Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 June 2022
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Informal Learning Through Work View all 8 articles

Entrepreneurs Can Know More Than They Can Tell: Conceptualizing and Measuring Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge

  • Research Center for Entrepreneurship and Family Firms (RCEF), Faculty of Business Economics, Hasselt University, Hasselt, Belgium

Organizational knowledge components dominate research on tacit knowledge. In order to overcome this dominance, we introduce Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK). TEK is conceptualized as one’s experiential learning from past experiences and insights that result in tacit knowledge regarding entrepreneurship that is implicit, personal, and uncodified. For this study the situational judgment test (SJT) approach is adopted to overcome the common limitations in quantifying an individual’s tacit knowledge. The SJT is a scenario-based measurement instrument that allows us to quantify an individual’s TEK. The SJT is developed using three steps: first, scenarios were collected through interviews, followed by formulating responses to the scenarios, and finally, the effectiveness of the responses for each scenario was evaluated. The outcome of this research article is threefold; first, a comprehensive conceptualization of TEK, including delineation of its nomological network. Second, the development of a measurement instrument for TEK and subsequent scoring method. Finally, an antecedent-consequence model which includes potential contingencies associated with these relationships. In the debate on tacit knowledge, our measurement is innovative and relevant, as previous research failed to uncover an individual’s tacit knowledge in the context of entrepreneurship, despite its importance in various entrepreneurial processes. This study aspires to ignite research into TEK by demonstrating important research opportunities unlocked by our conceptualization and subsequent measurement, offering future researchers a wide range of avenues to uncover the black box of tacit knowledge in entrepreneurship.

Introduction

Tacit knowledge is widely accepted as one of the most valuable resources of any organization (Amit and Schoemaker, 1993). Scholars have provided evidence that tacit knowledge is an essential driver of firm competitiveness (Grant, 1996a; Smith et al., 2005), innovation (Cavusgil et al., 2003; Zhao et al., 2011), and the introduction of new services (Leiponen, 2006). However, entrepreneurship research on tacit knowledge has tended to be dominated by a focus on organizational-level knowledge components such as outsider assistance, firm investments in employees and training, and universities’ geographical proximity (Chrisman, 1999; Le Breton–Miller and Miller, 2006; Ghio et al., 2016). This course of action has yielded considerable advances, such as to how organizations learn (Brown and Duguid, 1991; Huber, 1991), how knowledge is created in organizations (Nelson and Winter, 1982; Nonaka, 1994; Grant, 1996b), and how knowledge affects firm performance (Wiklund and Shepherd, 2003).

This organizational perspective has pushed the research agenda for tacit knowledge in the direction of knowledge transfer (e.g., Kogut and Zander, 1993; Szulanski, 1996; Osterloh and Frey, 2000; Tsai, 2001), without measuring how much, which, or if there is any tacit knowledge whatsoever to be transferred. Yet, the intense focus on organizational-level knowledge components is remarkable as they circumvent the individual-level as the primary source of knowledge within organizations (Nelson and Winter, 1982; Nonaka, 1994; Grant, 1996b).

Indeed, the attunement of tacit knowledge to the individual is especially important in the context of entrepreneurship, as entrepreneurs’ competitive advantages emerge from their idiosyncratic experiences and insights (Hambrick and Mason, 1984; Sternberg, 2004). Entrepreneurs are guided by their own experiences and prior knowledge to respond appropriately to opportunities and leverage resources consistent with their objectives using their entrepreneurial judgments (Shane, 2000; Foss and Klein, 2012; Li et al., 2020). Disregarding the individual has limited our understanding of the individual’s tacit knowledge and understates the entrepreneur’s role at the helm of the venture (Shane, 2000; Shook et al., 2003).

In order to overcome this blind spot, we introduce a new concept: Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK). We define TEK as one’s experiential learning from past experiences and insights that result in tacit knowledge regarding entrepreneurship that is implicit, personal, and uncodified. TEK captures an individual’s tacit knowledge as an assemblage of experiences accumulated over a lifetime.

Next, we develop a measurement instrument for TEK. Traditionally, the difficulty in measuring tacit knowledge lies in the elusive nature of the concept. It is challenging to measure tacit knowledge by a standard measurement approach on account of its context-specific, embedded, and subjective character (Gertler, 2003; Tsoukas, 2003). We seek to overcome this important limitation by leveraging an alternative approach to quantify tacit knowledge, namely situational judgment tests (SJT). The SJT is an approach that is much more adapted to measuring and quantifying procedural elements, such as tacit knowledge, than standard approaches (Lievens et al., 2008; Motowidlo et al., 2018). This is because the differentiating factor of the SJTs lies in the use of work-related scenarios, proceeded by a range of potential responses, which is not only a valuable method of exploring entrepreneurship (Shook et al., 2003) but also provides a deeper look into the cognition of the entrepreneur and the social interactions. In addition, the SJT is a well-established selection tool, best known for its predictive power regarding job performance, and has been adapted to fit various industry domains, including academia (Wagner and Sternberg, 1985; Sternberg et al., 1993), accounting (Tan and Libby, 1997; Bol et al., 2018), education (Stemler et al., 2006; Pretz, 2008), management (Colonia-Willner, 1998; Armstrong and Mahmud, 2008). With the introduction of TEK and a subsequent measurement instrument, we make three significant contributions to the field of entrepreneurship.

First, we conceptualize TEK and distinguish the construct from related constructs in its nomological network. A conceptual delineation is vital as it captures the essential characteristics of the construct, its observable manifestations, and the interrelationships (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955). Articulating the nomological network of TEK has important implications for future research and practice, as it enables future researchers to test the empirical validity of TEK in relation to other variables.

Second, by developing a measurement instrument to quantify the TEK of individual entrepreneurs, we address the main empirical challenge of tacit knowledge, which is the operationalization of an individual’s tacit knowledge in the context of entrepreneurship (Sternberg, 2004; Baum et al., 2011; Bird, 2014; Andrews and Smits, 2018). In the debate on tacit knowledge, our measurement instrument is innovative and relevant, as previous research has failed to uncover an individual’s tacit knowledge in the context of entrepreneurship, despite its considerable importance in various entrepreneurial processes (Grant, 1996a; Smith et al., 2005). In addition, our measurement instrument allows for a more in-depth understanding of the entrepreneur’s behavior (Hlady-Rispal et al., 2021) rather than focusing on organizational-level knowledge components, such as technology tacitness and transferability thereof (Kogut and Zander, 1993).

Finally, with both our concept and subsequent instrument development, we aspire to ignite research into TEK. We reveal promising research opportunities by proposing an antecedent-consequence model, including potential contingencies associated with these relationships. The model offers future researchers a wide range of research avenues to uncover the black box of individual-level tacit knowledge in entrepreneurship.

The article is structured as follows. The following section clarifies the need for a new construct for the entrepreneurship domain, with a subsequent conceptualization of TEK. We then present the methodology used to develop the measurement instrument, followed by the validation of the instrument. Lastly, we provide implications for further research, supported by a reflection on our instrument.

Why a New Concept?

Before developing Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK), it is essential to clarify the necessity and value of a new concept to the field. For decades now, tacit knowledge has been scrutinized by many scholars for its elusive nature. The interest has arisen from explorations carried out from a philosophical point of view, such as Ryle (1949), Wittgenstein (1969), Taylor (1989), and Polanyi (2009). Michael Polanyi first described the term tacit knowing in his book, The Tacit Dimension (Polanyi, 2009). Tacit knowing was established on the premise that “we know more than we can tell” (Polanyi, 2009, p. 4) and found its way to various branches of the social sciences, including economics, psychology, and sociology. In the 1980s and 1990s, the focus of organizational studies centered on advancing the theory of the firm, in which knowledge had become an increasingly crucial explanatory factor (Mahoney and Pandian, 1992). The focus on organizational knowledge prompted the research on tacit knowledge and offered insights into how organizations learn (Brown and Duguid, 1991; Huber, 1991), how knowledge is created in organizations (Nelson and Winter, 1982; Nonaka, 1994; Grant, 1996b), how organizations acquire external knowledge (Cohen and Levinthal, 1990), transfer knowledge (Howells, 1996; Szulanski, 1996), both inter- (Tolstoy, 2010; Acs et al., 2013) as intra-organizational (Tsai, 2001; Park et al., 2015), and how knowledge affects firm performance (Wiklund and Shepherd, 2003). A continued insistence toward organizational knowledge is also apparent in entrepreneurship literature addressing tacit knowledge. Most entrepreneurship studies concentrate on an organizational-level component of tacit knowledge, such as outsider assistance for nascent entrepreneurs (Chrisman, 1999), investments in staff and training in the case of family businesses (Le Breton–Miller and Miller, 2006), the proportion of outside researchers and original researchers collaborating in science-based spin-offs (Knockaert et al., 2011). The intense focus on organizational knowledge components is remarkable, considering it bypasses the individual as the primary source of knowledge within organizations (Nelson and Winter, 1982; Nonaka, 1994; Grant, 1996b). Highlighting organizational knowledge components not only eludes the core principle of tacit knowledge, but also devalues the role of the individual entrepreneur at the helm of the firm (Shane, 2000; Shook et al., 2003). For instance, the influence of leading entrepreneurs on their respective venture, as exemplified by Jeff Bezos (Amazon) and Elon Musk (Tesla), cannot be represented by specific organizational components, such as investment in staff and training or the input of outsider assistance, but demands a more direct approach for tacit knowledge at the level of the individual entrepreneur.

