Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 11 May 2022
Sec. Psychology of Aging
This article is part of the Research Topic Methods and Applications in Psychology of Aging View all 7 articles

They Cannot, They Will Not, or We Are Asking the Wrong Questions: Re-examining Age-Related Decline in Social Cognition

  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, United States

Social cognition is critical for successfully navigating social relationships. Current evidence suggests that older adults exhibit poorer performance in several core social-cognitive domains compared to younger adults. Neurocognitive decline is commonly discussed as one of the key arbiters of age-related decline in social-cognitive abilities. While evidence supports this notion, age effects are likely attributable to multiple factors. This paper aims to recontextualize past evidence by focusing issues of motivation, task design, and representative samples. In light of these issues, we identify directions for future research to aide our understanding of social-cognitive aging.

Introduction

Social connectedness has been widely implicated in preserving older adults’ cognitive, physical, and mental wellbeing (Shankar et al., 2011; Boss et al., 2015; Kuiper et al., 2015). Social-cognitive function—the process by which people understand, store, and apply information about others (Fiske and Taylor, 1991)—is essential for maintaining social connectedness (see Krendl and Heatherton, 2009), and relates to social relationships in later life (Krendl et al., 2022). Several key social-cognitive domains are disrupted by aging including emotion recognition—decoding another person’s feelings through non-verbal cues (Ruffman et al., 2008; Gonçalves et al., 2018; Hayes et al., 2020), impression formation—forming and managing impressions of others (Cassidy et al., 2016, 2020; Krendl and Kensinger, 2016; Krendl, 2018), and theory of mind—inferring the mental states of others (Henry et al., 2013; Moran, 2013). This paper outlines the support for several mechanisms that may contribute to age-related social-cognitive decline. However, it is not meant as a systematic or meta-analytic article as others have published such work (e.g., Demichelis et al., 2020; Hayes et al., 2020).

Although the preponderance of research on age-related social-cognitive decline has focused on declines in general cognitive ability, we argue that mechanisms are likely more nuanced. Over-emphasizing a single mechanism may limit potential interventions targets by overlooking key factors that contribute to deficits. To be forthcoming on our perspective, we provide Figure 1 as a consolidated visualization of the key arguments (formulated as questions), their ties to each proposed mechanism reviewed here, and their contributions to the design’s internal and external validity. Importantly, mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and may work in tandem (i.e., two mechanisms may answer the same question), but each provides its own unique strengths as an explanatory mechanism underlying social-cognitive processes across adulthood. Additionally, replicability concerns are not new and well-known across science (Ioannidis, 2005; Open Science Collaboration, 2012), and research must address generalizability by explicitly improving sample representativeness. We offer this paper as a re-examination of past research while focusing on motivation, study design, and sample representativeness and considering the threat to internal and/or external validity that each factor may pose.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Questions for evaluating future research. Arrows represent if a mechanism relates to each question.

“They Cannot”: Links Between Cognitive Decline and Social-Cognitive Ability

Many studies have focused on the overlap between general cognitive and social-cognitive decline (see Moran, 2013; Gonçalves et al., 2018; Demichelis et al., 2020; Schlegel et al., 2020). Normative declines in fluid abilities (i.e., speed of processing, working memory, and executive function: see Salthouse, 2019) co-occur with declines in social-cognitive function (Sandoz et al., 2014; Moreau et al., 2016). For instance, inhibitory failures that arise from poorer executive functioning beget bias-prone responses during impression formation (e.g., Cassidy et al., 2016, 2020; Krendl and Kensinger, 2016; Krendl, 2018; Von Hippel, 2007). However, it should be noted that most social-cognitive aging studies are cross-sectional in nature, limiting any assessment of causality in the absence of longitudinal data.

