Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 30 May 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars In: Environmental Psychology View all 10 articles

Pleistocene Hypothesis – Moving Savanna Perceptual Preference Hypothesis Beyond Savanna

\r\nJoachim Rathmann*Joachim Rathmann1*Kalevi M. KorpelaKalevi M. Korpela2Philipp StojakowitsPhilipp Stojakowits1
  • 1Institute of Geography, Augsburg University, Augsburg, Germany
  • 2Faculty of Social Sciences/Psychology, Tampere University, Tampere, Finland

We provide an extension of the Savanna perceptual preference hypothesis (“Savanna Hypothesis”), supposing that interaction with landscapes offering survival advantage for human groups during evolution might have gradually evolved to permanent landscape preferences. This additional support is based on the palaeoenvironmental analysis of the spread of modern humans into Europe in the late Pleistocene and their living environments there. Our hypothesis is that the preference for park-like landscapes after African savannas experienced a kind of “refreshment” in the Pleistocene. Thus, preferences for certain types of natural settings and scenes may have a more continuous evolutionary history than previously thought. The extended Savanna Hypothesis termed “Pleistocene Hypothesis” might stimulate further work on this important topic linking human evolution and human environmental preferences.

Introduction

In this paper, we provide an extension of the perceptual Savanna Hypothesis supposing that human interaction with landscapes offering survival advantage during evolution might have gradually developed into enduring patterns to prefer certain landscapes. This additional support is based on the palaeoenvironmental analysis of the spread of modern humans into Europe and their living environments there. Different factors play a role in this process (Klein, 2008). The role of climate forcing is one of them (e.g., Müller et al., 2011; Staubwasser et al., 2018). Another aspect is the innovation dynamic (Shennan, 2001; Richerson et al., 2009), i.e., the acquired unique abilities to colonize new environments (Hoffecker, 2009). The spread of early hominids can, however, also be explained by the acquisition of food, being essential for the development of the brain as the dispersal of early hominids is correlated to the historical migration of the African buffalo (van Ginneken et al., 2017; van Ginneken, 2019). In this context, theories on the evolution of the human brain should further discuss the current way of landscape perception in relation to that of the hominids (Chin et al., 2022).

But finally, when modern humans arrived in a new area what preferences did they have to answer the question: should I stay or should I go (further)?

The savanna hypothesis was originally formulated as an answer to why humans are bipedal. Thus, it places the agents of selection for bipedality on open grasslands, resulting from the transition of the human ancestors from an arboreal lifestyle to one on the savannas in response to the opening of the African landscape during long-term aridification of the continent (Richmond et al., 2001). The second aspect of this hypothesis focuses on the psychological processes assuming that due to the long-term hominization in the East African savanna, such environments are part of general landscape preferences of humans. The basic claim of the Savanna Hypothesis is that humans have innate, even automatic tendencies toward preferring certain types of natural settings that provide food, water, and security for survival (Ulrich, 1993). Ulrich (1983) has presented an integrated theory of esthetic and affective response to the natural environment assuming that landscape preferences can be defined as “the first level of reaction to the environment of generalized affect, such as liking or interest” simultaneous with or followed by approach-avoidance behavior (Ulrich, 1983, p. 90). These affective reactions to natural scenes are adaptive and foster well-being in terms of the total behavior of the individual. The initial affective reaction is elicited quickly by certain general properties or preferenda of the view, including gross structural aspects of settings, (e.g., focality, deflected vistas), gross depth properties that require little inference (spaciousness, ground surface texture conducive to movement) and general classes of environmental content, such as vegetation and water. Thus, in this theory, preferences are linked via preferenda to survival and well-being. Evolutionary psychology argues that many psychological mechanisms, such as preferences and emotions and behavioral strategies, are solutions to the adaptive problems our species has faced in natural evolution (Buss, 1995). We believe that the scientific basis of a widespread Savanna hypothesis requires further study and we focus on theoretical research of evolutionary biology theories.

Fear and Preference, Savanna Hypothesis and Prospect Refuge Theory

For the longest time in human history our ancestors lived in hunter–gatherer bands, thus being intimately connected to other living organisms (Wilson, 1984). Automatic reactions of fear and escape after the detection of predators were important agents of selection on ancestral humans (Öhman and Mineka, 2001). Many studies stress the evolutionary origins of human fear of some animals and show evidence that people detect the presence of fear-relevant animals (i.e., snake, spider) faster than the presence of a pleasant stimulus (i.e., flower, mushroom) (Öhman et al., 2001; Lipp et al., 2004). Even preschool children share this special attention to snakes and detect them faster than other stimuli (i.e., flowers, frogs, and caterpillars) (LoBue and DeLoache, 2008). Thus, it forms an evolutionary advantage to recognize dangers quickly and to react to them as efficiently as possible. Therefore, wilderness may also be partly associated with death and mortality (Koole and Van den Berg, 2005). On the other hand, research literature on preferences for natural settings shows that people generally prefer landscapes with a variety of landscape structures, open green spaces, differences in relief that enable views and orientation, as well as loose trees and the presence of water (Ulrich, 1993; Hill and Daniel, 2007).

Such preferences seem to relate not only to savanna but rather to mosaic environments (Domínguez-Rodrigo, 2014) and we develop this line of argument by first looking at the evidence on the preference of natural vs urban landscapes and then proceed to the preferred types and features of natural environments.

