Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 06 July 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Fluency and Reading Comprehension in Typical Readers and Dyslexic Readers, volume II View all 17 articles

The Interface Between Reading and Handwriting

  • Speech-Language Pathology Program, Midwestern University, Downers Grove, IL, United States

Introduction

Zelaznik and Goffman (2010) state: “Language production, whether spoken, signed, or written, is a motor activity” (p. 383). There are specific connections between the linguistic process of reading and the motoric process of handwriting, including how they develop, where and how they are instantiated within the brain, and what can occur when their underlying neurological processes are compromised.

This connection is reflected in even the earliest research regarding disorders of reading and writing. Broadbent (1872), Berlin (1887), and Kussmaul (1887) first reported that acquired brain damage could cause patients to lose their speech, reading, and writing skills even when their non-verbal cognition was preserved. English physician Morgan (1896) and Scottish ophthalmologist Hinshelwood (1900) believed that medical problems caused difficulties in reading and writing. Researchers Myklebust and Johnson (1962) described symptoms which they observed in many of 200 children with dyslexia, including subclinical disturbances in motor coordination.

The Development of Handwriting

Children learn to recognize letters before becoming able to write them (Reutzel et al., 2019). A child first begins to scribble with no discernible pattern (Coates and Coates, 2016). Between the ages of 2 and 3 years, he/she learns to imitate shapes (vertical strokes, horizontal strokes, and circles). The child may be ready to begin writing when he/she is able to cross the body's midline to imitate an oblique cross (Feder and Majnemer, 2007). Even at this young age, children use different arm and hand movement patterns when drawing as opposed to writing. McCutchen (2006) concludes that, to a certain extent, even these young children can distinguish between drawing and writing. By age four, children write marks arranged in a line with regular spacing (Scott, 2012). Researchers have learned about children's handwriting development from measures as diverse as examining their visual-motor integration abilities, kinematics of their pen movements, and eye movements as they write (Fears and Lockman, 2020). Researchers have also explored dynamic measures including the velocity, acceleration, pressure, and tilt of writers' pens (Gargot et al., 2020) and their pause behaviors (Sumner et al., 2013; Alamargot et al., 2020).

Throughout the early school years, learning handwriting contributes to the child's overall writing development (Graham et al., 2008). Learning stroke sequencing helps young people to memorize letter shapes (Longcamp et al., 2005; Mangen and Velay, 2010; Stevenson and Just, 2014)—a skill that keyboarding alone does not necessarily accomplish. Children also recognize letters more efficiently when writing than when typing them (Longcamp et al., 2005). A student's handwriting legibility can influence whether he/she qualifies for special education services (Graham et al., 2008; Cahill, 2009), ultimately impacting the child's motivation and self-esteem (Gargot et al., 2020).

Reading and Handwriting in the Brain

Accounts of the ways in which both reading and handwriting are instantiated in the brain are remarkably similar. Both involve hierarchical sequences of processing. In their Local Combination Detector Model, Dehaene et al. (2005) describe how the brain processes increasingly complex combinations of visual features en route to reading. Kemmerer (2015) summarizes seven consecutive stages of processing in this “assembly line.” Processing begins when the retina of the eye perceives the visual stimulus and transmits information to the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus. Here, cells process simple stimuli such as series of dots. Following this step, the brain's primary visual center in the occipital lobe is engaged. The occipital lobe integrates the representations of the thalamic cells to capture bars or lines of a variety of orientations; captures sections of contours or fragments of letters; recognizes entire letter shapes; supports invariant processing while ignoring superficial differences in font and case; and finally, processes bigrams (sequences of two letters) and quadrigrams (sequences of four letters).

Writing to dictation also follows a hierarchical sequence, in this case with five steps. First, when the ear hears the word, the brain recognizes the sound structure of the word. Next, the brain accesses the spelling of that word within the orthographic lexicon. Following that step, the graphemic buffer—a working memory system which temporarily holds the identities of the graphemes while the word is being written—is activated. Next, allographic conversion occurs, and the graphemes are translated from their abstract identities into concrete forms, depending upon whether the writer wishes to use print, cursive, uppercase, or lowercase letters. Subsequently, graphomotor planning occurs, and the brain instructs the motor system as to the size, direction, and sequence of movements to be executed. Finally, the motor act of handwriting occurs (Kemmerer, 2015).