Beyond evidence of concomitant age differences, there are many parallels between cognitive and social-cognitive aging research. Given that most social-cognitive tasks have time constraints or require coordinating multiple pieces of information (as is the case with cognitive aging tasks), it is unsurprising that performance is confounded between the two. Moreover, performance across social-cognitive domains (i.e., Theory of Mind and emotion recognition) appear to be scaffolded in a manner consistent with general cognitive models (see meta-analysis by Schurz et al., 2021). For instance, during mentalization for Theory of Mind, older adults may make more errors than younger adults due to poor mobilization of these underlying cognitive resources (e.g., Lecce et al., 2019). Thus, it could be that older adults perform worse on social-cognitive tasks due to decreased cognitive ability.

An important caveat to the above point is that social-cognitive performance can be maintained even in the presence of cognitive decline (see Krendl et al., 2014 or Strickland-Hughes et al., 2020 as examples). In fact, general cognition and social cognition engage distinct, albeit overlapping, patterns of neural activation (e.g., MacPherson et al., 2002; Schurz et al., 2014) even in neuropathological decline (e.g., Poletti et al., 2012; Belfort et al., 2020 for a review). Thus, at least some of the brain networks that underscore social cognition may be relatively resilient to age-related decline. Consistent with this assertion, neurological development in early childhood does not linearly predict the emergence of social-cognitive abilities (e.g., Tousignant et al., 2017; Meinhardt-Injac et al., 2020). With this as a reference point, it is perhaps unsurprising to suggest that normative neurological decline may not be associated with universal declines in all aspects of social cognition.

Moreover, there is evidence that suggests lab-based measures misrepresent the magnitude of age effects in social cognition. For instance, older adults are better at recognizing emotions during conversations with marital partners despite relatively poor performance on lab-based tasks (Sze et al., 2012). Rather than adopting a “they cannot” mindset that conflates social cognition with general cognition, we must identify how older adults’ performance may be driven by other factors that could occlude accurate estimation of their social-cognitive abilities.

“They Will Not”: A Motivation-Based Account of Selective Engagement

Motivation in social-cognitive tasks may be shaped by myriad factors, including cognitive decline (e.g., Hess, 2006, 2014) or lifetime experiences (e.g., Carstensen et al., 1999). With respect to the former, social cognition is resource intensive, meaning it requires neurological and psychological resources (e.g., Fiske and Taylor, 1984). When resources are limited (e.g., due to cognitive decline), individuals may be less able or willing to expend them (e.g., Hess, 2006, 2014), which promotes selectivity in determining when and how to engage those resources. With respect to the latter, older adults may utilize experience-based social knowledge when deciding when and how to engage (e.g., Hess and Auman, 2001; Blanchard-Fields, 2007). Socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen et al., 1999) suggests that older adults are chronically motivated to prioritize familiar and close social partners. Older adults may expend more effort or utilize better strategies in such contexts, perhaps be getting better performance. Ultimately, varying age effects in past research may be due to older adults’ motivation rather than cognitive ability (as in cognitive aging: see Swirsky and Spaniol, 2019).

Evidence shows how explicitly motivating performance goals increases older adults’ performance. Indeed, age differences in emotion recognition and person perception disappear entirely when people are told that they will need to justify their judgments after completing the task (Hess et al., 2001, 2009a,b; Stanley and Isaacowitz, 2015), despite the fact that self-rated motivation does not influence emotion recognition (Ceccato et al., 2019). Thus, evidence of age-related decline in social cognition may be an artifact of older adults’ implicit resource preservation goals which leads to worse performance, especially when there are no consequences for being wrong. Yet, little work has tested if older adults will selectively modulate their level of effort based on explicit or implicit goals.

Certain factors (i.e., familiarity, wanting people to like you, and personal closeness) can increase older adults’ motivation. Own-age biases emerge for facial recognition and face–name associative memory tasks (Rhodes and Anastasi, 2012; Strickland-Hughes et al., 2020). Older adults better recognize emotions in their marital partners when compared to strangers (Sze et al., 2012). Theory of mind is greater among older adults who desire to be liked by others (Lecce et al., 2017) and when social closeness is experimental increased (Zhang et al., 2013, 2018). Conversely, older adults express more bias when perceiving members of out-groups, such as racial minorities, people who are homeless, and religious groups (Von Hippel et al., 2000; Cassidy et al., 2016, 2020; Krendl and Kensinger, 2016; Krendl, 2018). Altogether, these studies suggest that older adults can perform well but may choose not to.