Several studies have consistently found empirical support for the assumption that people respond more positively to natural vs. urban environments (Staats et al., 2003; Van den Berg et al., 2003; Berto, 2005). Urban environments in these studies refer to human-made, built environments, such as streetscapes and buildings. Many links between human well-being and nature contacts have been analyzed and numerous studies have highlighted the positive effects of different kinds of green spaces on human health, well-being and quality of life (Hansmann et al., 2007; Abraham et al., 2010; Hartig et al., 2014; McMahan and Estes, 2015; Ideno et al., 2017; Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018; Bell et al., 2018; Grilli and Sacchelli, 2020; Rathmann et al., 2020; Rathmann, 2021). Supporting the notion of potentially hard-wired differences in brain activity, it has been shown that lower attentional demands for natural versus urban images can be detected within 1,200 milliseconds (Grassini et al., 2019; see also Norwood et al., 2019). Rapid positive emotional reactions to natural scenes versus negative reactions to urban scenes are evoked in 200 ms (Korpela et al., 2002). Savanna preference hypothesis also assumes that along with the preference reaction during evolution, a capacity for restorative, i.e., stress-reducing responding to certain natural settings has developed (Ulrich, 1993). This would foster amelioration of stress responses after encounters with danger and threats, such as predators. Ample evidence shows that stress-reducing physiological responses, indexed by, e.g., heart rate variability, salivary cortisol, blood pressure and pulse rate, are detected more rapidly after a stressful situation while walking or sitting in natural settings than in urban settings (Park et al., 2010; Bratman et al., 2012; Ideno et al., 2017; Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018; Mygind et al., 2019). The perception of environments’ “naturalness” and its biodiversity has also been associated with the restorative qualities of a setting (Carrus et al., 2015).

To continue to the preferred types and features of landscapes, the theory of prospect and refuge can be traced back to the late nineteenth century and the anthropological belief in the human survival instinct leading to a stimulus, which directly connects human perceptions or reactions to environmental stimuli. The basic idea, that hominids have been living in the Savanna environments has started to be discussed since 1960, at least in archeology and history (Bender et al., 2012), underlining the paleoanthropological perspective of the Savanna theory. The psychological perspective of this theory focuses on the needs of the hominids for survival.

The prospect-refuge theory, proposed by Appleton (1975, 1996), describes a human behavioral and psychological need for places that allow a person to see, but without being seen. The basic idea was to show a simple model, relating preferences to a typology of landscapes based on behavioral and biological sciences. The whole model was “an agent of simplification for explanatory purposes” (Appleton, 1984, p. 92). The reduction was made deliberately to facilitate an explanation. Environmental perception is the key to all adaptation processes and humans perceive their environment in another way than animals perceive their habitat. Some aspects of the habitat are more important for survival than others. “Prospect” and “refuge” are the important aspects of a landscape to improve the chances of survival. Further on, this theory tried to show a biological interpretation for landscape esthetics as it states that taste in art is an acquired preference for particular methods of satisfying inborn desires which are basically opportunity (prospect) and safety (refuge). Humans are attracted to specific circumstances (art, landscape) that have unoccluded vistas into the landscape (prospects further include hills, mountains and trees), visible places for easy refuge (e.g., climbable trees with dense canopies nearby, caves, dense vegetation) and additionally water, plants, prey species. Landscape preferences further include spaces, where we are rather on the edge than in the middle of a place, where we are most exposed and places where we are covered, compared to the open sky. Landscape preferences therefore focus on areas which are optimal for survival and reproduction. The Savanna Hypothesis (Orians, 1980; Bender et al., 2012) argues that selection favored resource-rich environments whereas environments, lacking resources or with survival threats have been avoided. Such environments offered the essential landscape characteristics for survival of the early humans; the availability of resources, protection against predators, the possibility of orientation and overview in space are central requirements for a landscape that ensures the survival of early humans. These theoretical conceptions actually propose that not only certain landscape contents but certain structural properties might have been important for the development of permanent preferences. One such property has been described as “depth/spaciousness” characteristics that relate to surveillance, proximity to hidden threats, and escape opportunities (Ulrich, 1993) or intermediate complexity or density of the scene (Joye and van den Berg, 2011). Such structural emphasis may also fit with alternative theoretical explanations for the Savanna Hypothesis, such as the Perceptual Fluency Account (PFA). It states that natural scenes are affectively evaluated more positively than urban scenes because our visual system more fluently processes certain aspects of the visual structure of the former than of the latter (Joye and van den Berg, 2011). In PFA, in contrast to psychophysiological and emotional theories supporting Savanna Hypothesis, it is the structure of the landscape, visual coherence and fractal patterns, rather than “unthreatening vegetated settings per se” that might explain preference of greenspace (Joye and van den Berg, 2011).

Emphasizing contents rather than structure, Heerwagen and Orians (1993) argue that our landscape preferences are innate, these preferences include open spaces of low grasses, the presence of water, flowering and fruiting plants, and evidence of animal life. Beside the prospect-refuge-theory and the Savanna Hypothesis, Kaplan’s landscape preference matrix theory (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989) and the stress recovery theory (Ulrich et al., 1991) argue that humans prefer natural green environments. More importantly and in line with the concept of mosaic environments, in addition to the Savanna hypothesis, two other hypotheses with respect to the specific habitat where humans have evolved have been presented (Han, 2007; Mangone et al., 2021). One option is the forest hypothesis, which argues that human evolution took place in closed, forested settings and the other one is the grassland–woodland hypothesis, which proposes that a mosaic of both settings was the adaptive environment for hominids.

