are shown in Table 5 with EWB as outcome variables. Model 1 is significant (F = 18.257, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.191). The independent variables in Model 2 include AT, PC, and JI, with the significant outcomes (F = 85.685, p < 0.001) and higher explanatory power than that of Model 1 (R2 = 0.642). AT has a significant positive impact on EWB (β = 0.570, p < 0.01), as does PC (β = 0.488, p < 0.01). The impact of SN on EWB is not significant. Thus, Hypotheses 1a and 1c are supported, while Hypothesis 1b is not. Model 3 yields satisfactory explanatory power (R2 = 0.490), with JI serving as a significant independent variable, which indicates JI has a significant positive influence on EWB (β = 0.766, p < 0.001). Thus, Hypothesis 2 is supported.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of regression analysis and moderating effective test.

Moderating Effect Test

The results of moderating models (Model 4 and Model 5) in Table 5 show that both PT and PD exert a significant moderating effect on the relationship between JI and EWB (βJI*PT = 0.164, p < 0.05; βJI*PD = −0.052, p < 0.01).

We further tested these moderating effects on different levels, respectively, as shown in Table 6. Both a low PT and a high PT can positively moderate the positive effects of JI on EWB [SEPT(M–1SD) = 0.611, tPT(M–1SD) = 12.612, p < 0.001; SEPT(M+1SD) = 0.734, tPT(M+1SD) = 15.163, p < 0.001]. A similar finding was found with PD [SEPD(M–1SD) = 0.566, tPD(M–1SD) = 12.164, p < 0.001; SEPD(M+1SD) = 0.402, tPD(M+1SD) = 10.215, p < 0.001]. This result suggests that the positive effect of JI on EWB is positively moderated by both PT and PD. The results provide full support to Hypothesis H3a but partial support to Hypothesis H3b.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Moderating effects of PT and PD on different levels.

The simple slope plots (Figure 2) further reflect the degree of these moderating effects. A high PT positively moderates the influence of JI on EWB, whereas a weak moderating effect relaxes the same relationship. However, a high moderating effect of PD reduces the positive influence of JI on EWB when compared to a low moderating effect of PD. Thus, it can be seen that the positive effect of JI on EWB is conditional and dependent on the moderating effects of both PT and PD.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simple slope plots of interactions.

General Discussion

The contribution of this study is triple faceted. First, it extends the existing literature on volunteerism by particularly identifying the EWB of volunteers after their completion of volunteer tasks in the context of a public health emergency, seeking a better vision of their transformed life goals and personal growth from the perspectives of EWB. The completion of a volunteer task is likely to bring out a “turning point” (Starr, 1994, p. 137) or even “rebirth” (Tomazos and Butler, 2010, p. 363) for most volunteers. The disruption of life due to COVID-19 could last for a considerably longer period of time pervasive in many domains of human life, with a rising need for volunteers in a similar context. Second, a mixed method was designed in this study. This study is unique in that it used content analysis to generate a questionnaire that targeted the planned behaviors of volunteers. With a sequential and nested approach, this mixed study is expected to generate more insights into the mental state of volunteers (Chen and Chen, 2011). Volunteers’ dedication toward their job, “the significant others” who exerted social pressure on them, and volunteers’ perception of the difficulty were outlined in this research, together with the respective relationship with their EWB. This broadened understanding might serve as one of the valuable endeavors to unpack this group of unpaid workforces by exploring how they cope with pandemic-related uncertainties and challenges. Last but not least, this research also considers the role of empathy in the context of volunteerism in general, highlighting the regulating role of PT and PD in predicting volunteers’ EWB. So far, only a limited number of studies addressed the role of empathy as a moderator. This study joins in this limited stream of research by adopting this perspective and extending empathy to volunteerism in general and to volunteerism during the pandemic crisis in particular.

The high-frequency items from study 1 were extracted and framed into study 2. The list of attitudes that guide volunteer participation was generated, including altruism, fulfillment of ambitions, mental and emotional exhaustion, and a sense of helplessness. It can be seen that the volunteers mainly embrace other-oriented values to help and serve people in crisis, as altruism was listed as the most favorable appraisal of volunteerism. Fulfilling ambitions ranked second, which supports the argument that volunteering motives can be seen as a continuum between self-oriented and other-oriented values (Curtin and Brown, 2019), or between intrinsic and extrinsic orientation (Finkelstien, 2009). Volunteers’ attitude toward volunteerism can positively boost their EWB when completing their tasks, including even negative attitudes. It may be inferred that when volunteers take a retrospective look back on their journey, to be able to navigate and get over all the hardship presents a milestone or breakthrough for personal growth.