The interface between reading and writing has its source in the brain. Reading and writing share some, but not all, neural substrates (Hillis, 2001; Tainturier and Rapp, 2001; Hillis and Rapp, 2004; Purcell et al., 2011). Within the cortex, the area most finely tuned for reading is the visual word form area (VWFA). This area is located within the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus and is lateralized to the left hemisphere. The VWFA may be activated by both reading and writing (especially for words with atypical spellings); however, the evidence from studies of brain injuries is equivocal on this point (Kemmerer, 2015). The dorsal premotor region facilitates both recognizing and writing letters (Velay and Longcamp, 2013). Exner's area is one of the main writing centers of the brain, and it is also activated during reading (Pattamadilok et al., 2016). The handwriting network described by Planton et al. (2013), including the supplementary motor area (SMA), pre-SMA, and putamen, is also activated during reading (Gosse et al., 2022). These findings indicate that writing actively facilitates fluent reading (Pattamadilok et al., 2016). As Kemmerer (2015) states, “when we see letters, our brains automatically recall the motor memories of how we manipulate a pen or pencil to produce them” (p. 219).

What Clinicians Need to Know About the Interface

Individuals with reading differences/disorders including developmental dyslexia often experience persistent difficulties with handwriting (Sumner et al., 2016). Writing comprises the central components of language and orthography and the motor/peripheral components of execution (Planton et al., 2017). Gosse et al. (2022) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the writing network in children with dyslexia. They discovered that these children experienced differences in cerebellar activation in both central and peripheral components. Thus, writing is not just a motor skill, as individuals with dyslexia may have difficulties in writing which are not directly caused by difficulties in graphomotor planning. Difficulties in handwriting may be related to difficulties in spelling, as students who struggle with spelling also tend to write more slowly and less fluently (Berninger et al., 2008; Hebert et al., 2018).

Dyslexia often does not occur in isolation but is comorbid with other neurodevelopmental differences such as developmental coordination disorder (DCD; Hill, 2001; Visser, 2003). Authors of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) state that DCD has four diagnostic criteria: clumsiness, slowness, or inaccuracy in performing motor skills; difficulties that interfere with activities of daily living; early onset of symptoms; and symptoms that are not explained by intellectual disability, vision problems, or another underlying neurological condition. Children with DCD may experience difficulties dressing themselves (e.g., buttoning shirts or tying shoelaces), eating with utensils and without mess, playing physical games, using tools such as rulers and scissors in school, and—most relevant to the topic at hand—producing legible handwriting (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Motor difficulties in DCD may occur in either gross- or fine-motor skills (Harris et al., 2015).

There is a significant overlap between the experiences of individuals with language learning differences and individuals with DCD. Both may demonstrate weak hand lateralization and may be inclined to reach across their body's midline with their non-preferred hand (Hill et al., 1998). According to Hodgson and Hudson (2017), this is because unlike most adults with neurotypical development, individuals with neurodiversity may demonstrate altered hemispheric mapping and reduced left-hemisphere activation during speech production (Whitehouse and Bishop, 2008; Illingworth and Bishop, 2009). There are differences between these profiles as well. Berninger and Amtmann (2003) and Hebert et al. (2018) suggest that handwriting and spelling engage separate processes. Handwriting engages orthographic coding, whereas spelling engages both orthographic and phonological coding.

Professionals—including speech-language pathologists, school psychologists, and general and special education teachers—who work with individuals experiencing difficulties in reading, motor performance, or both, must be aware of several concepts. First, the task of writing is comparable to that of a juggler keeping many balls in the air simultaneously. These “balls” include planning one's thoughts, spelling, executing legible handwriting (McCutchen, 2006), and writing accurately. Often, the flight of one ball is sacrificed to keep the others aloft. Gombert (1992) describes this as a state of cognitive overload. Helping children to write more efficiently and automatically can allow them to direct their cognitive resources toward planning and generating ideas rather than dwelling on accurate spelling and handwriting (Wanzek et al., 2017). In clinical parlance, it is often beneficial to implement benign neglect. Rather than criticizing every aspect of the child's message, the clinician should focus on one aspect (e.g., grammar, spelling, handwriting, punctuation…) during a given lesson and neglect all other aspects to decrease the child's frustration. Concentrating on letter-writing fluency (LWF) as an instructional goal may be beneficial, as Reutzel et al. (2019) mention that LWF predicts academic success even in college students.