Some of the empirical evidence for the preference for savanna environments is related to biomes as a whole but mainly it includes only some features of savanna environments. Concerning biomes, the evidence is mixed. In one study, photographs of five biomes, rain forest, deciduous forest, coniferous forest, savanna, and desert were rated by the inhabitants of the rainforest belt of Nigeria (Falk and Balling, 2010). The results showed that savanna scenes were regarded as the most favorable place to live. In another study, college students’ psycho-physiological responses to the six major terrestrial biomes (desert, tundra, grassland, coniferous forest, deciduous forest, and tropical forest) showed that tundra and coniferous forest were the most favored biomes, whereas desert and grassland were the least favored (Han, 2007). Yet another study with a student population showed that irrespective of familiarity, beaches and lakes were preferred more and marshes and swamps preferred less than the other six biome types (beach, lake, tropical and temperate forest, marsh, swamp, meadow, park as a representative of savanna, mountain, and river) (Mangone et al., 2021). Concerning features of savanna, there is evidence on cross-cultural preferences for acacia-like Savanna trees (Orians and Heerwagen, 1992). Lohr and Pearson-Mims (2006) consider people’s preference for trees with spreading cones comparable to an acacia. Sommer (1997) confirms this result within a cross-national study thus confirming the refuge dimension of Appleton’s theory as such a canopy can represent habitat and safety. Anecdotal evidence points to the fact that looking at trees outside a hospital room helps in recovering more quickly from hospital stay than patients looking at a brick wall (Ulrich, 1984).

Moreover, the general question of the degree of our landscape preferences and behavior being innate versus learned during childhood is a part of a long debate. Humans have adapted to a broad range of conditions as social, cultural and natural selection unfold in tandem (Hartig, 2021). Thus, landscape preferences might be determined by culture as some studies stress a relationship between childhood memories and preferences for specific environments (Van den Berg et al., 1998) and others emphasize cultural determinants (Bourassa, 1992; Howley, 2011; Joye and van den Berg, 2011; Ward Thompson, 2018; Silva et al., 2020; Hartig, 2021). Landscape preferences, explained by cultural-based elements must first explain the concept of landscape, and these discussions have a very long tradition, especially in geography (Cosgrove, 2004; Wylie, 2007). The evolution of landscapes must be regarded as a reciprocal interplay of both ecological and cultural “factors.” Carl Sauer’s much-cited work on Sauer (1925) stresses the active agency of culture in shaping landscapes, rejecting environ mental determinism. Further, landscape is the esthetically perceived environment, therefore it affects human wellbeing. Wellbeing is regarded as an important factor in pro-environmental behavior across different cultures and countries (Capstick et al., 2022) stressing the importance of human-nature bonds for environmental policy.

Due to the complexity of the concept of landscape, it must be distinguished from concepts such as place, space, or territory in a political context (Menatti and Casado da Rocha, 2016). Landscape is the existentially experienced environment as a result of history, reflecting economy and society. And so are landscape preferences in this perspective.

The discussions about the different concepts of landscape are far beyond the scope of this contribution but such concepts of landscape offer the possibility of reconnection ecology, and psychology to current humanities concerns with culture, identity, meaning, and even ethics (Menatti and Casado da Rocha, 2016). Environmental psychology is increasingly recognizing that human–environment interactions are culture-bound (Tam and Milfont, 2020), thus discussing the cultural factors in landscape perception, preferences, and individual wellbeing.

Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez (2013) confirm the role of familiarity as there are preferences for images of lush green landscapes with water and familiar biomes (see also Mangone et al., 2021). Human nature attachment has been related to both evolution and cultural bonds to places, as landscape preferences are related to places where human beings feel safe and at home (Adevi and Grahn, 2012). Differences in preferences for nature between demographic groups appear to be small (Stamps, 1999) but differences in landscape preferences between Western students and non-Western students have been found (Hägerhäll et al., 2018) or preferences for tropical forest landscapes (Moura et al., 2018). Also, age-related changes in landscape preferences have been found (Balling and Falk, 1982). Following from this “innate-learned debate” and the complex mix of social, cultural, and natural selection, our focus in this paper on a special preference for certain types of natural settings does not exclude or oppose potential preferences for (or evolutionary adaptation to) urban settings; neither do we maintain that preferences (= visual inclinations to prefer) for nature per se necessarily signify particular health benefits from nature (cf. Hartig, 2021). Although there is ample evidence that such health or wellbeing benefits do exist particularly in comparison to urban scenes (Abraham et al., 2010; Hartig et al., 2014; Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018; Mygind et al., 2019) we emphasize here Ulrich’s (1983) theoretical notion that an individual’s affective reaction to a natural scene serves only as an action impulse for adaptive behavior which can be suppressed or denied, based on experience and learning.

To conclude, empirical evidence for preference for savanna-like environments is only partial and several types and features of natural environments attracting human preference have been presented. Thus, a very important and interesting observation in this respect is Domínguez-Rodrigo’s (2014) notion, based on palaeoecological evidence, that Savannas should be regarded as mosaic environments and not as open grasslands.