Given the importance of tacit knowledge for individuals and organizations, it can be expected that robust measurements or indicators already exist to date—the opposite holds (Robins and Wiersema, 1995; Smith et al., 2009). Tacit knowledge is most often operationalized by proxies. Proxies that focus on the individual tend to overemphasize the importance of experience, such as nascent entrepreneurs’ work experience (Davidsson and Honig, 2003) or entrepreneurial and industrial experience in angel investors (Erzurumlu et al., 2019). In prior research, it has been argued that experience can be misleading and restrictive for tacit knowledge research since experience disregards acquired insights (Levinthal and March, 1993; West and Noel, 2009). In a sense, the proxy experience does not differ from other proxies such as age, certainly not in entrepreneurship. Since it implicitly assumes that tacit knowledge, as measured by experience, deus ex machina will increase and lead to a competitive advantage. So as experience increases, so do the chances of success. Proxies, to some extent, certainly have merit, for instance, in most macro-economic studies (Warhurst and Thompson, 2006). However, to further advance the entrepreneurship field, our ambition should be to uncover more fine-grained research questions and paths, which is not always possible using proxies. For the sake of clarity, this is not a rejection of proxies, but an affirmation for an appropriate measurement instrument for individual-level tacit knowledge, specifically tailored to the domain of entrepreneurship.

Conceptualizing Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge

Defining the Construct

TEK is defined as experiential learning from past experiences and insights that results in tacit knowledge regarding entrepreneurship that is implicit, personal, and uncodified (Kogut and Zander, 1992; Grant, 1996b). We foresee that individuals with a high level of TEK, based on their own idiosyncratic experiences and insights related to entrepreneurship, will enable a venture to generate competitive advantages (Reed and Defillippi, 1990; Spender and Grant, 1996). It is through the combination of personal experiences that TEK potentially becomes valuable, rare, non-imitable, and non-replaceable, and therefore a crucial resource for both the individual entrepreneur and the entrepreneurial firm (Barney, 1991). TEK thus represents an individual’s experiences, insights, and learning moments regarding entrepreneurship. Therefore, the construct transcends sectors or industries since it emphasizes the individual entrepreneurs’ experiences, not their ventures. Entrepreneurs often recognize opportunities due to the knowledge they previously acquired (Shane, 2000). More specifically, people often have different beliefs based upon an inkling, instinct, or exclusive information, which is critical in discovering entrepreneurial opportunities (Shane and Venkataraman, 2000). This type of information, which can lead to a competitive advantage, is gained through knowledge incurred throughout personal experiences. TEK has a latent presence within any entrepreneur and is instrumental in deploying resources according to their objectives. Entrepreneurs facing identical situations, receiving identical information, and pursuing identical objectives will interpret the situation differently and make different decisions, drawing on their tacit knowledge as entrepreneurs (Foss and Klein, 2012). Nascent entrepreneurs tend to accrue a certain amount of TEK, even with limited entrepreneurial experience. Exposure to entrepreneurial role models, participation in entrepreneurship education, and past work experience in entrepreneurial firms shape each individual’s understanding of entrepreneurship (Peterman and Kennedy, 2003; Zapkau et al., 2017). In sum, as every entrepreneur has a different background (i.e., experience and education), the amount of TEK within the individual entrepreneur can be very heterogeneous since tacit knowledge varies between experts and novices (Wagner and Sternberg, 1985).

Through the conceptualization of TEK as a separate construct, it is clear that if we continue to make progress in this research domain, a measurement instrument for TEK is imperative.

The Nomological Network for Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge

In order to distinguish TEK from other related constructs and position it in the research context, we present a framework based on the essential characteristics of the concept to delineate a nomological network for TEK, in which it occurs (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955). Our framework consists of three dimensions: knowledge, experience, and time (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Three-dimensional framework of Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge’s (TEK) basic characteristics.

The first dimension of the framework relates to the distinction between tacit and explicit knowledge. The distinction between tacit and explicit knowledge offers more insight into how experiences are transformed in an entrepreneur’s memory. Since tacit knowledge is implicitly present in everyone, yet only accessible by “engaging in or running through the skills in which the knowledge is embedded” (Squire, 1986, p. 188). It is a fragment of procedural memory, acquired through personal experience, therefore directly influential for behavior and not readily articulable (Sternberg et al., 2000). In contrast, explicit knowledge is accumulated through formal training or teaching and feeds an individual’s semantic memory. Semantic memory refers to the capacity to store and recollect factual information and is especially necessary for the use of language (Tulving, 1972; Ribas Fernandes and Holroyd, 2017). It is seen as context-free memory concerning facts and is also called the general knowledge about the world (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997; Squire and Zola, 1998). While TEK is related to Baron and Ensley’s (2006) cognitive frameworks, the concepts differ in terms of tacit and explicit knowledge. The cognitive frameworks are also acquired through experience and play a critical role in the recognition of entrepreneurial patterns (Baron and Ensley’s, 2006). But they rely upon a combination of both explicit and tacit knowledge, whereas TEK relies solely on the latter.

The second dimension outlines the perspective of experiences. As Reuber and Fischer (1999) exemplified, experiences can be considered from two perspectives: a continuous stream or a constant stock (Reuber and Fischer, 1999). Since TEK is dynamic and the culmination of event-based experiences to capture an entrepreneur’s ongoing learning process concerning tacit knowledge, TEK can be seen as a continuous stream of experience, upon which past and future experiences are impacted, as opposed to a consistent stock of experience (Reuber and Fischer, 1999; Cope, 2005). In contrast, experiences scrutinized as stocks indicate a steady state at any given time, including depth and breadth of experience, and emphasize an exogenous learning process (Reuber and Fischer, 1999; Westhead et al., 2005). Measures such as age, specific experience (e.g., management, industry, and start-up), and education can be regarded as stocks of knowledge, considering these stocks can be summed together to produce an aggregate.

The final dimension outlines the aspect of time in the TEK construct. Experiential learning in entrepreneurship proceeds in separate dynamic temporal phases (Reuber and Fischer, 1999; Cope, 2005), and research on experiential learning tends to favor experiences and insights that occurred throughout the firm’s lifecycle (Naffziger et al., 1994; Cope, 2005). As opposed to mainstream concepts, TEK is not confined by temporal phases. On the contrary, it considers all insights gained from the entrepreneur’s experiences, whether before, during, or after dissolving the venture. TEK is not limited to a specific temporal phase but instead places the focal point on the life of the individual entrepreneur, even before the start of the entrepreneurial journey. Adjacent constructs, such as learning-by-doing, are reduced to learning elements that occur during the firm’s lifecycle (Cope and Watts, 2000).

The previous sections clarified the need for a new construct for the entrepreneurship domain and conceptualized TEK. In the following section, we will present the instrument development process, followed by the validation of the instrument.

Scenario Development

Methodology

Since tacit knowledge is characterized as context-specific, embedded, and subjective, it has for many years been considered extremely difficult to measure with standard measurement methods (Gertler, 2003; Tsoukas, 2003). Therefore, we are turning to an alternative approach, the situational judgment test (SJT). This approach is well-known for quantifying tacit knowledge (Lievens et al., 2008; Motowidlo et al., 2018), because SJTs employ work-related scenarios, followed by a set of possible responses. In so doing, the context is taken into account, and a measurement instrument is created in which reverberations between the entrepreneur’s cognition and social interactions are quantified. For this study, we opted for an SJT developmental approach consistent with the current literature on tacit knowledge (e.g., Wagner and Sternberg, 1985, 1987; Cianciolo et al., 2006; Sternberg, 2006). Developing an SJT is usually characterized by three steps (see Figure 2 for a full description of the development approach): first, the scenarios are collected through interviews, followed by formulating the responses to the scenarios, and finally, the effectiveness of the responses for each scenario is evaluated (Motowidlo et al., 1990; Lievens et al., 2008). As is standard procedure for SJTs, we engage with subject matter experts to develop our instrument. We recruited a panel of 14 subject matter experts based on the following three criteria: first, each entrepreneur needed to be the current and founding CEO of a high-growth firm in order to ensure that the entrepreneur has played a significant role in starting up a business venture (Stuart and Abetti, 1990). The first criterion ensured that the expert entrepreneur had gone through an evolution with the company and excluded inexperienced entrepreneurs from the panel. Second, the venture should apply to the high-growth firm definition, as defined by the European Commission (Eurostat - OECD, 2007), demonstrating an average annualized growth greater than 20% per annum in terms of employees or turnover, over a 3-year period. This criterion is not so much important for the stage in which the organization finds itself (Levie and Lichtenstein, 2010), but its importance lies in the distinguishing capacity of growth-oriented entrepreneurs. Assuming that entrepreneurs have succeeded in growing their new venture into a high-growth firm, we substantiated entrepreneurial success. Finally, there should be no more than two CEOs from corresponding sectors represented in the expert panel. By purposefully eliciting TEK from entrepreneurs active in different sectors, the findings will be allowed to be replicated within divergent sectors (Eisenhardt, 1989).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Situational judgment test (SJT) development approach.