Clinicians may execute a range of strategies to utilize this interface to improve the learning outcomes of their clients. Specifically, multimodal practice involving the physical formation of letters can enhance reading outcomes. One treatment for reading employing multiple modalities and enjoying support in the literature is the Orton-Gillingham approach. In this approach, clinicians emphasize multisensory input, encouraging children to engage their visual, auditory, and kinesthetic/tactile learning pathways while breaking down reading into its individual components (Sayeski et al., 2019). The Montessori method, originally pioneered by Italian psychologist Maria Montessori, involves activities such as tracing letters cut from sandpaper, drawing letters in shaving cream, and conducting activities which combine vision and a sense of space (Dehaene, 2009). The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing Program (Lindamood and Lindamood, 1998) also incorporates oral-motor, visual, and auditory feedback during teaching. The Wilson Reading System is another multisensory approach which includes instruction in phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension (Duff et al., 2015). Teachers using Elkonin boxes instruct children in phoneme segmentation and blending through the manipulation of tokens (Ross and Joseph, 2019). These are just some examples of therapeutic techniques that can be used to direct the client's attention to both reading and handwriting skills through the use of multiple modalities. Improvements in spelling can lead to improvements in handwriting and vice versa (Hebert et al., 2018).

The use of technology (including iPad applications) can improve handwriting legibility and speed (John and Renumol, 2018), as well as other visuomotor skills, in even very young children (Dessoye et al., 2017; Axford et al., 2018; Butler et al., 2019). Technology may be beneficial beyond traditional intervention approaches within occupational therapy, such as an emphasis on repetitive or multimodal practice (Zachry et al., 2020). Using technology can be particularly valuable because it provides convenience to teachers and motivation to students (Aronin and Floyd, 2013; Campigotto et al., 2013; Kaur, 2017; Zachry et al., 2020). Practice with technology can also improve other fine-motor outcomes including manual dexterity (Butler et al., 2019).

Discussion

Reading and handwriting share similarities at many levels. Their developmental progression and their instantiation within the brain substantially overlap. Researchers and clinicians should be aware of symptoms that occur when underlying neurological processes are compromised as well as therapeutic techniques that can support the development of these two skills. The multifaceted interactions between these two modalities of language deserve further attention within the literature, especially concerning their practical implications in clinical settings.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was supported by the College of Health Sciences of Midwestern University, Downers Grove, IL, United States.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alamargot, D., Morin, M. F., and Simard-Dupuis, E. (2020). Handwriting delay in dyslexia: children at the end of primary school still make numerous short pauses when producing letters. J. Learn. Disabil. 53, 163–175. doi: 10.1177/0022219420903705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth ed. Author.

Google Scholar

Aronin, S., and Floyd, K. (2013). Using an iPad in inclusive preschool classrooms to introduce STEM concepts. Teach. Except. Child. 45, 34–39. doi: 10.1177/004005991304500404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axford, C., Joosten, A. V., and Harris, C. (2018). iPad applications that required a range of motor skills promoted motor coordination in children commencing primary school. Austr. Occup. Ther. J. 65, 146–155. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, R.. (1887). Eine besondere art der wortblindheit (dyslexie) [A Special Type of Wordblindness Dyslexia]. Bergmann.

Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., and Amtmann, D. (2003). “Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: research into practice,” in Handbook of Research on Learning Disabilities, eds. H. L. Swanson, K. Harris, and S. Graham, eds. (New York, NY: Guilford), 345–363.

Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., and Raskind, W. (2008). Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: under-recognized and under-treated. J. School Psychol. 46, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, W. H.. (1872). On the cerebral mechanism of speech and thought. Transactions of the Royal Med. Chirurg. Soc. 15, 330–357. doi: 10.1177/095952877205500108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, C., Pimenta, R., Tommerdahl, J., Fuchs, C. T., and Caçola, P. (2019). Using a handwriting app leads to improvement in manual dexterity in kindergarten children. Res. Learn. Technol. 27, 1–10. doi: 10.25304/rlt.v27.2135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, S. M.. (2009). Where does handwriting fit in? Strategies to support academic achievement. Interv. Sch. Clin. 44, 223–228. doi: 10.1177/1053451208328826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campigotto, R., McEwen, R., and Denmans Epp, C. (2013). Especially social: exploring the use of an iOS application in special needs classrooms. Comput. Educ. 60, 74–86. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, E., and Coates, A. (2016). The essential role of scribbling in the imaginative and cognitive development of young children. J. Early Child. Literacy 16, 60–83. doi: 10.1177/1468798415577871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., and Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: a proposal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 335–341. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S.. (2009). Reading in the Brain: The New Science of How We Read. New York, NY: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Dessoye, J., Converse-Korhonen, C., McLaughlin, L., McSweeney, S., and Steinhoff, C. (2017). The effectiveness of iPad handwriting applications in improving visual-motor and handwriting skills in children with autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 71(Suppl. 1), 7111520282p1. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2017.71S1-PO2149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duff, D., Stebbins, M. S., Stormont, M., Lembke, E. S., and Wilson, D. J. (2015). Using curriculum-based measurement data to monitor the effectiveness of the Wilson Reading System for students with disabilities: an exploratory study. Int. J. Disabil. Hum. Dev. 15, 93–100. doi: 10.1515/ijdhd-2015-0007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fears, N. E., and Lockman, J. J. (2020). Using head-mounted eye-tracking to study handwriting development. J. Motor Learn. Dev. 8, 215–231. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2018-0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feder, K. P., and Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 312–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gargot, T., Asselborn, T., Pellerin, H., Zammouri, I.M, Anzalone, S., et al. (2020). Acquisition of handwriting in children with and without dysgraphia: a computational approach. PLoS ONE 15, e0237575. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gombert, J. E.. (1992). Metalinguistic Development. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Gosse, C., Dricot, L., and Van Reybroeck, M. (2022). Evidence of graphomotor dysfunction in children with dyslexia A combined behavioural and fMRI experiment. Cortex 148, 68–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Mason, L., Fink-Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., and Saddler, B. (2008). How do primary grade teachers teach handwriting? A national survey. Read. Writ. 21, 49–69. doi: 10.1007/s11145-007-9064-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. R., Mickelson, E. C., and Zwicker, J. G. (2015). Diagnosis and management of developmental coordination disorder. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 187, 659–665. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.140994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebert, M., Kearns, D. M., Hayes, J. B., Bazis, P., and Cooper, S. (2018). Why children with dyslexia struggle with writing and how to help them. Languag. Speech Hear. Serv. School. 49, 843–863. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-DYSLC-18-0024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, E. L.. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 36, 149–171. doi: 10.1080/13682820010019874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, E. L., Bishop, D. V. M., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Representational gestures in developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment: error-types and the reliability of ratings. Hum. Mov. Sci. 17, 655–678. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00017-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillis, A. E.. (2001). “The organization of the lexical system,” in The Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology, eds B. Rapp (London: Psychology Press), 185–210.

Google Scholar

Hillis, A. E., and Rapp, B. C. (2004). “Cognitive and neural substrates of written language: comprehension and production,” in The Cognitive Neurosciences, Third Edn. eds M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 775–787.

Google Scholar

Hinshelwood, J.. (1900). Congenital word-blindness. Lancet 1, 1506–1508. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)99645-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgson, J. C., and Hudson, J. M. (2017). Atypical speech lateralization in adults with developmental coordination disorder demonstrated using functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound. J. Neuropsychol. 11, 1–13. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Illingworth, S., and Bishop, D. (2009). Atypical cerebral lateralisation in adults with compensated developmental dyslexia demonstrated using functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound. Brain Lang. 111, 61–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, S., and Renumol, V. G. (2018). “Impact of fine motor skill development app on handwriting performance in children with dysgraphia: a pilot study,” in Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Digital Technology in Education, New York, NY, 11–16.

Google Scholar

Kaur, D.. (2017). Preservice teachers' perceptions of using iPads with students with learning disabilities. Qual. Rep. 22, 2428–2436. doi: 10.46743/2160-3715/2017.2922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D.. (2015). Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Kussmaul, A.. (1887). “Disturbances of speech,” in Encyclopedia of the Practice of Medicine, eds H. von Ziemssen (New York, NY: William Wood).

Google Scholar

Lindamood, P., and Lindamood, P. (1998). The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing Program for Reading, Spelling, and Speech (LiPS), Third Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M., and Velay, J. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychol. 119, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangen, A., and Velay, J. L. (2010). Digitizing literacy: reflections on the haptics of writing. Adv. Haptics 1, 86–401. doi: 10.5772/8710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutchen, D.. (2006). “Cognitive factors in the development of children's writing,” in Handbook of Writing Research, eds C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, and J. Fitzgerald (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 115–130.