Several studies also show that human evolution took place in different biomes and not only in savanna environments. Hominids evolved in East Africa in an ecologically diverse setting, including grassland, savanna and different forest structures (Kingston et al., 1994). But whereas the early Miocene mammalian faunas had a tropical-forest character, the Pliocene shows more faunas evolving a savanna-mosaic character (Cerling et al., 1997). But as the interactions between climate change, ecology, and evolution are rather complex, more studies are needed to investigate this complex interplay (Blumenthal et al., 2017).

We will further explore the possibilities of extending the Savanna Hypothesis including various environments with various structural properties, such as prospect and refuge, from an evolutionary perspective. With all the uncertainty in the reconstruction of the palaeoenvironmental conditions during hominization, the Savanna Hypothesis could be given new perspective by investigating the Pleistocene.

Beyond Savanna

In the warm periods of the Pleistocene, many of the northern hemispheric forest areas were apparently open woodlands with large open spaces, structurally somehow similar to Savannas. Based on the mega herbivore hypothesis, the forests were then under high grazing pressure from mega herbivores, i.e., large herbivores (e.g., forest elephants, forest rhinos, giant deer), which, depending on the author, are defined as weighing more than 800, 900 or 1,000 kg (Malhi et al., 2016; van Valkenburgh et al., 2016). During the Pleistocene cold periods, however, largely open landscapes with other mega herbivores (e.g., wooly mammoths, wooly rhinoceroses, musk oxen, giant sloths) dominated. Generally, an alternation between open steppe/tundra and forested landscapes took place affecting human evolution (Sanchez Goñi, 2020). Within a glacial period, warmer phases occurred in which not only tundra or steppe biomes were present, but also some patches of woodland and shrub stands leading to a complex mosaic of different plant communities. The best modern analog for this Pleistocene steppe-tundra environment is located in the Altai Mountains (Chytrý et al., 2019). Figure 1 illustrates the complex interactions of mankind, climate change, the impact of megaherbivores on the landscape structures and the subjective perception of landscape.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The simplified depiction of the interactions of mankind, climate change, the impact of mega herbivores on the landscape structures and the subjective perception of landscape. The focus is on the interactions between climate and the biosphere with human forcings affecting the environment and actively shaping their environment. Source: Winfried Weber, Institute of Geography and Geology, University of Würzburg, 2022. Reproduced with permission.

At the time of the last cold period, the early modern human settlement of southern Central Europe occurred during Marine Isotope Stage 3 in a medium-cold steppe-like environment with some boreal trees at climatically favorable sites (Nigst et al., 2014). The rare palynological studies of Southern Central Europe confirm this vegetation reconstruction (Brande, 1982; Müller et al., 2003; Stojakowits et al., 2021; Figure 2). Bielinis et al. (2018, 2019)—based on the salutogenic landscape preference framework—have shown that even snow-covered forests and broad–leafed forests in winter can trigger positive emotions and can lead to psychological relaxation. Another study suggests lowering of blood pressure and even immune system effects of forests in winter (Peterfalvi et al., 2021). Thus, we propose that Late Pleistocene environments might have partly contributed to the development of landscape preferences, although the most ancient preferences of Homo sapiens were formed in Africa during the two last glaciations (Riss and Würm) and interglacial stages (Mindel-Riss and Riss/Würm). The first traces of modern humans date to around 315,000 years ago (Richter et al., 2017).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Vegetation and palaeoclimatic development in comparison to the estimated population from c. 80 to 10 ka (x axis). According to Heiri et al. (2014), with additional palaeobotanical data (Müller et al., 2003) and estimated total number of people for the Upper Danube region until c. 42 ka (Müller-Beck, 1983), for the same region during the Aurignacian until c. 33 ka (Schmidt and Zimmermann, 2019), the early Gravettian until c. 29 ka and late Gravettian until c. 25 ka (Maier and Zimmermann, 2017), the LGM (Maier et al., 2016), and Magdalenian (Kretschmer, 2015; Maier, 2017). The Greenland ice core oxygen isotope record NGRIP is shown in the lower part of the figure according to Svensson et al. (2008), supplemented by Rasmussen et al. (2014).

According to genetic studies, modern humans likely mixed with Neanderthals (Prüfer et al., 2014). Both, the early modern humans and the Neanderthals were shaped by open landscapes over many generations. After the ice retreat Mesolithic tribes lived in open landscapes until the reforestation with Betula trees took place in the Bølling Interstadial at around 14.5 ka (Eusterhues et al., 2002). Later on, Pinus sylvestris also reimmigrated forming open pine-birch-forests until the onset of the Boreal at around 10.3 ka. Around 5,500 cal BC, the colonization of Neolithic tribes of the Linearbandkeramik (LBK) along the Danube started in Southern Central Europe (Gronenborn, 2004). These people used to live in open grass steppe landscapes as testified by pollen analyses (e.g., Wick et al., 2003; Litt et al., 2009). They created similar kinds of open landscapes in their newly settled areas on a small scale- which were densely forested at that time—in order to practice farming. Open lands are preferred settlement regions for humans over many thousands of years dating back to the Late Pleistocene. Although wilderness seems to be more fascinating than cultural landscapes (Barbiero and Berto, 2021) domestication of animals and plants during the Neolithic period might have contributed to favoring the cultural landscapes, over the wilderness landscapes.