Scenario Development

The subject matter experts were subjected to in-depth interviews following the critical interview technique, which is a qualitative procedure defined by Chell (2004) as “the investigation of significant occurrences (events, incidents, processes, or issues), identified by the respondent, the way they are managed, and the outcomes in terms of perceived effects. The objective is to gain an understanding of the incident from the perspective of the individual, taking into account cognitive, affective and behavioral elements” (p. 48). The mostly unstructured interviews addressed the entrepreneurs’ thought processes and feelings about crucial lessons they had learned over time, and their experience as successful entrepreneurs, emphasizing early career experiences (Sternberg et al., 2000; Taylor et al., 2013). Respondents extensively discussed the essential lessons, described the incidents in detail, and were asked to consider its impact on their development as entrepreneurs and its influence on business development (Chell and Baines, 2000). The entrepreneurs could elaborate on these lessons by clarifying them in the form of cases or stories (Sternberg et al., 2000). All interviews were recorded, and 47 interview extracts of the fragments that potentially contained a TEK component were produced (a detailed description of the extract selection is provided under instrument validity).

Responses

The final selection of interview extracts was drafted in brief scenarios, each followed by five suggested responses. The details in both the scenarios and response options were anonymized, such as organizations, entrepreneurs, and industry names, and were constructed using information from the interviews. This process resulted in 17 scenarios based on work-related circumstances, where a problem statement or case was formulated, followed by a range of potential responses specific to the preceding problem or case. Again, the subject matter experts were consulted to evaluate the 17 scenarios and their potential responses on three levels. First, they were asked to assess the consistency between the scenarios and response options or statements. Second, they evaluated the comprehensiveness of the response options, and finally, the quality of the inquiry (Sternberg et al., 2000). Whenever the experts proposed adjustments or indicated inconsistencies, an individual consultation was initiated to discuss the possible adjustments or potential inconsistencies.

Evaluation

As a final step in the SJT development approach, the effectiveness of the responses for each scenario was evaluated by the subject matter experts. The subject matter experts were asked to evaluate the final version of the scenario-based measurement. This final consultation round required the members to relay the extent of agreement with each scenario’s subsequent statements, using a seven-point Likert scale to develop the expert mean responses. After all, panelists had given their evaluation ratings, and the TEK instrument was created, consisting of 17 scenarios1 (see example scenario in Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Example scenario with five statements.

Methodological Implications

The main objective of this study lies in the development of the TEK construct and its measurement instrument. Due to its implicit and unobservable nature, no objective measurements of the criterion variable are available to date. Therefore, we decided to use the rational scoring approach suggested by Lievens et al. (2008) by relying on the judgments of subject matter experts for the TEK instrument. As opposed to an empirical scoring approach typically conducted on a large pilot sample (Lievens et al., 2008), a rational scoring approach is a commonly accepted scoring method for SJTs and consists of deriving the TEK-score for each scenario by comparing the results to a control group (Bergman et al., 2006). Since an SJT is an auspicious tool for overcoming cross-cultural validity issues (Peng et al., 1997), future researchers can relatively easily compile their own expert responses and subsequent benchmark.

The comparison between participants and the control group is performed by calculating the Mahalanobis distance (MD). The MD standardizes the data and adjusts for correlations among variables and is therefore particularly suitable for TEK since it measures similarity between groups using a multidimensional method across cases and variables (Hair et al., 2014). Typically, the MD is used to detect outliers in multivariate data (Penny, 1996). The standard approach pools all data to determine the centroid (i.e., the average discriminant z-score for all group members) and subsequent outliers from that centroid. In this study, we adopt an alternative approach, where the control group’s centroid is constructed, and the respondent’s distance is measured from that centroid. According to this approach, the control group’s centroid is used as the benchmark to calculate the respondents’ scores. This application is burgeoning in several research fields, such as archaeometry, and has merit over the standard approach (Karacic et al., 2016; Overholtzer et al., 2020). The main advantage is that by omitting the respondents from the control group’s centroid, the respondents are prevented from influencing the control group’s characteristics, leading to an unbiased comparison, especially important in smaller samples (Leese and Main, 1994).

The final scores are derived using a series of calculations2 based on 17 scenarios (j = 17) where each scenario has five potential reactions (i = 5) that can each be scored on a seven-point Likert scale. First, the distance between the respondent’s rating for each item (xij) and the control group (μ¯) is calculated.

x ij - μ ¯

Second, for each scenario, the inverse of the covariance matrix (C−1) of the control group’s responses is created.

C - 1

Third, the transpose of the distance between the participant’s rating for each item (xij) and the expert group (μ¯) is calculated.

( x ij - μ ¯ ) T

Fourth, a D2 per scenario per respondent is calculated by multiplying the three preceding computations.

D ij 2 = ( x ij - μ ¯ ) C - 1 ( x ij - μ ¯ ) T

Fifth, the MD is characterized by a chi-square distribution, and the number of variables per scenario equals the degrees of freedom (i.e., df = 5) (Hahs-Vaughn and Lomax, 2020). In order to calculate a TEK-score per scenario, each D2 is then transformed based on the respective chi-squared value for 5 degrees of freedom at a 0.001 confidence level, which, according to Hair et al. (2014), equals a critical value of 20.515. The transformation involves adjusting any outcome compared to the respective critical value. On the one hand, if the calculated D2 is greater than the critical value of 20.515, the TEK-score is transformed to zero. Since the MD is typically used for outlier detection, values greater than the critical value are treated as an outlier and are not assigned a score (Hair et al., 2014). On the other hand, if D2 is smaller than or equal to the critical value of 20.515, the TEK-score is transformed by subtracting the outcome by the critical value.

if D ij 2 > 20.515 then TEK ij = 0
if D ij 2 20.515 then TEK ij = 20.515 - D ij 2

Finally, to calculate the final TEK-score, the TEK-scores per scenario are added up. To facilitate interpretation, all scores are converted to 100 by multiplying the final scores by 100 and dividing them by 348.755.3

TEK total = ( TEK j = 1 + TEK j = 2 + + TEK j = 17 ) 100 348.755

The final two steps in the score calculation process ensure that TEK represents a score ranging between 0 and 100. The closer a respondent’s score approaches 100, the more significant that individual’s TEK is.

Instrument Validity

Reliability and validity are critical when developing a new measurement to ensure methodological rigor (Crook et al., 2010). In contrast to standard scale development techniques, our scenario development approach aligns closely with the qualitative case study method (Eisenhardt, 1989). Therefore, following the positive tradition, which is a conventional perspective in qualitative research (Aguinis and Solarino, 2019; Grodal et al., 2020; Pratt et al., 2020), the following four criteria were used to establish the rigor of the outcome scenarios: construct validity, internal validity, external validity, and reliability (Lincoln and Guba, 1986; Eisenhardt, 1989; Yin, 2018).

The construct validity is ensured when the scenarios offer a correct reflection of the focal construct, i.e., Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK) (Wigren, 2007; Gibbert et al., 2008). TEK, as a focal construct, was elicited by applying different strategies (Yin, 2018). First, by assembling multiple sources of evidence (i.e., subject matter experts). Second, it was possible to document a structured chain of evidence based on a predetermined protocol (i.e., SJT development approach). Third, a step toward construct validity was taken by subjecting the interview extracts to 3 assessments.

Following Sternberg et al. (2000), the first assessment consisted of translating the excerpts into the procedural form to evaluate whether the interview extract matched the procedural form. The procedural form consisted of a set of antecedent conditions (i.e., IF) and consequent actions (i.e., THEN), allowing fewer interpretation possibilities (Sternberg et al., 2000). These statements were combined by taking the connective of the statements (i.e., AND, OR, or ELSE) (see Table 2). After formatting all interview extracts into a procedural form, 1 extract did not fit into the procedural pattern and was subsequently removed.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Combination of the procedural format and contextual stories.