We must identify factors that motivate older adults and if these factors are equally motivating for younger adults. If older adults are less motivated by traditional laboratory paradigms than younger adults, this calls into question the internal validity of many studies to date as well as the degree of generalizability and external validity. One way future research can test this motivational account is by examining the strategies that older adults use and their relative effectiveness. If older adults use more effortful strategies when motivated to perform well, then they may use less effortful strategies when unmotivated and perform worse as a result (see Hess et al., 2013). However, it will be important to interrogate whether effectively utilizing effortful strategies is predicated on cognitive abilities. For instance, theory of mind performance increases with strategy training (Cavallini et al., 2015, 2021; Lecce et al., 2015, 2019), but only when older adults have sufficient cognitive resources (e.g., executive function: see Lecce et al., 2019). Thus, future research will need to interrogate the limits of using motivation to improve to performance on social-cognitive tasks and explicitly evaluate factors that motivate older adults to perform well.

In evaluating what is (and is not) motivating for older adults, the field needs to re-evaluate our methodologies. However, this redirects our focus to the stimuli and tasks commonly deployed and exposes possible pitfalls. For instance, evidence using a face sort task (instead of prototypical forced-choice paradigms) shows that older and younger adults’ emotion recognition performance (i.e., assigning emotion identifiers to faces) is mostly comparable, but older adults are more nuanced in the emotion terms they use (Hoemann et al., 2021). In fact, consistent concerns have been raised regarding the stimuli and tasks that are used to assess social-cognitive abilities (e.g., Isaacowitz and Stanley, 2011; Freund and Isaacowitz, 2013; Kunzmann and Isaacowitz, 2017). Beyond motivation to perform, we must address the tasks themselves.

Asking the Wrong Questions? the Limits of Current Tasks

Ecological validity in social cognition generally refers to the degree of the “realness” that the stimuli and tasks have. This usually hinges on two key dimensions: artificiality versus naturality and simplicity versus complexity (see Holleman et al., 2020 for discussion). However, ecological validity is often conflated with the issue of representative design—how effects translate from laboratory tasks to daily life contexts. Put simply, ecological validity refers to stimuli whereas representative design speaks to the context of the stimuli. Consequently, ecological validity is directly related to internal validity (i.e., testing the construct with appropriate items), whereas representative design addresses both internal and external validity as it represents the degree to which laboratory tasks mimic the phenomenon as it occurs in daily contexts. Although the former is more routinely discussed, the latter is a pressing issue that must be addressed for the field to move forward.

One of the main ecological validity concerns that has received the most attention is static versus dynamic stimuli. Differential age effects emerge depending on which is used (e.g., Hayes et al., 2020). For example, age-related differences in emotion recognition are less robust when using videos rather than pictures (e.g., Krendl and Ambady, 2010). However, the effects can be complex. That is, although dynamic stimuli (i.e., film clips) seem to increase performance (presumably due to their increased naturalness), they may simultaneously interfere with performance because they often increase the number of cues to be integrated (thereby increasing difficulty). This discordance is reflected in theory of mind research: age differences do not disappear when using dynamic stimuli (Henry et al., 2013), and, in some cases, may increase in magnitude (see Grainger et al., 2019, 2021). This illustrates the difficulty in defining the complexity and naturality of a task.

Instead, ecological validity should be evaluated in terms of how the stimuli (and how they are used in a task) plausibly represent the construct under investigation (see Holleman et al., 2020). Dynamic stimuli may be better at simulating a given phenomenon but offer less experimental control. To balance both, laboratory studies have leveraged virtual reality paradigms, which allow researchers to create a virtual social world while limiting what the participant can see and do (see Parsons, 2015). These paradigms appear to improve performance in younger adults with social-cognitive impairments (e.g., Kandalaft et al., 2013), and early evidence indicates that virtual reality interventions may promote assorted health-related benefits for older adults (see Dermody et al., 2020). Despite improving the accuracy of social-cognitive assessments (i.e., internal validity), concerns remain on how well findings replicate in other contexts (i.e., external validity). Poor representative design may provide incorrect conclusions about older adults’ social-cognitive ability in daily experiences.