Conclusion

Although the Savanna Hypothesis can be questioned in many ways, it may be extended by the integration of Pleistocene environments. Our hypothesis is, that the preference for mosaic, park-like landscapes experienced a kind of “refreshment” in the Pleistocene. We suggest a heuristic tool, as Appleton did, and not a comprehensive explanation of human behavior and perception, being aware that landscape preferences are not simply based on innate response to the environment. The extended Savanna Hypothesis, termed “Beyond-Savanna-Hypothesis” or according to our study “Pleistocene-Mosaic-Environments-Hypothesis,” might also stimulate work linking human evolution, human health, and general human-environment bond (c.f., Mangone et al., 2021; Chang et al., 2022). We propose empirical studies that would account for both genetic, environmental and developmental influences and their complex interactions on such preferences.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

JR: basic idea, literature review, and text. KK: text on psychology. PS: text on paleoenvironments. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abraham, A., Sommerhalder, K., and Abel, T. (2010). Landscape and well-being: a scoping study on the health-promoting impact of outdoor environments. Int. J. Public Health 55, 59–69. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-0069-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adevi, A. A., and Grahn, P. (2012). Preferences for landscapes: a matter of cultural determinants or innate reflexes that point to our evolutionary background?. Landsc. Res. 37, 27–49. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appleton, J. (1975). The Experience of Landscape. London: John Wiley.

Google Scholar

Appleton, J. (1984). Prospect and refuge re-visited. Landsc. J. 3, 91–103.

Google Scholar

Appleton, J. (1996). The Experience of Landscape. – Revised Version. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Google Scholar

Balling, J. D., and Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preference for natural environments. Environ. Behav. 14, 5–28.

Google Scholar

Barbiero, G., and Berto, R. (2021). Biophilia as evolutionary adaptation: an onto- and phylogenetic framework for biophilic design. Front. Psychol. 12:700709. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, S. L., Foley, R., Houghton, F., Maddrell, A., and Williams, A. M. (2018). From therapeutic landscapes to healthy spaces, places and practices: a scoping review. Soc. Sci. Med. 196, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.11.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, R., Tobias, P. V., and Bender, N. (2012). The savannah hypotheses: origin, reception and impact on paleoanthropology. Hist. Philos. Life Sci. 34, 147–184.

Google Scholar

Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 249–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bielinis, E., Omelan, A., Boiko, S., and Bielinis, L. (2019). The restorative effect of staying in a broad-leaves forest on healthy young adults in Winter and Spring. Baltic For. 24, 218–227.

Google Scholar

Bielinis, E., Takayama, N., Boiko, S., Omelan, A., and Bielinis, L. (2018). The effect of winter forest bathing on psychological relaxation of young polish adults. Urban For. Urban Green. 29, 276–283.

Google Scholar

Blumenthal, S. A., Levin, N. E., Brown, F. H., Brugal, J. P., Chritz, K. L., Harris, J. M., et al. (2017). Aridity and hominin environments. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 7331–7336. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700597114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourassa, S. (1992). The Aesthetics of Landscape. London: Bellhaven Press.

Google Scholar

Brande, A. (1982). Vegetation and landscape changes at the paleolithic site of Mauern (Bavaria), early man news 5/6. Newsl. Hum. Paleoecol. 3–5.

Google Scholar

Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P., and Daily, G. C. (2012). The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1249, 118–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06400.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: a new paradigm for psychological science. Psychol. Inq. 1:1.30.

Google Scholar

Capstick, S., Nash, N., Poortinga, W., Haggar, P., and Brügger, A. (2022). The connection between subjective wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviour: individual and cross-national characteristics in a seven-country study. Environ. Sci. Policy 133, 63–73.

Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Scopelliti, M., Lafortezza, R., Colangelo, G., Ferrini, F., and Salbitano, F. (2015). Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting urban and peri-urban green areas. Landsc.Urban Plan. 134, 221–228. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerling, T., Harris, J., MacFadden, B., Lea-key, M. G., Quade, J., Eh-leringer, J. R., et al. (1997). Global vegetation change through the Miocene/Pliocene boundary. Nature 389, 153–158. doi: 10.1007/s00114-008-0500-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C.-C., Cox, D. T. C., Fan, Q., Nghiem, T. P. L., Tan, C. L. Y., Oh, R. R. Y., et al. (2022). People’s desire to be in nature and how they experience it are partially heritable. PLoS Biol. 20:e3001500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, R., Chang, S. W. C., and Holmes, A. J. (2022). Beyond cortex: the evolution of the human brain. Psychol. Rev. [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1037/rev0000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chytrý, M., Horsák, M., Danihelka, J., Ermakov, N., German, D. A., Hájek, M., et al. (2019). A modern analogue of the Pleistocene steppe-tundra ecosystem in southern Siberia. Boreas 48, 36–56.

Google Scholar

Cosgrove, D. (2004). Landscape and Landschaft. Bull. German Hist. Inst. 35, 57–71.

Google Scholar

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. (2014). Is the “savanna hypothesis” a dead concept for explaining the emergence of the earliest hominins? Curr. Anthropol. 55, 59–81.

Google Scholar

Eusterhues, K., Lechterbeck, J., Schneider, J., and Wolf-Brozio, U. (2002). Late- and Post-Glacial evolution of Lake Steisslingen (I). Sedimentary history, palynological record and inorganic geochemical indicators. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 187, 341–371.