The second assessment involved an internal research team evaluating the remaining extracts. 46 encoded summaries and their subsequent transcribed extracts formed the internal review process’s input. This combination enabled the internal panel members to compare and evaluate the procedural format (i.e., the encoded summaries) and the contextual stories (i.e., interview extracts). The internal research team consisted of 3 researchers4 to conduct the internal review. All team members were familiar with the concept of tacit knowledge and evaluated each interview extract individually. The evaluation was based on 2 criteria for tacit knowledge: (1) the knowledge on the extracts had to contain information drawn from the entrepreneurs’ own experiences, insights, and individual learnings, and (2) they should not consist of information that is available in academic articles, books, classes, etc. (Sternberg et al., 2000). After the individual evaluation, the internal reviewers discussed the remaining extracts that were not evaluated unanimously and reached a consensus. This first validation process eliminated 12 interview extracts, reducing the preliminary collection of 46 extracts to 34.

The third assessment consisted of quality control by the subject matter experts, who were asked to rate the remaining 34 extracts according to three dimensions, using a five-point Likert scale (Sternberg et al., 2000). The 3 dimensions entailed (1) perceived quality, (2) the acceptance amongst entrepreneurs, and (3) the frequency of occurrence. We created an overall fit index for the remaining extracts based on the entrepreneurs’ responses across these three dimensions. The overall fit index considered the average across the three dimensions of each extract. As a result of averaging across the three dimensions, all dimensions were equally considered instead of giving weight to a particular dimension. Extracts that scored below average were deleted, resulting in a final selection of 17 interview extracts. Finally, construct validity was achieved by involving the entrepreneurs in every aspect of the validation process, such as the initial interviews, assessing the quality of the extracts, and evaluating the final scenarios on consistency and logic, ensuring that the final result inevitably corresponds to the focal construct.

External validity indicates whether the findings are generalizable across contexts and domains (Gibbert et al., 2008; Yin, 2018). The first step toward external validity was taken when refining the boundary criteria for the domain experts. In our research, we sought to interview entrepreneurs from various branches to increase the representativeness of the elicited knowledge. By purposefully including entrepreneurs active in different sectors, the findings will be allowed to be replicated within divergent sectors (Eisenhardt, 1989). The expert panel was selected for its expected relevance in illuminating the concept of TEK and is not meant to be representative of any population. They are selected through theoretical sampling, which Eisenhardt and Graebner (2007) describe as follows: “just as laboratory experiments are not randomly sampled from a population of experiments, but rather, chosen for the likelihood that they will offer theoretical insight, so too are cases sampled for theoretical reasons, such as the revelation of an unusual phenomenon, replication of findings from other cases, contrary replication, elimination of alternative explanations, and elaboration of the emergent theory” (p. 27).

A study’s reliability demonstrates the study’s replicability in terms of consistency, logic, and traceability (Lincoln and Guba, 1986; Wigren, 2007; Yin, 2018). This study’s methodology and subsequent scenario development follow a well-documented protocol and approach. Continuous involvement of the expert panel throughout the process, an internal and external evaluation of the interview extracts concerning theory (i.e., internal assessment) and practice (i.e., external assessments), and the precise notation of each step ensure that this study is replicable in other contexts. Replication does not imply, as in quantitative research, that identical findings can be reproduced using the same data and the same procedures. For qualitative research, this is not appropriate or attainable (Pratt et al., 2020).

It is important to note that the preceding four quality criteria are not different criteria but interrelated (McGrath, 1981; Gibbert et al., 2008). An example of this interrelatedness is interaction with the expert panel in relation to the interview extracts. According to Sternberg et al. (2000), the subject matter experts’ quality control results in interview extracts represent experience and indicate entrepreneurial success, elevating the outcome scenarios internal, external, reliability, and construct validity. The construction of a clear scenario development approach also contributed to the internal validity and the procedure’s reliability.

Discussion and Contributions

Prior research has mainly focused on organizational-level tacit knowledge. Although these studies have provided many valuable insights, the central tenet of tacit knowledge, which is the individual as the primary source of knowledge within the firm, is most often circumvented or neglected. Therefore, this study aimed to develop an individual tacit knowledge measurement for the entrepreneurship domain, namely Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK). We believe this is an essential step for entrepreneurship research as, until today, entrepreneurship scholars had to rely on proxies, such as prior experience, to quantify an entrepreneur’s individual-level tacit knowledge.

Throughout this study, valuable lessons from experienced entrepreneurs were elicited, transformed into scenarios, and validated according to established criteria. This qualitative development of a scenario-based measurement for TEK draws on Wagner and Sternberg (1985) groundbreaking work and is based upon a sound methodological foundation (Armstrong and Mahmud, 2008). The sheer breadth and depth of scientific work provided by Wagner, Sternberg, and colleagues on tacit knowledge provide compelling evidence to broaden this method to the field of entrepreneurship.

The introduction of TEK is important to entrepreneurship, as it represents an undebated yet crucial element within the scholarly field. The development of the scenario-based measurement offers the entrepreneurship field the possibility to investigate an entrepreneur’s TEK. Placing TEK at the center of an antecedent-consequence model (see Figure 3) and exploring potential contingencies that serve both ends of these relationships reveals the applicability of this study. Moreover, the contribution of TEK as a construct becomes abundantly clear from an empirical point of view when considering the predictive power of tacit knowledge, which has already been established in other domains (Sternberg, 1997). Such an instrument adds value to the field of entrepreneurship as it enables us to measure a crucial entrepreneurial concept that researchers have failed to grasp so far. It brings to light future research paths that may enrich entrepreneurship literature and advance our understanding of the black box of an individual’s tacit knowledge in entrepreneurship.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Antecedent-consequence model of Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK).

Antecedents of Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge

TEK is accumulated as a result of past experiences, insights, and learnings regarding entrepreneurship, which makes it self-evident that TEK is influenced by factors that enable the individual to develop a specific image of entrepreneurship, identify opportunities, and allocate resources to achieve their entrepreneurial objectives. As possible antecedents of TEK, the primary focus in this study lies in an individual’s learning path and how it might be instrumental in the accumulation of TEK.

This ability is reflected in a person’s learning orientation, which is the tendency to cultivate competence by obtaining new skills and comprehending new situations (Dweck, 1986; VandeWalle, 1997; De Clercq et al., 2013). Empirical evidence demonstrates that learning-oriented individuals are consistently open to new experiences (VandeWalle et al., 2001), prefer to engage in more challenging tasks (Elliott and Dweck, 1988), and appreciate activities that might enhance personal development (VandeWalle and Cummings, 1997). As it is a fact that tacit knowledge is best transmitted through social interactions and experiences (Cook and Brown, 1999; Haldin-Herrgard, 2000), an individual’s willingness or openness to such valuable learning opportunities governs his exposure to TEK.

The same holds for prior entrepreneurial exposure (PEX). PEX is understood as an individual’s personal history related to entrepreneurship, such as entrepreneurial parents or prior work experience in entrepreneurial firms (Krueger, 1993; Zapkau et al., 2017). While the individual might not be an entrepreneur (yet), entrepreneurial insights are already being gathered through exposure from within the family or beyond, forming a breeding ground for possible future entrepreneurial development. This is how even nascent entrepreneurs tend to amass a certain amount of TEK. It has been argued that PEX facilitates the transfer of tacit knowledge, yet evidence remains sparse (Dohse and Walter, 2012). The introduction of TEK into the PEX-literature has the potential to shed light on this subject.

Consequences of Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge

An individual who has amassed TEK has a distinct perspective on the entrepreneurial process and guides his venture based on personal insights and experiences (Hambrick, 2007). The idiosyncratic combination of experiences makes this knowledge valuable, rare, non-replaceable, and non-imitable (Barney, 1991), and therefore an asset to both the entrepreneur and the organization (Hambrick and Mason, 1984). This is reflected in a dichotomous disaggregation of the consequences of TEK. On the one hand, the consequences of TEK for the individual entrepreneur and, on the other hand, the consequences of TEK for the organization.

At the individual level, TEK can be considered an explanatory factor for nascent entrepreneurship. The impact of TEK on entrepreneurial intentions would be a fruitful research avenue to investigate since these entrepreneurial intentions are partly based on intuition or hunches (Bird, 1988), and the cognitive processes behind intentions are not yet fully understood (Krueger et al., 2000). As Ajzen’s (1991) theory of planned behavior is often used to study the entrepreneurial intentions of (nascent) entrepreneurs, it would be interesting to investigate the extent to which TEK influences entrepreneurial intent drivers. For example, TEK may serve as the antecedent of personal attitude, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control in the decision-making process of becoming an entrepreneur.

Research on passion proliferates in entrepreneurship literature, mainly because of its positive impact on several vital outcomes (Murnieks et al., 2020). Yet to date, there is little understanding of the determinants of passion (Shepherd, 2015). Prior research has already identified several important stimuli for entrepreneurial passion, such as an individual’s social capital (Hoang and Gimeno, 2010), making way for TEK as an additional explanatory factor. TEK can be a possible impetus for this research, for instance, by investigating the relationship between an individual’s TEK and entrepreneurial passion (i.e., passion for inventing, founding, and developing) (Cardon et al., 2013). We contend that a high TEK-score, which equates to being proficient in entrepreneurship, results in a higher intense positive feeling associated with the different dimensions of entrepreneurial passion.