In terms of SN, frontline health workers, family and friends, other volunteers, and government/community/volunteer organizations were the top four sources of social pressure as revealed in study 1. In the public health emergency environment of the COVID-19 epidemic, the frontline health workers serve as the strongest reference group for volunteers according to study 1. It might be their dedication that highly motivates their peers. Volunteers respond actively to them, which indicates that there is still plenty of sizable room for volunteer organizations and governments to better engage with them and facilitate efficient volunteer recruitment. It is important to note that although volunteers are inclined to comply with these referents’ beliefs to gain possible approval, SN shows no direct influence on volunteers’ EWB in study 2. This is probably because people who participate in volunteering behavior tend to have high internal control and self-efficiency (Lee, 2018). It might be particularly the case for volunteers in public health emergency who normally have great willpower and perseverance to face the challenges.

Regarding PC, the high risk of infection, shortage of supplies for epidemic prevention, lack of effective medicine/therapeutic schedule, and being away or keeping secret from family/friends were perceived as the most serious difficulties by volunteers in study 1. This perception of control of beliefs can significantly contribute to volunteers’ EWB. As contended by Ajzen (1991), the controlling factors may be categorized as an internal one, which is related to self-efficacy and knowledge, and an external one, which is related to the environment. As coronavirus is highly contagious, some precautionary measurements, such as distribution of PPE and rationalizing the donated materials, can effectively ease the perceived difficulties and barriers during the task, consequently boosting the EWB of volunteers. Thus, volunteers’ mental health should be monitored regularly, and effective interventions need to be made available on a regular basis, not only before or during, but also at the post-pandemic stage.

Among all the dimensions of EWB, purpose in life and personal growth ranked at the top, with mean values of 6.550 and 6.462, respectively. It can be inferred that respondents have a fairly satisfactory level of EWB after completing their volunteering activities during the pandemic. This confirms our hypothesis that the effects of attitude and PC on volunteers’ EWB were significantly positive, while the impact of SN was not. Although SN offer guidance from references for volunteers, they do not directly influence EWB. JI has a significant positive effect on the volunteers’ EWB. The studies of Riipinen (1997) and Zhou et al. (2019) helped in shedding light on this finding. JI has a positive impact on the well-being of volunteers when they are fulfilled through sentiments of growth and development, which may further help ameliorate the psychological burdens of volunteers.

Although both PT and PD exhibit moderating effect on the influence of JI on EWB, their modulating power is inconsistent at different levels. Volunteers with a higher level of PT are more likely to experience better EWB than those with a lower level of PT. However, volunteers with a higher level of PD are less likely to experience positive EWB compared to those with a lower level of PD. Enhancing volunteers’ PT skills presents an effective strategy to help volunteers, particularly in public health emergency environment which needs a quick response or a large volume of assistance. It probably results from the fact that when volunteers have sensitive insight into other peoples’ feelings and thoughts, it is easier to engage in problem-solving activities more efficiently. This result is consistent with the research of López-Pérez (2013), which reports that professional health workers with higher PT enjoy better performance. As an affective dimension of empathy, lower PD is beneficial for the stress relief of volunteers in disaster aftermath (Cristea et al., 2014). Hence, governments and volunteer organizations should take precautions to channel their distress or regulate it within a reasonable capacity.

Positive JI is likely to exert a more positive EWB among volunteers with high PT compared to volunteers with a low PT during COVID-19 volunteering work. This finding denotes that volunteers with a high PT can benefit from a stronger relationship between JI and EWB. When the volunteers have the ability to look beyond themselves and extend their perspectives on other people, in other words, putting themselves in the recipients’ shoes and considering problems from multiple aspects, they are more likely to gain personal growth and find the purpose of their task. As an important component of affective empathy, PD does not appear to exert negative effects all the time as we previously assumed. A moderate level of PD can motivate them to be as adaptable as possible to a stressful environment to tap their potential. However, volunteers with a high PD would not enjoy that situation. It might be inferred that only the right amount of PD can help volunteers gain self-actualization.