Google Scholar

Falk, J. H., and Balling, J. D. (2010). Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference. Environ. Behav. 42, 479–493.

Google Scholar

Grassini, S., Revonsuo, A., Castellotti, S., Petrizzo, I., Benedetti, V., and Koivisto, M. (2019). Processing of natural scenery is associated with lower attentional and cognitive load compared with urban ones. J. Environ. Psychol. 62, 1–11.

Google Scholar

Grilli, G., and Sacchelli, S. (2020). Health benefits derived from forest: a review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:6125. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17176125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gronenborn, D. (2004). Comparing contact-period archaeologies: the expansion of farming and pastoralist societies to continental temperate Europe and to southern Africa. Before Farm. 3, 1–35.

Google Scholar

Hägerhäll, C. M., Ode Sang, Å, Englund, J.-E., Ahlner, F., Rybka, K., Huber, J., et al. (2018). Do humans really prefer semi-open natural landscapes? A cross-cultural reappraisal. Front. Psychol. 9:822. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, K.-T. (2007). Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and restorativeness. Environ. Behav. 39, 529–556.

Google Scholar

Hansmann, R., Hug, S. M., and Seeland, K. (2007). Restoration and stress relief through physical activities in forests and parks. Urban For. Urban Green 6, 213–225. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2007.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T. (2021). “Restoration in nature: Beyond the conventional narrative,” in Nature and Psychology: Biological, Cognitive, Developmental, and Social, eds A. Schutte, J. Torquati, and J. Stevens (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 89–151.

Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., de Vries, S., and Frumkin, H. (2014). Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 35, 207–228.

Google Scholar

Hartmann, P., and Apaolaza-Ibáñez, V. (2013). Desert or rain: standardisation of green advertising versus adaptation to the target audience’s natural environment. Eur. J. Mark. 47, 917–933. doi: 10.1108/03090561311308091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heerwagen, J. H., and Orians, G. H. (1993). “Humans, habitats, and aesthetics,” in The Biophilia Hypothesis, eds S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (Washington, DC: Washington Island Press), 138–172.

Google Scholar

Heiri, O., Koinig, K. A., Spötl, C., Barrett, S. J., Brauer, A., Drescher-Schneider, R., et al. (2014). Palaeoclimate records 60–8 ka in the Austrian and Swiss Alps and their forelands. Quat. Sci. Rev. 106, 186–205.

Google Scholar

Hill, D., and Daniel, T. C. (2007). Foundations for an ecological aesthetic: Can information alter landscape preferences? Soc. Nat. Resour. 1, 34–49. doi: 10.1080/08941920701655700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffecker, J. F. (2009). The spread of modern humans in Europe. PNAS 106, 16040–16045.

Google Scholar

Howley, P. (2011). Landscape aesthetics: assessing the general publics’ preferences towards rural landscapes. Ecol. Econ. 72, 161–169.

Google Scholar

Ideno, Y., Hayashi, K., Abe, Y., Ueda, K., Iso, H., Noda, M., et al. (2017). Blood pressure-lowering effect of Shinrin-yoku (Forest bathing): a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 17:409. doi: 10.1186/s12906-017-1912-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joye, Y., and van den Berg, A. (2011). Is love for green in our genes? A critical analysis of evolutionary assumptions in restorative environments research. Urban For. Urban Green. 10, 261–268.

Google Scholar

Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kingston, J. D., Marino, B. D., and Hill, A. (1994). Isotopic evidence for Neogene hominid paleoenvironments in the Kenya rift valley. Science 264, 955–959. doi: 10.1126/science.264.5161.955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, R. G. (2008). Out of Africa and the evolution of human behavior. Evol. Anthropol. 17, 267–281.

Google Scholar

Koole, S. L., and Van den Berg, A. E. (2005). Lost in the wilderness: terror management, action orientation, and nature evaluation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 1014–1028. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.1014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korpela, K. M., Klemettilä, T., and Hietanen, J. K. (2002). Evidence for rapid affective evaluation of environmental scenes. Environ. Behav. 34, 478–494. doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0064-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kretschmer, I. (2015). Demographische Untersuchungen zu Bevölkerungsdichten, Mobilität und Landnutzung im späten Jungpaläolithikum. Kölner Studien zur Prähistorischen Archäologie 6. Rahden: Verlag Marie Leidorf.

Google Scholar

Lipp, O. V., Derakshan, N., Waters, A. M., and Logies, S. (2004). Snakes and cats in the flower bed: fast detection is not specific to pictures of fear-relevant animals. Emotion 4, 233–250. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litt, T., Krastel, S., Sturm, M., Kipfer, R., Örcen, S., Heumann, G., et al. (2009). ‘PALEOVAN’, International Continental Scientific Drilling Program (ICDP): site survey results and perspectives. Quat. Sci. Rev. 28, 1555–1567.

Google Scholar

LoBue, V., and DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol. Sci. 19, 284–289.

Google Scholar

Lohr, V. I., and Pearson-Mims, C. H. (2006). Responses to scenes with spreading, rounded and conical tree forms. Environ. Behav. 38, 667–688.

Google Scholar

Maier, A. (2017). Population and settlement dynamics from the gravettian to the magdalenian. Mitteilungen Gesellschaft Urgeschichte 26, 83–101.