Finally, the prospect of inspiring and motivating followers is an essential prerequisite for many entrepreneurial founders. Although research on leadership in entrepreneurship is burgeoning, cognitive processes influencing a founder’s leadership behavior are sparse (Leitch and Volery, 2017). Jensen and Luthans (2006) emphasize the importance of prior experience in exhibiting leadership behavior. Consequently, TEK may also be applied as a potential explanatory factor for various leadership behaviors, such as transactional and transformational leadership.

At the organizational level, a crucial factor in the longevity of a venture’s success is its dynamic capabilities (Teece et al., 1997). A firm’s dynamic capabilities are defined as “the capacity (1) to sense and shape opportunities and threats, (2) to seize opportunities, and (3) to maintain competitiveness through enhancing, combining, protecting, and, when necessary, reconfiguring the business enterprise’s intangible and tangible assets” (Teece, 2007, p. 1,319). Organizational dynamic capabilities are closely intertwined with its founder, particularly in the context of a new business venture (Adner and Helfat, 2003; Teece, 2012). Both Zahra et al. (2006) and Teece (2007) have argued that entrepreneurial activities play a crucial role in the theory of dynamic capabilities since “the invisible hand must have fingers that can work in a coordinated fashion” (Augier and Teece, 2009, p. 410). Experiential learning lies at the heart of developing dynamic capabilities (Zollo and Winter, 2002), and this type of learning results from the tacit accumulation of experiences. Therefore, we believe TEK to be a driver for organizational dynamic capabilities. Moreover, we suspect founders who have amassed high TEK levels to influence their venture’s dynamic capabilities positively.

An additional role for TEK at the organizational level is evident in new ventures. New ventures are often initiated and led by a collective (Beckman, 2006; Schjoedt et al., 2013; Klotz et al., 2014), resulting in the convergence of people with different backgrounds, experiences, and mental models (Beckman, 2006; Leung et al., 2013). These emerging ventures depend on and utilize their new venture team members’ knowledge due to the absence of capital, organizational structures, and formalized rules (Gilbert et al., 2006; Dai et al., 2016). It is the new venture team’s collective perspective that steers organizational decisions (Mitchell et al., 2007; West, 2007). Many scholars find consensus for the fact that collective knowledge exceeds an individual’s cognitive capabilities (Knockaert et al., 2011; Bryant, 2014), but what knowledge is addressed or exchanged to elevate the collective remains unclear (de Mol et al., 2015). Moreover, how is valuable information regarding opportunities distributed among the collective? TEK represents a possible explanatory factor for the distribution of collective knowledge, considering that antecedents have received little attention in scientific research (Seyb et al., 2019).

Contingencies

The relationship between internal characteristics and organizational effectiveness is highly influenced by contextual factors (Donaldson, 2001). We posit that this is also the case for an individual’s TEK. Certain contingencies can potentially influence the main effect between TEK and its antecedents or consequences. Since the individual is at the center of our model, several important contextual factors can be considered, such as firm size, industry, and institutional conditions.

Small and large companies are equipped with different resources and capabilities to interact with their business environment (Dean et al., 1998). The size differences are also apparent in the firm’s tacit knowledge resources (Antonelli and Scellato, 2015). Examples include a nascent entrepreneur who relies only on his own knowledge stock (Chrisman, 1999) or his new venture team (Schjoedt et al., 2013) to pursue his entrepreneurial endeavors. Large firms rely more on highly qualified personnel to innovate through formal R&D activities leading to intellectual property and patents (Antonelli and Scellato, 2015). Therefore, we expect a variance of firm size (either in prior or present work experience) to affect the central relation between TEK and its antecedents or consequences.

Much of the current literature on intra-firm knowledge transfer pays particular attention to the role of the industry. However, there are conflicting rationales for this role. On the one hand, some industries require a particular shared team background and experience to facilitate tacit knowledge transfer (Knockaert et al., 2011). On the other hand, there are industries where team heterogeneity is necessary to innovate and respond appropriately to external events or opportunities (Beckman, 2006). Given the differences in knowledge intensity, an exploration of the sector heterogeneity as a contingency variable on the process of accumulation and distribution of an individual’s TEK, in line with Park et al. (2015), would be an exciting research avenue.

The macro-economic perspective of entrepreneurship mainly explores a growth relationship, making way for a more deliberate focus on the individual’s unique sets of resources (Terjesen et al., 2016). Therefore, we believe that the introduction of TEK into the macro-economic perspective would unlock a broader perspective for the field. Scientific evidence suggests that institutional conditions, such as a nation’s start-up procedures, unemployment rate, income disparity, FDI inflows, and labor market conditions, can potentially impact a country’s total level of entrepreneurial activity (Casson and Wadeson, 2007; Chowdhury et al., 2015). Therefore, we posit that beneficial institutional conditions can enhance the relationships between TEK and its antecedent and consequences, respectively.

Implications for Policy and Practice

This research has implications for different stakeholders in the entrepreneurial ecosystem for a variety of reasons. The TEK measure developed in this paper can be used by different stakeholders to reduce the information asymmetry related to the tacit knowledge of an entrepreneur. For instance, TEK can be a valuable instrument for banks to make an initial, preliminary assessment of entrepreneurs who turn up for a loan. Consistent with this argument, business angels and venture capitalists can make a better ex-ante assessment of new ventures and their entrepreneurs. In line with this reasoning, accelerators can integrate TEK into their selection processes. Moreover, the TEK measure developed in this paper can be used by entrepreneurs themselves in their search for new employees or cofounders. Namely, research suggests that companies need entrepreneurs within their organization (i.e., intrapreneurs) to drive innovation and explore new opportunities (Kraus et al., 2019). The TEK tool can help identify the tacit knowledge related to entrepreneurship of potential employees or cofounders during the recruitment process.

Conclusion

Prior research and theory have suggested that a firm’s tacit knowledge is a crucial ingredient for success. However, tacit knowledge has many hidden secrets still to be discovered, especially its exploration on the individual level. Due to the longstanding empirical standstill, many research paths have remained unexplored. The introduction of an SJT as a valuable analytical tool makes way for a new stream of research to unravel the role of individual Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge (TEK). With the development of TEK, it is now possible to quantify an entrepreneur’s insights, experiences, and individual learnings (Sternberg et al., 2000) and its subsequent effect on organizational knowledge (Nelson and Winter, 1982; Kogut and Zander, 1992; Nonaka, 1994; Grant, 1996b). This article has endeavored to outline essential research gaps and promising research avenues for further research. We hope to revitalize inquiry on tacit knowledge in entrepreneurship and, by extension, in the broader management domain.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon request, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by Prof. Dr. Liesbeth Todts and UHasselt Ethics Committee. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This study was supported by the Special Research Fund (BOF) of Hasselt University (BOF19OWB25).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ The complete TEK instrument is available from the corresponding author upon request.
  2. ^ An Excel form for the calculations is available from the corresponding author upon request.
  3. ^ 348.755 is calculated by multiplying the critical value (i.e., 20.515) by the number of scenarios (i.e., 17).
  4. ^ None of the authors collaborating in this research study were included. The internal research team consisted of faculty members closely familiar with the concept of tacit knowledge (1 postdoctoral researcher, 1 associate professor and 1 full professor).

References

Acs, Z. J., Audretsch, D. B., and Lehmann, E. E. (2013). The knowledge spillover theory of entrepreneurship. Small Bus Econ. 41, 757–774.

Google Scholar

Adner, R., and Helfat, C. E. (2003). Corporate effects and dynamic managerial capabilities. Strat. Manag. J. 24, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1002/smj.331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguinis, H., and Solarino, A. M. (2019). Transparency and replicability in qualitative research: the case of interviews with elite informants. Strat. Manag. J. 40, 1291–1315.

Google Scholar

Ajzen’s, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizat. Behav. Hum. Dec. Proc. 50, 179–211.

Google Scholar

Amit, R., and Schoemaker, P. J. H. (1993). Strategic assets and organizational rent. Strat. Manag. J. 14, 33–46. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250140105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews, M., and Smits, S. J. (2018). Knowing what we know: uncovering tacit knowledge for improved organizational performance. J. Org. Psychol. 18, 26–43.