Unfortunately, representative design has been underdiscussed in comparison to ecological validity despite evidence that lab-based tasks may underestimate social-cognitive ability (as described previously with regard to motivation as in Lecce et al., 2019 or Sze et al., 2012). It could be that daily social interactions in older adulthood are far more motivating and less demanding than any lab-based task. Consequently, the field must develop paradigms that evaluate social-cognitive processes in daily life. Ecological momentary assessments may help capture how people behave in more natural environments and have been used to capture the types of social interactions people have in daily life (e.g., Zhaoyang et al., 2018). However, such studies must be accessible to all and not overly complex to avoid inadvertently eliciting age effects (Cain et al., 2009). They also do not inherently solve the problem of evaluating social cognition during real interactions. Addressing representative design will require developing sensitive measures of daily social interactions, whether in the lab or daily life.

Ultimately, improving our measures is important as it will directly improve levels of internal and external validity in any given study. However, there is another major methodological concern that must be called out and specifically addressed in future research. The lack of representative samples is a looming blind spot in aging research at-large, and there may be important variability that is missed by overlooking sociodemographic factors and their relationship to social cognition.

A Blind Spot in the Literature: Sample Representativeness

Sample representativeness in social-cognitive aging research must be addressed as differences are known to manifest when considering sociodemographic diversity (see Gutchess and Boduroglu, 2015). Evidence from an online study of over 40,000 people aged 10 to 70 revealed that theory of mind performance may be more sensitive to race, ethnicity, and education than it is to cognition (Dodell-Feder et al., 2020) as White, college-educated individuals perform the best. Thus, unaccounted variance across groups directly limits the external validity of past studies since a preponderance utilize White, middle-class, and college-educated samples.

Currently, 25% of people aged 65 or older in the United States identify as a member of a racial minority, which is expected to increase to 34% in the next two decades (Administration for Community Living, 2021). Extending the postulates of minority stress models (Forrester et al., 2019), older members of racial minority groups may have a lifetime of utilizing social resources to cope with multifaceted disadvantages (e.g., structural and interpersonal discrimination). Beginning as early as grade school (see Rowley et al., 2008), members of racial minorities may learn to be hypervigilant to out-group threats and mistrust unfamiliar social partners for self-preservation in the face of discrimination (see Brondolo et al., 2018). Members of racial minorities have a lifetime of maintaining these behaviors which may manifest patterns of social-cognitive aging that differ from members of racial majorities. Thus, one possibility is that older members of racial minorities may display age-related expertise in certain social situations. No direct evidence exists to evaluate the likelihood of this possibility, however, which only reiterates the need for future research.

Gender is another sociodemographic factor to consider. Some work has shown that women perform better than men on social-cognitive tasks, such as theory of mind (e.g., Wacker et al., 2017). These gender differences may be underscored by differences in strategy use (e.g., Adenzato et al., 2017, 2019). However, these differences may reflect how social cognition itself (i.e., understanding others’ thought, feelings, and behaviors) is a stereotypically feminine notion (see Martin and Slepian, 2021 for a discussion). Female gender roles of warmth and communality are reinforced from birth onward, presumably leading to social-cognitive differences that percolate and accumulate from adolescence into later life.

Future work should maintain an intersectional approach (see Collins and Bilge, 2020) to understand how membership in multiple groups may influence social cognition. As an example, Black women in the workforce often contend with several negative stereotypes due to their race and gender (see Collins, 2004). To face pervasive and systemic stereotypes throughout their lives, Black women may utilize an abundance of mindreading (i.e., Theory of Mind) to navigate their hypervisibility (Dickens et al., 2019) and avoid activating negative stereotypes associated with their identities (see Collins, 2004). Even with hypervigilant meta-cognitive awareness, they may still experience discrimination and microaggressions and utilize social-cognitive strengths (e.g., strong support networks) to cope (Holder et al., 2015). Thus, the lifelong utility of social-cognitive abilities may be best understood through an intersectional lens.