Google Scholar

Maier, A., Lehmkuhl, F., Ludwig, P., Melles, M., Schmidt, I., Shao, Y., et al. (2016). Demographic estimates of hunter-gatherers during the Last Glacial Maximum in Europe against the background of palaeoenvironmental data. Quat. Int. 425, 49–61.

Google Scholar

Maier, A., and Zimmermann, A. (2017). Populations headed south? The Gravettian from a palaeodemographicpoint of view. Antiquity 91, 573–588.

Google Scholar

Malhi, Y., Doughty, C. E., Galetti, M., Smith, F. A., Svenning, J. C., and Terborgh, J. W. (2016). Megafauna and ecosystem function from the Pleistocene to the Anthropocene. PNAS 113, 838–846. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502540113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangone, G., Dopko, R. L., and Zelenski, J. M. (2021). Deciphering landscape preferences: investigating the roles of familiarity and biome types. Landsc. Urban Plann. 214:104189.

Google Scholar

McMahan, E. A., and Estes, D. (2015). The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 10, 507–519. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menatti, L., and Casado da Rocha, A. (2016). Landscape and health: connecting psychology, aesthetics, and philosophy through the concept of Affordance. Front. Psychol. 7:571. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moura, J. M. B., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Silva, T. C., and Albuquerque, U. P. (2018). The influence of the evolutionary past on the mind: an analysis of the preference for landscapes in the human species. Front. Psychol. 9:2485. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, U. C., Pross, J., and Bibus, E. (2003). Vegetation response to rapid climate change in Central Europe during the past 140,000 yr based on evidence from the Füramoos pollen record. Quat. Res. 59, 235–245.

Google Scholar

Müller, U. C., Pross, J., Tzedakis, P. C., Gamble, C., Kotthoff, U., Schmiedl, G., et al. (2011). The role of climate of climate in the spread of modern humans into Europe. Quat. Sci. Rev. 30, 273–279.

Google Scholar

Müller-Beck, H. (1983). “Sammlerinnen und Jäger von den Anfängen bis vor 35000 Jahren,” in Urgeschichte Baden-Württembergs, ed. H. Müller-Beck (Germany: Stuttgart), 214–272.

Google Scholar

Mygind, L., Kjeldsted, E., Hartmeyer, R., Mygind, E., Stevenson, M. P., Quintana, D., et al. (2019). Effects of public green space on acute psychophysiological stress response: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the experimental and quasi-experimental evidence. Environ. Behav. 53, 184–226. doi: 10.1177/0013916519873376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigst, P. R., Haesaerts, P., Damblon, F., Frank-Fellner, C., Mallol, C., Viola, B., et al. (2014). Early modern human settlement of Europe north of the Alps occurred 43,500 years ago in a cold steppe-type environment. PNAS 111, 14394–14399. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412201111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norwood, M. F., Lakhani, A., Maujean, A., Zeeman, H., Olivia Creux, O., and Kendall, E. (2019). Brain activity, underlying mood and the environment: a systematic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 65:101321.

Google Scholar

Orians, G. (1980). “Habitat selection: general theory and applications to human behavior,” in The Evolution of Human Social Behavior, ed. J. S. Lockard (Chicago, IL: Elsevier), 49–66.

Google Scholar

Orians, G., and Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). “Evolved responses to landscapes,” in The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, eds J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 555–579.

Google Scholar

Öhman, A., Flykt, A., and Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. 130, 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108, 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, B. J., Tsunetsugu, Y., Kasetani, T., Kagawa, T., and Miyazaki, Y. (2010). The physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. EHPM 15, 18–26. doi: 10.1007/s12199-009-0086-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterfalvi, A., Meggyes, M., Makszin, L., Farkas, N., Miko, E., Miseta, A., et al. (2021). Forest bathing always makes sense: blood pressure-lowering and immune system-balancing effects in late spring and winter in central Europe. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2067. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18042067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prüfer, K., Racimo, F., Patterson, N., Jay, F., Sankararaman, S., Sawyer, S., et al. (2014). The complete genome sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains. Nature 505, 43–49. doi: 10.1038/nature12886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, S. O., Bigler, M., Blockley, S., Blunier, T., Buchardt, S. L., Clausen, H., et al. (2014). A stratigraphic framework for abrupt climatic changes during the Last Glacial period based on three synchronized Greenland ice-core records: refining and extending the INTIMATE event stratigraphy. Quat. Sci. Rev. 106, 14–28.

Google Scholar

Rathmann, J. (2021). Therapeutic Landscapes. Landscape and Health in an Interdisciplinary Perspective. Wiesbaden: Springer.

Google Scholar

Rathmann, J., Beck, C., Flutura, S., Seiderer, A., Aslan, I., and André, E. (2020). Towards quantifying forest recreation: exploring outdoor thermal physiology and human well-being along exemplary pathways in a central European urban forest (Augsburg, SE-Germany). Urban For. Urban Green. 49:126622.

Google Scholar

Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., and Bettinger, R. L. (2009). Cultural innovations and demographic change. Hum. Biol. 81, 211–235. doi: 10.3378/027.081.0306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richmond, B. G., Begun, D. R., and Strait, D. S. (2001). The origin of human bipedalism: the knuckle-walking hypothesis revisited. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 116, 70–105. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10019.abs

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, D., Grün, R., Joannes-Boyau, R., Steele, T. E., Amani, F., Rué, M., et al. (2017). The age of the hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, and the origins of the Middle Stone Age. Nature 546, 293–296. doi: 10.1038/nature22335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez Goñi, F. (2020). Regional impacts of climate change and its relevance to human evolution. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2:e55.