Google Scholar

Antonelli, C., and Scellato, G. (2015). Firms size and directed technological change. Small Bus Econ. 44, 207–218. doi: 10.1007/s11187-014-9593-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, S. J., and Mahmud, A. (2008). Experiential learning and the acquisition of managerial tacit knowledge. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 7, 189–208. doi: 10.5465/amle.2008.32712617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Augier, M., and Teece, D. J. (2009). Dynamic capabilities and the role of managers in business strategy and economic performance. Org. Sci. 20, 410–421. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1090.0424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barney, J. (1991). Firm resources and sustained competitive advantage. J. Manag. 17, 99–120. doi: 10.1177/014920639101700108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. A., and Ensley’s, M. D. (2006). Opportunity recognition as the detection of meaningful patterns: evidence from comparisons of novice and experienced entrepreneurs. Manag. Sci. 52, 1331–1344. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1060.0538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baum, J. R., Bird, B. J., and Singh, S. (2011). The practical intelligence of entrepreneurs: antecedents and a link with new venture growth. Person. Psychol. 64, 397–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01214.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckman, C. M. (2006). The influence of founding team company affiliations on firm behavior. Acad. Manag. J. 49, 741–758. doi: 10.5465/amj.2006.22083030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergman, M. E., Drasgow, F., Donovan, M. A., Henning, J. B., and Juraska, S. E. (2006). Scoring situational judgment tests: once you get the data, your troubles begin. Int. J. Select. Asses. 14, 223–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00345.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, B. (1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: the case for intention. Acad. Manag. Rev. 13, 442–453. doi: 10.5465/amr.1988.4306970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, B. (2014). “Toward a taxonomy of entrepreneurs’ behavior,” in Handbook of entrepreneurial cognition, eds J. R. Mitchell, R. K. Mitchell, and B. Randolph-Seng (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing), 113–131. doi: 10.4337/9781781006597.00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bol, J. C., Estep, C., Moers, F., and Peecher, M. E. (2018). The role of tacit knowledge in auditor expertise and human capital development: the role of tacit knowledge. J. Account. Res. 56, 1205–1252. doi: 10.1111/1475-679x.12220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. S., and Duguid, P. (1991). Organizational learning and communities-of-practice: toward a unified view of working, learning, and innovation. Org. Sci. 2, 40–57. doi: 10.1287/orsc.2.1.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, P. T. (2014). Imprinting by design: the microfoundations of entrepreneurial adaptation. Entrepren. Theory Pract. 38, 1081–1102. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2012.00529.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardon, M. S., Gregoire, D. A., Stevens, C. E., and Patel, P. C. (2013). Measuring entrepreneurial passion: conceptual foundations and scale validation. J. Bus. Vent. 28, 373–396. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2012.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casson, M., and Wadeson, N. (2007). The discovery of opportunities: extending the economic theory of the entrepreneur. Small Bus Econ. 28, 285–300. doi: 10.1007/s11187-006-9037-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavusgil, S. T., Calantone, R. J., and Zhao, Y. (2003). Tacit knowledge transfer and firm innovation capability. Jnl Bus Indus. Mark. 18, 6–21. doi: 10.1108/08858620310458615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chell, E. (2004). “Critical incident technique,” in Essential guide to qualitative research methods in organizational research, eds C. Cassell and G. Symon (London: SAGE Publications Ltd), 45–60.

Google Scholar

Chell, E., and Baines, S. (2000). Networking, entrepreneurship and microbusiness behaviour. Entrepr. Reg. Dev. 12, 195–215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chowdhury, F., Terjesen, S., and Audretsch, D. B. (2015). Varieties of entrepreneurship: institutional drivers across entrepreneurial activity and country. Eur. J. Law Econ. 40, 121–148.

Google Scholar

Chrisman, J. J. (1999). The influence of outside-generated knowledge resources on venture creation. J. Small Bus. Manag. 37, 42–58.

Google Scholar

Cianciolo, A. T., Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., Gil, G., Drebot, M. E., and Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Practical intelligence and tacit knowledge: advancements in the measurement of developing expertise. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 16, 235–253. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2006.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, W. M., and Levinthal, D. A. (1990). Absorptive capacity: a new perspective on learning and innovation. Administr. Sci. Q. 35, 128–152. doi: 10.2307/2393553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychol. Aging 13, 45–57. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.13.1.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, S. D. N., and Brown, J. S. (1999). Bridging epistemologies: the generative dance between organizational knowledge and organizational knowing. Org. Sci. 10, 381–400. doi: 10.1287/orsc.10.4.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cope, J. (2005). Toward a dynamic learning perspective of entrepreneurship. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 29, 373–397.

Google Scholar

Cope, J., and Watts, G. (2000). Learning by doing – An exploration of experience, critical incidents and reflection in entrepreneurial learning. Internat. J. Entrepr. Behav. Res. 6, 104–124.

Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J., and Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psycholog. Bull. 52, 281–302. doi: 10.1037/h0040957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crook, T. R., Shook, C. L., Morris, M. L., and Madden, T. M. (2010). Are we there yet? An assessment of research design and construct measurement practices in entrepreneurship research. Org. Res. Meth. 13, 192–206. doi: 10.1177/1094428109334368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, Y., Roundy, P. T., Chok, J. I., Ding, F., and Byun, G. (2016). ‘Who knows what?’ in new venture teams: transactive memory systems as a micro-foundation of entrepreneurial orientation. J. Manag. Stud. 53, 1320–1347.

Google Scholar

Davidsson, P., and Honig, B. (2003). The role of social and human capital among nascent entrepreneurs. J. Bus. Vent. 18, 301–331. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9026(02)00097-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Clercq, D., Honig, B., and Martin, B. (2013). The roles of learning orientation and passion for work in the formation of entrepreneurial intention. Internat. Small Bus. J. 31, 652–676. doi: 10.1177/0266242611432360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Mol, E., Khapova, S. N., and Elfring, T. (2015). Entrepreneurial team cognition: a review. Internat. J. Manag. Rev. 17, 232–255. doi: 10.1111/ijmr.12055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, T. J., Brown, R. L., and Bamford, C. E. (1998). Differences in large and small firm responses to environmental context: strategic implications from a comparative analysis of business formations. Strat. Manag. J. 19, 709–728. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0266(199808)19:8<709::aid-smj966>3.0.co;2-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohse, D., and Walter, S. G. (2012). Knowledge context and entrepreneurial intentions among students. Small Bus Econ. 39, 877–895. doi: 10.1007/s11187-011-9324-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, L. (2001). The contingency theory of organizations. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. Am. Psychol. 41, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.41.10.1040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). Building theories from case study research. Acad. Manag. Rev. 14, 532–550. doi: 10.1258/0951484021912851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenhardt, K. M., and Graebner, M. E. (2007). Theory building from cases: opportunities and challenges. AMJ 50, 25–32. doi: 10.5465/amj.2007.24160888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, E. S., and Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 54, 5–12. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erzurumlu, S. S., Joglekar, N., Lévesque, M., and Tanrisever, F. (2019). How angel know-how shapes ownership sharing in stage-based contracts. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 43, 773–801. doi: 10.1177/1042258717744205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eurostat - OECD (2007). Manual on business demography statistics. Luxembourg: European Commission. Available online at: https://www.oecd.org/sdd/39974460.pdf (accessed date July 1, 2019)

Google Scholar

Foss, N. J., and Klein, P. G. (2012). Organizing entrepreneurial judgment: A new approach to the firm. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Gertler, M. S. (2003). Tacit knowledge and the economic geography of context, or the undefinable tacitness of being (there). J. Econ. Geogr. 3, 75–99. doi: 10.1093/jeg/3.1.75

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghio, N., Guerini, M., and Rossi-Lamastra, C. (2016). University knowledge and the creation of innovative start-ups: an analysis of the Italian case. Small Bus Econ. 47, 293–311. doi: 10.1007/s11187-016-9720-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbert, M., Ruigrok, W., and Wicki, B. (2008). What passes as a rigorous case study? Strateg. Manag. J. 29, 1465–1474. doi: 10.1002/smj.722

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, B. A., Mcdougall, P. P., and Audretsch, D. B. (2006). New venture growth: a review and extension. J. Manag. 2006, 926–950. doi: 10.1177/0149206306293860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, R. M. (1996a). Prospering in dynamically-competitive environments: Organizational capability as knowledge integration. Org. Sci. 7, 375–387. doi: 10.1287/orsc.7.4.375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, R. M. (1996b). Toward a knowledge-based theory of the firm. Strat. Manag. J. 17, 109–122. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250171110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grodal, S., Anteby, M., and Holm, A. L. (2020). Achieving rigor in qualitative analysis: the role of active categorization in theory building. Acad. Manag. Rev. 46:3.

Google Scholar

Hahs-Vaughn, D. L., and Lomax, R. G. (2020). An introduction to statistical concepts. Fourth edition. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F. Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2014). Multivariate data analysis. London: Pearson Education Limited.