Social-cognitive aging research will benefit from exploring the role of sociodemographic factors. Special efforts need to be made to gather representative samples of older adults to interrogate the generalizability of social-cognitive aging effects and determine whether differential effects emerge within underrepresented populations (i.e., non-White, less educated, and low socioeconomic status). Thus, future research should examine sociodemographic factors, such as race and gender, to understand within-group variability for older populations. Ultimately, evidence garnered from this line of inquiry will be crucial for the advancement of the field.

Concluding Remarks

Despite recent progress, many important questions remain. General cognitive decline appears to be concomitant with social-cognitive difficulties, but not all evidence supports this narrative. Other mechanisms, such as motivated selectivity, ecological validity, representative design, and sociodemographic factors should be further investigated. We believe that social-cognitive aging research has many paths forward including the development of new tasks and evaluating the day-to-day impact of social-cognitive function across adulthood. Although the current challenges require innovative solutions, the rich history of social-cognitive aging research suggests that researchers will meet these demands and continue to push the field into the future.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, and further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

LH lead the project by conducting literature searches and writing the manuscript. AG also collected relevant literature and wrote portions of initial drafts. AK provided conceptual guidance and critical feedback throughout the project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by National Institute on Aging Grant R01 AG070931 (PI: AK).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Amani Holder-Dixon for providing feedback on early versions of this manuscript.

References

Adenzato, M., Brambilla, M., Manenti, R., De Lucia, L., Trojano, L., Garofalo, S., et al. (2017). Gender differences in cognitive theory of mind revealed by transcranial direct current stimulation on medial prefrontal cortex. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/srep41219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adenzato, M., Manenti, R., Gobbi, E., Enrici, I., Rusich, D., and Cotelli, M. (2019). Aging, sex and cognitive theory of mind: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54469-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Administration for Community Living (2021). 2020 Profile of Older Americans. Available at: https://acl.gov/sites/default/files/Profile%20of%20OA/2020ProfileOlderAmericans_RevisedFinal.pdf (Accessed March 11, 2022).

Google Scholar

Belfort, T., Simões, J. P., Santos, R. L., Lacerda, I., and Dourado, M. C. N. (2020). Social cognition: patterns of impairments in mild and moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 35, 1385–1392. doi: 10.1002/gps.5379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 26–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boss, L., Kang, D. H., and Branson, S. (2015). Loneliness and cognitive function in the older adult: a systematic review. Int. Psychogeriatr. 27, 541–553. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brondolo, E., Blair, I. V., and Kaur, A. (2018). “Biopsychosocial mechanisms linking discrimination to health: A focus on social cognition,” in The Oxford Handbook of Stigma, Discrimination, and Health. eds. B. Major, J. F. Dovidio, and B. G. Link (England: Oxford University Press), 219–240.

Google Scholar

Cain, A. E., Depp, C. A., and Jeste, D. V. (2009). Ecological momentary assessment in aging research: a critical review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 987–996. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol. 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, B. S., Hughes, C., Lanie, S. T., and Krendl, A. C. (2020). Effects of executive ability on bias and ingroup perceptions in aging. Psychol. Aging 35, 283–294. doi: 10.1037/pag0000420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, B. S., Lee, E. J., and Krendl, A. C. (2016). Age and executive ability impact the neural correlates of race perception. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1752–1761. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavallini, E., Bianco, F., Bottiroli, S., Rosi, A., Vecchi, T., and Lecce, S. (2015). Training for generalization in theory of mind: a study with older adults. Front. Psychol. 6:1123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavallini, E., Ceccato, I., Bertoglio, S., Francescani, A., Vigato, F., Ianes, A. B., et al. (2021). Can theory of mind of healthy older adults living in a nursing home be improved? A randomized controlled trial. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 3029–3037. doi: 10.1007/s40520-021-01811-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceccato, I., Lecce, S., Cavallini, E., Vugt, F. T., and Ruffman, T. (2019). Motivation and social-cognitive abilities in older adults: convergent evidence from self-report measures and cardiovascular reactivity. PLoS One 14:e0218785. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, P. H. (2004). Black Sexual Politics: African Americans, Gender, and the New Racism. England: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Collins, P. H., and Bilge, S. (2020). Intersectionality. United States: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Demichelis, O., Coundouris, S., Grainger, S., and Henry, J. (2020). Empathy and theory of mind in Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 26, 963–977. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720000478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dermody, G., Whitehead, L., Wilson, G., and Glass, C. (2020). The role of virtual reality in improving health outcomes for community-dwelling older adults: systematic review. J. Med. Internet Res. 22:e17331. doi: 10.2196/17331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickens, D. D., Womack, V. Y., and Dimes, T. (2019). Managing hypervisibility: an exploration of theory and research on identity shifting strategies in the workplace among black women. J. Vocat. Behav. 113, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2018.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodell-Feder, D., Ressler, K. J., and Germine, L. T. (2020). Social cognition or social class and culture? On the interpretation of differences in social cognitive performance. Psychol. Med. 50, 133–145. doi: 10.1017/S003329171800404X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., and Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social Cognition. United States: Addison-Wesley.

Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., and Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Cognition. New York: Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.

Google Scholar

Forrester, S. N., Gallo, J. J., Whitfield, K. E., and Thorpe, R. J. Jr. (2019). A framework of minority stress: From physiological manifestations to cognitive outcomes. The Gerontologist 59, 1017–1023. doi: 10.1093/geront/gny104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, A. M., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2013). Beyond age comparisons: a plea for the use of a modified Brunswikian approach to experimental designs in the study of adult development and aging. Hum. Dev. 56, 351–371. doi: 10.1159/000357177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonçalves, A. R., Fernandes, C., Pasion, R., Ferreira-Santos, F., Barbosa, F., and Marques-Teixeira, J. (2018). Effects of age on the identification of emotions in facial expressions: a meta-analysis. PeerJ 6:e5278. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, S. A., Rakunathan, V., Adams, A. G., Canty, A. L., and Henry, J. D. (2021). An assessment of age differences in theory of mind using the virtual assessment of mentalizing ability. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 28, 97–107. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2020.1713290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, S. A., Steinvik, H. R., Henry, J. D., and Phillips, L. H. (2019). The role of social attention in older adults’ ability to interpret naturalistic social scenes. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 72, 1328–1343. doi: 10.1177/1747021818791774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A. H., and Boduroglu, A. (2015). “Cognition in adulthood across cultures,” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Development and Culture: An Interdisciplinary Perspective. ed. L. A. Jensen (England: Oxford University Press), 621–636.