Google Scholar

Sauer, C. (1925). The Morphology of Landscape. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, I., and Zimmermann, A. (2019). Population dynamics and socio-spatial organization of the Aurignacian: scalable quantitative demographic data for western and central Europe. PLoS One 14:e0211562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Camb. Archaeol. J. 11, 5–16.

Google Scholar

Silva, R. H., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Moura, J. M. B., and Albuquerque, U. P. (2020). The link between adaptive memory and cultural attraction: new insights for evolutionary ethnobiology. Evol. Biol. 47, 273–284.

Google Scholar

Sommer, R. (1997). Further cross-national studies of tree form preference. Ecol. Psychol. 2, 153–160. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0902_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staats, H., Kievet, A., and Hartig, T. (2003). Where to recover from attentional fatigue: an expectancy-value analysis of environmental preference. J. Environ.Psychol. 23, 147–157. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00112-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamps, I. I. I. A. E. (1999). Demographic effects in environmental aesthetics: a meta-analysis. J. Plann. Lit. 14, 155–175.

Google Scholar

Staubwasser, M., Drǎgusin, V., Onac, B. P., Assonov, S., Ersek, V., Hoffmann, D. L., et al. (2018). Impact of climate change on the transition of Neanderthals to modern humans in Europe. PNAS 115, 9116–9121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1808647115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stojakowits, P., Mayr, C., Ivy-Ochs, S., Preusser, F., Reitner, J., and Spötl, C. (2021). Environments at the MIS 3/2 transition in the northern Alps and their foreland. Quat. Int. 58, 99–113.

Google Scholar

Svensson, A., Andersen, K. K., Bigler, M., Clausen, H. B., Dahl-Jensen, D., Davies, S. M., et al. (2008). A 60 000 year Greenland stratigraphic ice core chronology. Clim. Past. 4, 47–57.

Google Scholar

Tam, K.-P., and Milfont, T. L. (2020). Towards cross-cultural environmental psychology: a state-of-the-art review and recommendations. J. Environ. Psychol. 71:101474.

Google Scholar

Twohig-Bennett, C., and Jones, A. (2018). Health benefits of the great outdoors: a systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ. Res. 166, 628–637. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S. (1983). “Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment,” in Human behavior and Environment: Advances in Theory and Research, Vol. 6, eds I. Altman and J. F. Wohlwill (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 85–125. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.676032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 224, 420–421. doi: 10.1126/science.6143402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S. (1993). “Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes,” in The Biophilia Hypothesis, eds S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (Washington, DC: Washington Island Press), 73–137. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230.

Google Scholar

Van den Berg, A. E., Koole, S. L., and Van der Wulp, N. Y. (2003). Environmental preference and restoration: (How) are they related. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 135–146.

Google Scholar

Van den Berg, A. E., Vlek, C. A. J., and Coeterier, J. F. (1998). Group differences in the aesthetic evaluation of nature development plans: a multi-level approach. J. Environ. Psychol. 18, 141–157.

Google Scholar

van Ginneken, V. (2019). Multiple ‘genetic bottleneck theory’ of humans and the house mouse via chilling enzyme Δ12-desaturase. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. Int. J. 4:000156

Google Scholar

van Ginneken, V., Van Meerveld, A., Wijgerde, T., Verheij, E., de Vries, E., and Van der Greef, J. (2017). Hunter-prey correlation between migration routes of African buffaloes and early hominids: evidence for the “Out of Africa” hypothesis. Integr. Mol. Med. 4, 1–5.

Google Scholar

van Valkenburgh, B., Hayward, M. W., Ripple, W. J., Meloro, C., and Roth, V. L. (2016). The impact of large terrestrial carnivores on Pleistocene ecosystems. PNAS 113, 862–867. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502554112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward Thompson, C. (2018). “Landscape perception and environmental psychology,” in The Routledge Companion to Landscape Studies, eds P. Howard, I. Thompson, E. Waterton, and M. Atha (London: Routledge), 19–38.

Google Scholar

Wick, L., Lemcke, G., and Sturm, M. (2003). Evidence of Lateglacial and Holocene climatic change and human impact in eastern Anatolia: high-resolution pollen, charcoal, isotopic and geochemical records from the laminated sediments of Lake Van, Turkey. Holocene 13, 665–675.

Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wylie, J. (2007). Landscape. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Keywords: savanna hypothesis, landscape preferences, human health-environment relationships, paleoanthropology, therapeutic landscapes

Citation: Rathmann J, Korpela KM and Stojakowits P (2022) Pleistocene Hypothesis – Moving Savanna Perceptual Preference Hypothesis Beyond Savanna. Front. Psychol. 13:901799. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.901799

Received: 22 March 2022; Accepted: 09 May 2022;
Published: 30 May 2022.

Edited by:

Susana Alves, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Judith Heerwagen, General Services Administration (GSA), United States
Giuseppe Barbiero, Università Della Valle d’Aosta, Italy
Laura Menatti, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Copyright © 2022 Rathmann, Korpela and Stojakowits. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joachim Rathmann, am9hY2hpbS5yYXRobWFubkBnZW8udW5pLWF1Z3NidXJnLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.