Google Scholar

Haldin-Herrgard, T. (2000). Difficulties in diffusion of tacit knowledge in organizations. Jnl Intellect. Capital 1, 357–365. doi: 10.1108/14691930010359252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hambrick, D. C. (2007). Upper echelons theory: an update. Acad. Manag. Rev. 32, 334–343. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.24345254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hambrick, D. C., and Mason, P. A. (1984). Upper echelons: the organization as a reflection of its top managers. Acad. Manag. Rev. 9, 193–206. doi: 10.5465/amr.1984.4277628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hlady-Rispal, M., Fayolle, A., and Gartner, W. B. (2021). In search of creative qualitative methods to capture current entrepreneurship research challenges. J. Small Bus. Manag. 59, 887–912. doi: 10.1080/00472778.2020.1865541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoang, H., and Gimeno, J. (2010). Becoming a founder: how founder role identity affects entrepreneurial transitions and persistence in founding. J. Bus. Vent. 25, 41–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2008.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howells, J. (1996). Tacit knowledge, innovation and technology transfer. Technol. Anal. Strat. Manag. 8, 91–106.

Google Scholar

Huber, G. P. (1991). Organizational learning: the contributing processes and the literatures. Organiz. Sci. 2, 88–115. doi: 10.1287/orsc.2.1.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, S., and Luthans, F. (2006). Entrepreneurs as authentic leaders: impact on employees’ attitudes. LODJ 27, 646–666. doi: 10.1108/01437730610709273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karacic, S., Boulanger, M. T., and Glascock, M. D. (2016). Geochemical analysis of the Hittite-period pottery from Tarsus-Gözlükule, Turkey. Archaeometry 58, 23–38. doi: 10.1111/arcm.12233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klotz, A. C., Hmieleski, K. M., Bradley, B. H., and Busenitz, L. W. (2014). New venture teams: a review of the literature and roadmap for future research. J. Manag. 40, 226–255. doi: 10.1177/0149206313493325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knockaert, M., Ucbasaran, D., Wright, M., and Clarysse, B. (2011). The relationship between knowledge transfer, top management team composition, and performance: the case of science–based entrepreneurial firms. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 35, 777–803. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2010.00405.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kogut, B., and Zander, U. (1992). Knowledge of the firm, combinative capabilities, and the replication of technology. Organiz. Sci. 3, 383–397. doi: 10.1287/orsc.3.3.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kogut, B., and Zander, U. (1993). Knowledge of the firm and the evolutionary theory of the multinational corporation. J. Internat. Bus. Stud. 1993:21.

Google Scholar

Kraus, S., Breier, M., Jones, P., and Hughes, M. (2019). Individual entrepreneurial orientation and intrapreneurship in the public sector. Int. Entrep. Manag. J. 15, 1247–1268. doi: 10.1007/s11365-019-00593-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, N. (1993). The impact of prior entrepreneurial exposure on perceptions of new venture feasibility and desirability. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 18, 5–21.

Google Scholar

Krueger, N. F., Reilly, M. D., and Carsrud, A. L. (2000). Competing models of entrepreneurial intentions. J. Bus. Vent. 15, 411–432. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9026(98)00033-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Breton–Miller, I., and Miller, D. (2006). Why do some family businesses out–compete? Governance, long–term orientations, and sustainable capability. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 30, 731–746. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2006.00147.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leese, M. N., and Main, P. L. (1994). The efficient computation of unbiased mahalanobis distances and their interpretation in archaeometry. Archaeometry 36, 307–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-4754.1994.tb00972.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leiponen, A. (2006). Managing knowledge for innovation: the case of business-to-business services. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 23, 238–258.

Google Scholar

Leitch, C. M., and Volery, T. (2017). Entrepreneurial leadership: insights and directions. Internat. Small Bus. J. 35, 147–156. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199707000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A., Der Foo, M., and Chaturvedi, S. (2013). Imprinting effects of founding core teams on HR values in new ventures. Entrepre. Theory Pract. 37, 87–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2012.00532.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levie, J., and Lichtenstein, B. B. (2010). Terminal assessment of stages theory: introducing a dynamic states approach to entrepreneurship. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 34, 317–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2010.00377.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levinthal, D. A., and March, J. G. (1993). The myopia of learning. Strat. Manag. J. 14, 95–112.

Google Scholar

Li, H., Hang, Y., Shah, S. G. M., Akram, A., and Ozturk, I. (2020). Demonstrating the Impact of Cognitive CEO on Firms’ Performance and CSR Activity. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lievens, F., Peeters, H., and Schollaert, E. (2008). Situational judgment tests: a review of recent research. Person. Rev. 37, 426–441.

Google Scholar

Lincoln, Y. S., and Guba, E. G. (1986). But is it rigorous? Trustworthiness and authenticity in naturalistic evaluation. New Direct. Prog. Eval. 1986, 73–84. doi: 10.1002/ev.1427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahoney, J. T., and Pandian, J. R. (1992). The Resource-Based View Within the Conversation of Strategic Management. Strat. Manag. J. 13, 363–380. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250130505

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGrath, J. E. (1981). Dilemmatics: the study of research choices and dilemmas. Am. Behav. Sci. 25, 179–210. doi: 10.1177/000276428102500205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R. K., Busenitz, L. W., Bird, B., Marie Gaglio, C., McMullen, J. S., Morse, E. A., et al. (2007). The central question in entrepreneurial cognition research 2007. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 31, 1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2007.00161.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motowidlo, S. J., Dunnette, M. D., and Carter, G. W. (1990). An alternative selection procedure: the low-fidelity simulation. J. Appl. Psychol. 75, 640–647. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.75.6.640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motowidlo, S. J., Lievens, F., and Ghosh, K. (2018). Prosocial implicit trait policies underlie performance on different situational judgment tests with interpersonal content. Hum. Perf. 31, 238–254. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2018.1523909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murnieks, C. Y., Cardon, M. S., and Haynie, J. M. (2020). Fueling the fire: examining identity centrality, affective interpersonal commitment and gender as drivers of entrepreneurial passion. J. Bus. Vent. 35, 1–17.

Google Scholar

Naffziger, D. W., Hornsby, J. S., and Kuratko, D. F. (1994). A proposed research model of entrepreneurial motivation. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 18, 29–42.

Google Scholar

Nelson, R. R., and Winter, S. G. (1982). An evolutionary theory of economic change. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Nonaka, I. (1994). A dynamic theory of organizational knowledge creation. Org. Sci. 5, 14–37. doi: 10.1287/orsc.5.1.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osterloh, M., and Frey, B. S. (2000). Motivation, knowledge transfer, and organizational form. Org. Sci. 11, 538–550. doi: 10.1287/orsc.11.5.538.15204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overholtzer, L., Pierce, D. E., and Glascock, M. D. (2020). Aztec black-on-orange and redware pottery production from the Middle Postclassic to early Colonial period: insight from instrumental neutron activation analysis (INAA) at Xaltocan, Mexico. J. Archaeol. Sci. 34:102642. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2020.102642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C., Vertinsky, I., and Becerra, M. (2015). Transfers of tacit vs. explicit knowledge and performance in international joint ventures: the role of age. Internat. Bus. Rev. 24, 89–101. doi: 10.1016/j.ibusrev.2014.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, K., Nisbett, R. E., and Wong, N. Y. C. (1997). Validity problems comparing values across cultures and possible solutions. Psycholog. Methods 2, 329–344. doi: 10.1186/s13054-016-1208-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penny, K. I. (1996). Appropriate critical values when testing for a single multivariate outlier by using the Mahalanobis distance. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series C 45, 73–81.

Google Scholar

Peterman, N. E., and Kennedy, J. (2003). Enterprise education: influencing students’ perceptions of entrepreneurship. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 28, 129–144. doi: 10.1046/j.1540-6520.2003.00035.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanyi, M. (2009). The tacit dimension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Pratt, M. G., Kaplan, S., and Whittington, R. (2020). Editorial essay: the tumult over transparency: decoupling transparency from replication in establishing trustworthy qualitative research. Administ. Sci. Q. 65, 1–19. doi: 10.1177/0001839219887663

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pretz, J. E. (2008). Intuition versus analysis: strategy and experience in complex everyday problem solving. Memory Cogn. 36, 554–566. doi: 10.3758/mc.36.3.554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, R., and Defillippi, R. J. (1990). Causal ambiguity, barriers to imitation, and sustainable competitive advantage. Acad. Manag. Rev. 15:88. doi: 10.5465/amr.1990.4308277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuber, A. R., and Fischer, E. (1999). Understanding the consequences of founders’ experience. J. Small Bus. Manag. 37, 30–45.

Google Scholar

Ribas Fernandes, J. J. F., and Holroyd, C. B. (2017). Episodic, Semantic, Pavlovian, and Procedural Cognitive Maps. Biorvix doi: 10.1101/161141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, J., and Wiersema, M. F. (1995). A resource-based approach to the multibusiness firm: empirical analysis of portfolio interrelationships and corporate financial performance. Strat. Manag. J. 16, 277–299.

Google Scholar

Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.