Although efforts were made to diversify job types and demographics of volunteers for the purpose of sampling, the study could not be seen as entirely free of bias. In the future, broadening the volunteer job types, recruiting a larger sample, and comparing volunteers’ EWB among various geographic and cultural contexts warrant further academic attention. Certain factors, such as age cohort, educational level, income factor, personal health, and honor incentives, may also influence volunteers’ EWB in emergency events, and thus is worthy of further investigation. Besides, although this study did not aim to explore volunteers’ intention to participate further in a similar volunteering task in a near future, which was out of the scope of this study, longitudinal research is suggested in a middle-range or even long term for volunteers who ever participated in this unprecedented public health emergency rescue with their mental state and subsequent volunteering behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

JT, X-CL, and XZ were involved in the conceptualization, literature review, methodological design, investigation, data analysis, writing, and review. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://wolframalpha.com/

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Human Decis. Processes 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Hoboken, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Alfes, K., Shantz, A., and Bailey, C. (2016). Enhancing volunteer engagement to achieve desirable outcomes: What can non-profit employers do? Voluntas: Int. J. Volunt. Nonprofit Organ. 27, 595–617. doi: 10.1007/s11266-015-9601-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allport, G. W. (1943). The psychology of participation. Psychol. Rev. 52, 117–132. doi: 10.1037/h0056704

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyatzis, R. (1998). Transforming Qualitative Information: Thematic analysis and code development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1980). “Translation and content analysis of oral and written materials,” in Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology, eds H. C. Triandis and J. W. Berry (Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon), 389–444. doi: 10.3390/healthcare6030093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, R., and Tomazos, K. (2011). Volunteer tourism: altruism, empathy or self enhancement? New Problem Tourism 1, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Canniff, J. (1990). Development of the Volunteer Empathy Test. PhD dissertation. The University of Alberta, Canada.

Google Scholar

Chen, L., and Chen, J. (2011). The motivations and expectations of international volunteer tourists: A case study of “Chinese village traditions”. Tourism Manag. 32, 435–442. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2010.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claxton, M., Onwumere, J., and Fornells-Ambrojo, M. (2017). Do family interventions improve outcomes in early psychosis? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:371. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S. (1980). After-effects of stress on human performance and social behavior: a review of research and theory. Psychol. Bull. 88, 82–108.

Google Scholar

Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., and McDavis, R. (1978). Empathic mediation of helping; a two-stage model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36, 752–766. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.7.752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crane, M. (2019). The invisibles: The role of volunteer emergency service members in human health emergency response. Global Biosecurity 1, 116–118. doi: 10.31646/gbio.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cristea, I., Legge, E., Prosperi, M., Guazzelli, M., David, D., and Gentili, C. (2014). Moderating effects of empathic concern and personal distress on the emotional reactions of disaster volunteers. Disasters 38, 740–752. doi: 10.1111/disa.12075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtin, S., and Brown, L. (2019). Travelling with a purpose: an ethnographic study of the eudemonic experiences of volunteer expedition participants. Tourist Stud. 19, 192–214. doi: 10.1177/146879761880416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (2018). Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach. Milton Park: Routledge, doi: 10.4324/9780429493898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolan, P., and Metcalfe, R. (2012). Measuring subjective well-being: recommendations on measures for use by national governments. J. Soc. Policy 41, 409–427. doi: 10.1017/S0047279411000833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmondson, D. R., Matthews, L. M., and Ambrose, S. C. (2019). A meta-analytic review of emotional exhaustion in a sales context. J. Pers. Sell. Sales Manag. 39, 275–286.

Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N. (2000). “Empathy and sympathy,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones (Guilford, CT: Guilford), 677–691.

Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., and Fabes, R. (1990). Empathy: conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motiv. Emot. 14, 131–149. doi: 10.1007/BF00991640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., and Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 101, 91–119. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Finkelstien, M. A. (2009). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivational orientations and the volunteer process. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 46, 653–658. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gechman, A. S., and Wiener, Y. (1975). Job involvement and satisfaction as related to mental health and personal time devoted to work. J. Appl. Psychol. 60, 521–523. doi: 10.1037/h0076902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleichgerrcht, E., and Decety, J. (2013). Empathy in clinical practice: How individual dispositions, gender, and experience moderate empathic concern, burnout, and emotional distress in physicians. PLoS One 8:e61526. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godin, G., and Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behaviour: a review of its applications to health related behaviours. Am. J. Health Promo. 11, 87–98. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, E. (2009). Public education and parents: Eliciting salient beliefs using the theory of planned behaviour as a qualitative research framework. Int. Rev. Public Non. Mark. 6, 99–108. doi: 10.1007/s12208-009-0024-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, E., and Lee, C. (2018). A workforce to be reckoned with: the emerging pivotal generation z hospitality workforce. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 73, 20–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2018.01.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, E., and Ritchie, B. (2011). Using the theory of planned behavior to understand student attitudes and constraints toward attending field trips. J. Teach. Travel Tour. 11, 179–194. doi: 10.1080/15313220.2011.575024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenslade, J. H., and White, K. M. (2005). The prediction of above-average participation in volunteerism: a test of the theory of planned behavior and the volunteer’s functions inventory in older Australian adults. J. Soc. Psychol. 145, 155–172. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.145.2.155-172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallberg, U., and Schaufeli, W. (2006). “Same same” but different? Can work engagement be discriminated from job involvement and organizational commitment? Europ. Psychol. 11, 119–127. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.11.2.119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, 2nd Edn. Guilford, CT: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., Montoya, A. K., and Rockwood, N. J. (2017). The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: process versus structural equation modeling. Austr. Mark. J. 25, 76–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ausmj.2017.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heath, C., Sommerfield, A., and Von Ungerna-Sternberg, B. (2020). Resilience strategies to manage psychological distress among healthcare workers during the COVID-19 pandemic: a narrative review. Anaesthesia 75, 1364–1371. doi: 10.1111/anae.15180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, P. L., and Allemand, M. (2010). Forgivingness and adult patterns of individual differences in environmental mastery and personal growth. J. Res. Pers. 44, 245–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, M., Wiggins, R. D., Higgs, P., and Blane, D. B. (2003). A measure of quality of life in early old age: the theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging Mental Health 7, 186–194. doi: 10.1080/1360786031000101157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igbaria, M., Parasuraman, S., and Badawy, M. (1994). Work experiences, job involvement, and quality of work life among information systems personnel. MIS Q. 18, 175–201. doi: 10.2307/249764

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jibeen, T. (2019). Subjective well-being of afghan refugees in Pakistan: The moderating role of perceived control in married men. Community Ment. Health J. 55, 144–155. doi: 10.1007/s10597-018-0342-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanungo, R. (1982). Measurement of job and work involvement. J. Appl. Psychol. 67, 341–349. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.67.3.341

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karyagina, T. D., and Kukhtova, N. V. (2016). “M. Davis Empathy test: content validity and adaptation in cross-cultural context”. Couns. Psychol. Psychother. 24, 33–61. doi: 10.17759/cpp.2016240403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. J. Consul. Clin. Psychol. 73, 539–548. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. (2018). Roles of perceived behavioral control and self-efficacy to volunteer tourists’ intended participation via theory of planned behavior. Int. J. Tour. Res. 20, 182–190. doi: 10.1002/jtr.2171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindsey, L. L. M., Yun, K. A., and Hill, J. B. (2007). Anticipated guilt as motivation to help unknown others: An examination of empathy as a moderator. Commun. Res. 34, 468–480. doi: 10.1177/0093650207302789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, I., Chan, C., and Chan, T. (2016). Perceived importance of childbearing and attitudes toward assisted reproductive technology among Chinese lesbians in Hong Kong: Implications for psychological well-being. Fertility Sterility 106, 1221–1229. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2016.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodahl, T., and Kejnar, M. (1965). The definition and measurement of job involvement. J. Appl. psychol. 49, 24–33. doi: 10.1037/h0021692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Pérez, B. (2013). Feeling at hospitals: perspective-taking, empathy and personal distress among professional nurses and nursing students. Nurse Educ. Today 33, 334–338. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2013.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marjanovic, Z., Struthers, C., and Greenglass, E. (2012). Who helps natural-disaster victims? Assessment of trait and situational predictors. Analyses Soc. Issues Public Policy 12, 245–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2011.01262.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milligan-Saville, J., Choi, I., Deady, M., Scott, P., Tan, L., Calvo, R. A., et al. (2018). The impact of trauma exposure on the development of PTSD and psychological distress in a volunteer fire service. Psychiatry Res. 270, 1110–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muralidharan, S., and Kim, E. A. (2020). Can empathy offset low bystander efficacy? Effectiveness of domestic violence prevention narratives in India. Health commun. 35, 1229–1238. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2019.1623645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, J., and O’ Driscoll, M. (1990). An empirical assessment of Kanungo’s (1982) concept and measure of job involvement. Applied Psychol. 39, 293–306. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1990.tb01055.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R., and Valdez, A. (2005). Using snowball-based methods in hidden populations to generate a randomized community sample of gang-affiliated adolescents. Youth Viol. Juvenile Just. 3, 151–167. doi: 10.1177/1541204004273316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piliavin, J., and Siegl, E. (2007). Health benefits of volunteering in the Wisconsin longitudinal study. J. Health Soc. Behav. 48, 450–464. doi: 10.1177/002214650704800408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulos, S., Elison, J., and Lennon, R. (2004). The hierarchical structure of the interpersonal reactivity index. Soc. Behav. Pers. 32, 355–359. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2004.32.4.355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, C. L., and Smith, C. S. (2001). Refining Lodahl and Kejner’s job involvement scale with a convergent evidence approach: Applying multiple methods to multiple samples. Organ. Res. Method. 4, 91–111. doi: 10.1177/109442810142001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riipinen, M. (1997). The relationship between job involvement and well-being. J. Psychol. 131, 81–89. doi: 10.1080/00223989709603506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C., and Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C., and Singer, B. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: a eudemonic approach to psychological well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 13–39. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9019-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salessi, S., and Omar, A. (2019). Job involvement in current research: Update and state of the art. Papeles Del Psicól. 40, 46–56. doi: 10.23923/pap.psicol2019.2882