Google Scholar

Morgan, W. P.. (1896). A case of congenital word blindness. Br. Med. J. 2, 1378. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.1871.1378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myklebust, H. R., and Johnson, D. (1962). Dyslexia in children. Except. Child. 29, 14–26. doi: 10.1177/001440296202900103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattamadilok, C., Ponz, A., Planton, S., and Bonnard, M. (2016). Contribution of writing to reading: dissociation between cognitive and motor process in the left dorsal premotor cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1531–1543. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Planton, S., Jucla, M., Roux, F.-E., and Demonet, J.-F. (2013). The “handwriting brain”: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of motor versus orthographic processes. Cortex 49, 2772–2787. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Planton, S., Longcamp, M., Péran, P., Démonet, J. F., and Jucla, M. (2017). How specialized are writing-specific brain regions? An fMRI study of writing, drawing and oral spelling. Cortex. 88, 66–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purcell, J., Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., and Rapp, B. (2011). Examining the central and peripheral processes of written word production through meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2, 239. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reutzel, P., Mohr, K. A., and Jones, C. D. (2019). Exploring the relationship between letter recognition and handwriting in early literacy development. J. Early Child. Literacy 19, 349–374. doi: 10.1177/1468798417728099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, K. M., and Joseph, L. M. (2019). Effects of word boxes on improving students' basic literacy skills: a literature review. Prevent. Sch. Fail. Altern. Educ. Children Youth 63, 43–51. doi: 10.1080/1045988X.2018.1480006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayeski, K. L., Earle, G. A., Davis, R., and Calamari, J. (2019). Orton Gillingham: who, what, and how. Teach. Except. Child. 51, 240–249. doi: 10.1177/0040059918816996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, C. M.. (2012). “Learning to write,” in Language and Reading Disabilities, Third Edn, eds A. G. Kamhi and H. W. Catts (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson), 244–268.

Google Scholar

Stevenson, N. C., and Just, C. (2014). In early education, why teach handwriting before keyboarding? Early Child. Educ. J. 42, 49–56. doi: 10.1007/s10643-012-0565-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, E., Connelly, V., and Barnett, A. L. (2013). Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution. Read. Writ. 26, 991–1008. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9403-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, E., Connelly, V., and Barnett, A. L. (2016). The influence of spelling ability on vocabulary choices when writing for children with dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 49, 293–304. doi: 10.1177/0022219414552018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tainturier, M. J., and Rapp, B. (2001). “The spelling process,” in The Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology, eds B. Rapp (London: Psychology Press), 263–290.

Google Scholar

Velay, J. L., and Longcamp, M. (2013). “Motor skills and written language perception: Contribution of writing knowledge to visual recognition of graphic shapes,” in Language and Action in Cognitive Neuroscience, eds Y. Coello and A. Bartolo (London: Psychology Press), 161–176.

Google Scholar

Visser, J.. (2003). Developmental coordination disorder: a review of research subtypes and comorbidities. Hum. Mov. Sci. 22, 479–493. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wanzek, J., Gatlin, B., Al Otaiba, S., and Kim, Y. S. G. (2017). The impact of transcription writing interventions for first-grade students. Read. Writ. Quart. 33, 484–499. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2016.1250142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehouse, A., and Bishop, D. (2008). Cerebral dominance for language function in adults with specific language impairment or autism. Brain 131, 3193–3200. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zachry, A. H., Arnwine, R., and Garrett, A. (2020). The effectiveness of iPad apps to improve handwriting legibility in first and second grade students: a pilot study. J. Occup. Therapy Sch. Early Interven. 13, 410–419. doi: 10.1080/19411243.2020.1751768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelaznik, H. N., and Goffman, L. (2010). Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 53, 383–393. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0204)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reading, handwriting, visual word form area, typical development, dyslexia, developmental coordination disorder

Citation: Saletta Fitzgibbons M (2022) The Interface Between Reading and Handwriting. Front. Psychol. 13:892913. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.892913

Received: 09 March 2022; Accepted: 16 June 2022;
Published: 06 July 2022.

Edited by:

Giseli Donadon Germano, São Paulo State University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Rui Alexandre Alves, University of Porto, Portugal
Mohamad Ahmad Eid, New York University Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates

Copyright © 2022 Saletta Fitzgibbons. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Meredith Saletta Fitzgibbons, mfitzg@midwestern.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.