Google Scholar

Schjoedt, L., Monsen, E., Pearson, A., Barnett, T., and Chrisman, J. J. (2013). New venture and family business teams: understanding team formation, composition, behaviors, and performance. Entrepre. Theory Pract. 37, 1–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2012.00549.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyb, S. K., Shepherd, D. A., and Williams, T. A. (2019). Exoskeletons, entrepreneurs, and communities: a model of co-constructing a potential opportunity. J. Bus. Vent. 34:105947. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2019.105947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shane, S. (2000). Prior knowledge and the discovery of entrepreneurial opportunities. Org. Sci. 11, 448–469. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shane, S., and Venkataraman, S. (2000). The promise of entrepreneurship as a field of research. Acad. Manag. Rev. 25:217. doi: 10.5465/amr.2000.2791611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, D. A. (2015). Party On! A call for entrepreneurship research that is more interactive, activity based, cognitively hot, compassionate, and prosocial. J. Bus. Vent. 30, 489–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2015.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shook, C. L., Priem, R. L., and McGee, J. E. (2003). Venture creation and the enterprising individual: a review and synthesis. J. Manag. 29, 379–399. doi: 10.1016/s0149-2063(03)00016-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, B. R., Matthews, C. H., and Schenkel, M. T. (2009). Differences in entrepreneurial opportunities: the role of tacitness and codification in opportunity identification. J. Small Bus. Manag. 47, 38–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-627x.2008.00261.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. G., Collins, C. J., and Clark, K. D. (2005). Existing knowledge, knowledge creation capability, and the rate of new product introduction in high-technology firms. Acad. Manag. J. 48, 346–357. doi: 10.5465/amj.2005.16928421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spender, J.-C., and Grant, R. M. (1996). Knowledge and the firm: overview. Strat. Manag. J. 17, 5–9. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250171103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R. (1986). “Memory and the brain,” in The brain, cognition, and education, eds S. L. Friedman, K. A. Klivington, and R. W. Peterson (Orlando, Fl: Academic Press Inc), 171–197.

Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., and Zola, S. M. (1998). Episodic Memory, Semantic Memory, and Amnesia. Hippocampus 8, 205–211.

Google Scholar

Stemler, S. E., Elliott, J. G., Grigorenko, E. L., and Sternberg, R. J. (2006). There’s more to teaching than instruction: seven strategies for dealing with the practical side of teaching. Educ. Stud. 32, 101–118. doi: 10.1080/03055690500416074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Managerial intelligence: why IQ isn’t enough. J. Manag. 23, 475–493. doi: 10.1016/s0149-2063(97)90038-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Successful intelligence as a basis for entrepreneurship. J. Bus. Vent. 19, 189–201. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9026(03)00006-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The rainbow project: enhancing the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical, and creative skills. Intelligence 34, 321–350. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., Forsythe, G. B., Hedlund, J., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., Horvath, J. A., et al. (2000). Practical intelligence in everyday life. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., and Okagaki, L. (1993). “Practical intelligence: The nature and role of tacit knowledge in work and at school,” in Mechanisms of Everyday Cognition, eds J. M. Puckett and H. W. Reese (Hove: Psychology Press), 205–227. doi: 10.4324/9781315789095-11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, R. W., and Abetti, P. A. (1990). Impact of entrepreneurial and management experience on early performance. J. Bus. Vent. 5, 151–162.

Google Scholar

Szulanski, G. (1996). Exploring internal stickiness: impediments to the transfer of best practice within the firm: exploring internal stickiness. Strat. Manag. J. 17, 27–43. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250171105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, H.-T., and Libby, R. (1997). Tacit managerial versus technical knowledge as determinants of audit expertise in the field. J. Account. Res. 35:97. doi: 10.2307/2491469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of the modern identity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Taylor, T. Z., Elison-Bowers, P., Werth, E., Bell, E., Carbajal, J., Lamm, K. B., et al. (2013). A police officer’s tacit knowledge inventory (POTKI): establishing construct validity and exploring applications. Police Pract. Res. 14, 478–490. doi: 10.1080/15614263.2013.802847

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teece, D. J. (2007). Explicating dynamic capabilities: the nature and microfoundations of (sustainable) enterprise performance. Strat. Manag. J. 28, 1319–1350. doi: 10.1002/smj.640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teece, D. J. (2012). Dynamic capabilities: routines versus entrepreneurial action. J. Manag. Stud. 49, 1395–1401. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2012.01080.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teece, D. J., Pisano, G., and Shuen, A. (1997). Dynamic capabilities and strategic management. Strat. Manag. J. 18, 509–533.

Google Scholar

Terjesen, S., Hessels, J., and Li, D. (2016). Comparative international entrepreneurship: a review and research agenda. J. Manag. 42, 299–344. doi: 10.1177/0149206313486259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolstoy, D. (2010). Network development and knowledge creation within the foreign market: a study of international entrepreneurial firms. Entrepr. Reg. Dev. 22, 379–402. doi: 10.1080/08985620903168273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, W. (2001). Knowledge transfer in intraorganizational networks: effects of network position and absorptive capacity on business unit innovation and performance. Acad. Manag. J. 44, 996–1004. doi: 10.5465/3069443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsoukas, H. (2003). “Do we really understand tacit knowledge?,” in Handbook of organizational learning and knowledge, eds M. Easterby-Smith and M. A. Lyles (Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell), 410–427.

Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1972). “Episodic and Semantic Memory,” in Organization of Memory, eds E. Tulving and W. Donaldson (London: Academic Press Inc), 381–403.

Google Scholar

VandeWalle, D. (1997). Development and validation of a work domain goal orientation instrument. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 57, 995–1015. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.681597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VandeWalle, D., Cron, W. L., and Slocum, J. W. (2001). The role of goal orientation following performance feedback. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 629–640. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.4.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VandeWalle, D., and Cummings, L. L. (1997). A test of the influence of goal orientation on the feedback-seeking process. J. Appl. Psychol. 82, 390–400. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., and Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential Effects of Early Hippocampal Pathology on Episodic and Semantic Memory. Science 277, 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5324.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, R. K., and Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Practical intelligence in real-world pursuits: the role of tacit knowledge. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 49, 436–458. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.2.436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, R. K., and Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Tacit knowledge in managerial success. J. Bus Psychol. 1, 301–312. doi: 10.1007/BF01018140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warhurst, C., and Thompson, P. (2006). Mapping knowledge in work: proxies or practices? Work, Employ. Soc. 20, 787–800.

Google Scholar

West, G. P., and Noel, T. W. (2009). The impact of knowledge resources on new venture performance. J. Small Bus. Manag. 47, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.gim.2021.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, G. P. I. (2007). Collective cognition: when entrepreneurial teams, not individuals, make decisions. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 31, 77–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2007.00164.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westhead, P., Ucbasaran, D., and Wright, M. (2005). Decisions, actions, and performance: Do novice, serial, and portfolio entrepreneurs differ? J. Small Bus. Manag. 43, 393–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-627x.2005.00144.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigren, C. (2007). “Assessing the quality of qualitative research in entrepreneurship,” in Handbook of qualitative research methods in entrepreneurship, eds H. Neergaard and J. Ulhøi (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing), 383–405.

Google Scholar

Wiklund, J., and Shepherd, D. (2003). Knowledge-based resources, entrepreneurial orientation, and the performance of small and medium-sized businesses. Strat. Mgmt. J. 24, 1307–1314. doi: 10.1002/smj.360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittgenstein, L. (1969). On certainty. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (2018). Case study research and applications: Design and methods, 6th Edn. Los Angeles: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Zahra, S. A., Sapienza, H. J., and Davidsson, P. (2006). Entrepreneurship and dynamic capabilities: a review, model and research agenda. J. Manag. Stud. 43, 917–955. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2006.00616.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapkau, F. B., Schwens, C., and Kabst, R. (2017). The role of prior entrepreneurial exposure in the entrepreneurial process: a review and future research implications. J. Small Bus. Manag. 55, 56–86. doi: 10.1111/jsbm.12232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Y., Li, Y., Lee, S. H., and Bo Chen, L. (2011). Entrepreneurial orientation, organizational learning, and performance: evidence from China. Entrepr. Theory Pract. 35, 293–317.

Google Scholar

Zollo, M., and Winter, S. G. (2002). Deliberate learning and the evolution of dynamic capabilities. Org. Sci. 13, 339–351. doi: 10.1287/orsc.13.3.339.2780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tacit knowledge, cognition, informal learning, entrepreneurship, qualitative research method, scenarios

Citation: Wuytens N, Schepers J, Vandekerkhof P and Voordeckers W (2022) Entrepreneurs Can Know More Than They Can Tell: Conceptualizing and Measuring Tacit Entrepreneurial Knowledge. Front. Psychol. 13:892223. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.892223

Received: 08 March 2022; Accepted: 19 April 2022;
Published: 07 June 2022.

Edited by:

Dominik E. Froehlich, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Serena Cubico, University of Verona, Italy
Augustina Asih Rumanti, Telkom University, Indonesia
Stephen Turner, University of South Florida, United States

Copyright © 2022 Wuytens, Schepers, Vandekerkhof and Voordeckers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nils Wuytens, bmlscy53dXl0ZW5zQHVoYXNzZWx0LmJl

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.