Google Scholar

Hayes, G. S., McLennan, S. N., Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Terrett, G., Rendell, P. G., et al. (2020). Task characteristics influence facial emotion recognition age-effects: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Aging 35, 295–315. doi: 10.1037/pag0000441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., and Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychol. Aging 28, 826–839. doi: 10.1037/a0030677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M. (2006). Adaptive aspects of social cognitive functioning in adulthood: age–related goal and knowledge influences. Soc. Cogn. 24, 279–309. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.3.279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M. (2014). Selective engagement of cognitive resources: motivational influences on older adults’ cognitive functioning. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 388–407. doi: 10.1177/1745691614527465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., and Auman, C. (2001). Aging and social expertise: The impact of trait-diagnostic information on impressions of others. Psychol. Aging 16, 497–510. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Germain, C. M., Swaim, E. L., and Osowski, N. L. (2009a). Aging and selective engagement: The moderating impact of motivation on older adults’ resource utilization. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64B, 447–456. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Leclerc, C. M., Swaim, E., and Weatherbee, S. R. (2009b). Aging and everyday judgments: the impact of motivational and processing resource factors. Psychol. Aging 24, 735–740. doi: 10.1037/a0016340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Queen, T. L., and Ennis, G. E. (2013). Age and self-relevance effects on information search during decision making. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 68, 703–711. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Rosenberg, D. C., and Waters, S. J. (2001). Motivation and representational processes in adulthood: the effects of social accountability and information relevance. Psychol. Aging 16, 629–642. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoemann, K., Vicaria, I. M., Gendron, M., and Stanley, J. T. (2021). Introducing a face sort paradigm to evaluate age differences in emotion perception. J. Gerontol. B 76, 1272–1281. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holder, A. M., Jackson, M., and Ponterotto, J. (2015). Racial microaggression experiences and coping strategies of black women in corporate leadership. Qual. Psychol. 2, 164–180. doi: 10.1037/qup0000024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holleman, G. A., Hooge, I. T., Kemner, C., and Hessels, R. S. (2020). The ‘real-world approach’ and its problems: A critique of the term ecological validity. Front. Psychol. 11:721. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2:e124. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., and Stanley, J. T. (2011). Bringing an ecological perspective to the study of aging and recognition of emotional facial expressions: past, current, and future methods. J. Nonverbal Behav. 35, 261–278. doi: 10.1007/s10919-011-0113-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandalaft, M. R., Didehbani, N., Krawczyk, D. C., Allen, T. T., and Chapman, S. B. (2013). Virtual reality social cognition training for young adults with high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 34–44. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1544-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C. (2018). Reduced cognitive capacity impairs the malleability of older adults’ negative attitudes to stigmatized individuals. Exp. Aging Res. 44, 271–283. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2018.1475152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C., and Ambady, N. (2010). Older adults’ decoding of emotions: role of dynamic versus static cues and age-related cognitive decline. Psychol. Aging 25, 788–793. doi: 10.1037/a0020607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C., and Heatherton, T. F. (2009). “Self versus others/self-regulation,” in Handbook of Neuroscience for the Behavioral Sciences. eds. G. G. Bernston and J. T. Cacioppo (New Jersy: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 859–878.

Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C., Kennedy, D. P., Hugenberg, K., and Perry, B. L. (2022). Social cognitive abilities predict unique aspects of older adults’ personal social networks. J. Gerontol. B 77, 18–28. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C., and Kensinger, E. A. (2016). Does older adults’ cognitive function disrupt the malleability of their attitudes toward outgroup members?: An fMRI investigation. PLoS One 11:e0152698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krendl, A. C., Rule, N. O., and Ambady, N. (2014). Does aging impair first impression accuracy? Differentiating emotion recognition from complex social inferences. Psychol. Aging 29, 482–490. doi: 10.1037/a0037146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuiper, J. S., Zuidersma, M., Voshaar, R. C. O., Zuidema, S. U., van den Heuvel, E. R., Stolk, R. P., et al. (2015). Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 22, 39–57. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunzmann, U., and Isaacowitz, D. (2017). Emotional aging: taking the immediate context seriously. Res. Hum. Dev. 14, 182–199. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2017.1340048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecce, S., Bottiroli, S., Bianco, F., Rosi, A., and Cavallini, E. (2015). Training older adults on theory of mind (ToM): transfer on metamemory. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 60, 217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., Bianco, F., Rosi, A., Bottiroli, S., and Cavallini, E. (2017). Theory of mind and social relationships in older adults: the role of social motivation. Aging Ment. Health 21, 253–258. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1114586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., and Cavallini, E. (2019). Investigating ToM in aging with the MASC: from accuracy to error type. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 26, 541–557. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2018.1500996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., and Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 17, 598–609. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A. E., and Slepian, M. L. (2021). The primacy of gender: gendered cognition underlies the big two dimensions of social cognition. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 1143–1158. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinhardt-Injac, B., Daum, M. M., and Meinhardt, G. (2020). Theory of mind development from adolescence to adulthood: testing the two-component model. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 38, 289–303. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, J. M. (2013). Lifespan development: The effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behav. Brain Res. 237, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, N., Rauzy, S., Viallet, F., and Champagne-Lavau, M. (2016). Theory of mind in Alzheimer disease: evidence of authentic impairment during social interaction. Neuropsychology 30, 312–321. doi: 10.1037/neu0000220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Open Science Collaboration (2012). An open, large-scale, collaborative effort to estimate the reproducibility of psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 657–660. doi: 10.1177/1745691612462588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, T. D. (2015). Virtual reality for enhanced ecological validity and experimental control in the clinical, affective and social neurosciences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:660. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poletti, M., Enrici, I., and Adenzato, M. (2012). Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2147–2164. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, M. G., and Anastasi, J. S. (2012). The own-age bias in face recognition: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 138, 146–174. doi: 10.1037/a0025750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowley, S. J., Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., and Zeisel, S. A. (2008). Racial identity, social context, and race-related social cognition in African Americans during middle childhood. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1537–1546. doi: 10.1037/a0013349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., and Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 863–881. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2019). Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 34, 17–24. doi: 10.1037/pag0000288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandoz, M., Démonet, J. F., and Fossard, M. (2014). Theory of mind and cognitive processes in aging and Alzheimer type dementia: a systematic review. Aging Ment. Health 18, 815–827. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2014.899974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegel, K., Palese, T., Mast, M. S., Rammsayer, T. H., Hall, J. A., and Murphy, N. A. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between emotion recognition ability and intelligence. Cognit. Emot. 34, 329–351. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1632801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., and Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Tholen, M. G., Maliske, L., Margulies, D. S., Mars, R. B., et al. (2021). Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychol. Bull. 147, 293–327. doi: 10.1037/bul0000303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shankar, A., McMunn, A., Banks, J., and Steptoe, A. (2011). Loneliness, social isolation, and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychol. 30, 377–385. doi: 10.1037/a0022826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanley, J. T., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2015). Caring more and knowing more reduces age-related differences in emotion perception. Psychol. Aging 30, 383–395. doi: 10.1037/pag0000028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strickland-Hughes, C. M., Dillon, K. E., West, R. L., and Ebner, N. C. (2020). Own-age bias in face-name associations: evidence from memory and visual attention in younger and older adults. Cognition 200:104253. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swirsky, L. T., and Spaniol, J. (2019). Cognitive and motivational selectivity in healthy aging. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 10:e1512. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sze, J. A., Goodkind, M. S., Gyurak, A., and Levenson, R. W. (2012). Aging and emotion recognition: not just a losing matter. Psychol. Aging 27, 940–950. doi: 10.1037/a0029367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tousignant, B., Eugène, F., and Jackson, P. L. (2017). A developmental perspective on the neural bases of human empathy. Infant Behav. Dev. 48, 5–12. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Hippel, W. (2007). Aging, executive functioning, and social control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 240–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00512.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Hippel, W., Silver, L. A., and Lynch, M. E. (2000). Stereotyping against your will: the role of inhibitory ability in stereotyping and prejudice among the elderly. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 523–532. doi: 10.1177/0146167200267001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wacker, R., Bölte, S., and Dziobek, I. (2017). Women know better what other women think and feel: gender effects on mindreading across the adult life span. Front. Psychol. 8:1324. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Fung, H. H., Stanley, J. T., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Ho, M. Y. (2013). Perspective taking in older age revisited: a motivational perspective. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1848–1858. doi: 10.1037/a0031211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., Cavallini, E., Zhang, L., and Chen, T. (2018). Plasticity in older adults’ theory of mind performance: the impact of motivation. Aging Ment. Health 22, 1592–1599. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1376313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhaoyang, R., Sliwinski, M. J., Martire, L. M., and Smyth, J. M. (2018). Age differences in adults’ daily social interactions: an ecological momentary assessment study. Psychol. Aging 33, 607–618. doi: 10.1037/pag0000242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social cognition and aging, motivation, research design, ecological validity, representative samples

Citation: Hamilton LJ, Gourley AN and Krendl AC (2022) They Cannot, They Will Not, or We Are Asking the Wrong Questions: Re-examining Age-Related Decline in Social Cognition. Front. Psychol. 13:894522. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.894522

Received: 11 March 2022; Accepted: 20 April 2022;
Published: 11 May 2022.

Edited by:

Magdalena Leszko, University of Szczecin, Poland

Reviewed by:

Elena Cavallini, University of Pavia, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Hamilton, Gourley and Krendl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucas J. Hamilton, ljhamilt@iu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.