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A new understanding of happiness and well-being–And how to achieve them. London: Nicholas Brealy.

Google Scholar

Shen, L. (2010). Mitigating psychological reactance: The role of message-induced empathy in persuasion. Human Commun. Res. 36, 397–422.

Google Scholar

Singer, T., and Klimecki, O. M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Curr. Biol. 24, R875–R878. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Son, J., and Wilson, J. (2012). Volunteer work and hedonic, eudemonic, and social well-being. Sociol. Forum 27, 658–681. doi: 10.1111/j.1573-7861.2012.01340.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srivastava, A., and Krishna, A. (1992). Employees’ job involvement and mental health in public and private sector organizations: a comparative study. Indian J. Ind. Relat. 28, 62–70.

Google Scholar

Srivastava, S. K. (2001). Job involvement and mental health among executives and supervisors. J. Commun. Guid. Res. 18, 365–372.

Google Scholar

Starr, J. M. (1994). Peace corps service as a turning point. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 39, 137–161. doi: 10.2190/1206-9618-DFJ2-M6TX

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stueber, K. (2013). Empathy. Int. Encycl. Ethics. 1, 1606–1614. doi: 10.1002/9781444367072.wbiee736

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, J., and Wang, Y. (2020). Does tourism sports event make residents happier? – Exploring the SWB of Macau residents in the case of macau grand prix. J. Tour. Cult. Change 19, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/14766825.2019.1711103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrell, S. R. (2012). Mixed-methods research methodologies. Q. Rep. 17, 254–280.

Google Scholar

Thomas, M., Dyrbye, L., Huntington, J., Lawson, K., Novotny, P., Sloan, J., et al. (2007). How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicenter study. J. Gen. Int. Med. 22, 177–183. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-0039-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomazos, K., and Butler, R. (2010). The volunteer tourist as ‘hero’. Curr. Issues Tour. 13, 363–380. doi: 10.1080/13683500903038863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhoutte, B. (2014). The Multidimensional structure of subjective well-being in later life. J. Popul. Ageing 7, 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s12062-014-9092-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudemonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 678–691. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WolframAlpha (2022). Compute Expert-Level Answers Using Wolfram’s Breakthrough Algorithms, Knowledgebase and AI Technology. Available online at: https://www.wolframalpha.com/ (accessed April 25, 2022).

Google Scholar

Wolf-Wendel, L., Ward, K., and Kinzie, J. (2009). A tangled web of terms: the overlap and unique contribution of involvement, engagement, and integration to understanding college student success. J. College Stud. Dev. 50, 407–428. doi: 10.1353/csd.0.0077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2020). WHO Characterizes COVID-19 as a Pandemic. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Zhou, Q., Chen, G., and Liu, W. (2019). Impact of perceived organizational culture on job involvement and subjective well-being: a moderated mediation model. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 47, 1–13. doi: 10.2224/sbp.7478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix: Interview Questions

The Appendix comprises the interview questions included in study 1:

Question 1: What are the attitudes of volunteers toward volunteering behavior? (Positive/negative).

Question 2: Who are the important people/social groups that influence volunteers toward volunteering behavior?

Question 3: What are the perceived difficulties/barriers faced by volunteers toward volunteering behavior?

APPENDIX TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Appendix Table A1.

Keywords: public health emergency, volunteer, eudemonic wellbeing, job involvement, empathy

Citation: Tang J, Li X-c and Zhang X (2022) The Eudemonic Wellbeing of Volunteers in a Public Health Emergency: COVID-19 in China. Front. Psychol. 13:903147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.903147

Received: 23 March 2022; Accepted: 03 May 2022;
Published: 02 June 2022.

Edited by:

Wenchao Ma, University of Alabama, United States

Reviewed by:

Mingze Ma, Zhengzhou University, China
Taoran Liu, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Copyright © 2022 Tang, Li and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xi Zhang, zx58012@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.