Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 02 May 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Role of Death-Anxiety-Induced Fear of COVID-19 in Compliance With and Acceptance of Government-Issued COVID-19 Regulations

  • Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany

The present research was conducted to empirically examine whether death anxiety is the fundamental fear that feeds people’s fear of COVID-19 and leads to increased behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations. Results from an online survey of 313 participants from New York City show that death anxiety was, indeed, positively associated with behavioral compliance with, but not acceptance of, COVID-19 regulations via an increased fear of COVID-19. Hence, media campaigns that are designed to increase people’s compliance with restrictive COVID-19 measures by stirring up their death anxiety are likely to meet their target, but they do not necessarily lead to increased public acceptance of the measures taken.

Introduction

Since its outbreak in late 2019, the COVID-19 pandemic has had a devastating impact on global health and the world economy, with an enormous death toll and economic losses exceeding hundreds of billions of United States Dollars (Nicola et al., 2020; Weiss et al., 2021). In attempting to restrain the spread of COVID-19, governments around the world reacted by imposing severe restrictions on their people, some of which (e.g., lockdowns and school closures) violated fundamental human rights (Quintana and Uriburu, 2020; Thomson and Ip, 2020). Supported by the press, government officials explained and defended their restrictive measures by stressing the deadly potential of COVID-19, with TV channels perpetuating footage of overcrowded emergency rooms, dead bodies, and piled up coffins (Reed, 2020). Public health campaigns were crafted to raise the salience of death, which researchers have argued may have led to an increase in people’s death anxiety (Menzies and Menzies, 2020; Pyszczynski et al., 2021). Clearly, the notion underlying these campaigns was that, by heightening the salience of personal vulnerability and death, fear of COVID-19 would increase, and people would be more likely to comply with and accept the severe and unprecedented restrictions. However, this proposition has yet to be empirically tested. Therefore, the present research was conducted to empirically test whether death anxiety is, indeed, positively associated with fear of COVID-19 and, subsequently, with behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations.

Death Anxiety and Fear of COVID-19

Death anxiety, the fear of the threat of non-being, has been proposed to be a central and universal part of human existence (Becker, 1993). Researchers have identified death anxiety as a basic and fundamental fear that lies at the root of numerous psychological conditions (e.g., hypochondriasis, panic disorder, and anxiety disorders) and have therefore suggested that it should be considered a transdiagnostic construct (Iverach et al., 2014). Accordingly, death anxiety can fuel other, more specific fears (e.g., fear of illnesses). In line with this, researchers recently proposed that death anxiety may also predict fear of COVID-19 (Menzies and Menzies, 2020).

According to Terror Management Theory (TMT), there are two different defense mechanisms against death anxiety, depending on whether death anxiety lies within or outside of a person’s conscious awareness: proximal and distal defenses (Pyszczynski et al., 2015, 2021; Menzies and Menzies, 2020). When people consciously experience death anxiety, they employ proximal defenses to alleviate the fear (Pyszczynski et al., 2015). Proximal defenses include suppressing death-related thoughts (e.g., by avoiding social media) or trying to prevent death (e.g., by staying home; Iverach et al., 2014; Menzies and Menzies, 2020). Conversely, distal defenses (e.g., enhancing one’s self-esteem or endorsing one’s worldview) are employed when people subconsciously experience death anxiety (Menzies and Menzies, 2020; Pyszczynski et al., 2021). Because COVID-19-related measures (e.g., social distancing, heightened hygiene, or avoidance of public transportation) are proximal defenses, the present research focuses on proximal defenses and consciously experienced death anxiety. We propose that people who consciously experience death anxiety will be fearful of COVID-19 and will subsequently adhere to government-imposed COVID-19 regulations.

There is evidence from recent research that fear of COVID-19 is indeed associated with increased adherence to government-imposed COVID-19-related measures. Harper et al. (2021) found that fear of COVID-19 was associated with heightened behavioral compliance with government-mandated personal restrictions, and Zettler et al. (2020) reported that heightened levels of anxiety and fear were associated with a greater level of acceptance of COVID-19-related measures.

Interestingly, most studies on adherence to COVID-19 regulations have not differentiated between compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations (e.g., Sabat et al., 2020) or have focused entirely on compliance rather than acceptance (e.g., Harper et al., 2021; Morbée et al., 2021). The study by Franzen and Wöhner (2021) is a notable exception. In a Swiss sample of young adults, the authors found that individuals living with a member of a risk group had higher compliance with — but not higher acceptance of—COVID-19 measures. The authors also reported that individuals’ perceptions of the social risk of the coronavirus led to increased acceptance of the COVID-19 measures, but their perceptions of personal risk did not.

In sum, death anxiety has been identified as a potential source of increased fear of COVID-19 (Menzies and Menzies, 2020), and fear of COVID-19 has been shown to be associated with increased compliance with and acceptance of government-imposed COVID-19 restrictions (Zettler et al., 2020; Franzen and Wöhner, 2021; Harper et al., 2021). Further, consciously experienced death anxiety is known to entail proximal defenses (Pyszczynski et al., 2015), presumably including compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 restrictions. From the above, our prediction was that death anxiety drives fear of COVID-19, which subsequently leads to increased compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 restrictions. Because no studies have examined relationships between death anxiety and compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 restrictions or identified fear of COVID-19 as a potential mediator of this relationship, we conducted this empirical study.

Hypotheses

From the above, we derived the following hypotheses1 :

H1: Death anxiety is significantly positively associated with fear of COVID-19.

H2a: Death anxiety is significantly positively associated with behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations.

H2b: Death anxiety is significantly positively associated with acceptance of COVID-19 regulations.

H3a: Fear of COVID-19 is significantly positively associated with behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations.

H3b: Fear of COVID-19 is significantly positively associated with acceptance of COVID-19 regulations.

H4a: The positive effect of death anxiety on behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations is mediated by fear of COVID-19.

H4b: The positive effect of death anxiety on acceptance of COVID-19 regulations is mediated by fear of COVID-19.

In view of Open Science recommendations, all materials concerning the study, analyses, and data are publicly available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/fvmjr/?view_only= 8c55d0ebeeca4798a8636a572e834d30. Furthermore, we preregistered our hypotheses, study design, sampling plan, variables, and analysis plan on the Open Science Framework publicly available at https://osf.io/sx5kv?view_only=7b1b3260 cffc47f5ba22329b02e9e003. There are only minor deviations from our preregistration, which we report in the following sections.

Materials and Methods

Data Collection Procedure and Participants

With an expected small indirect effect (ab = 0.04; α = 0.05, β = 0.80), our statistical power analysis (Kenny, 2017) yielded a sample size requirement of 252 participants. However, we decided to aim for a sample size of 400 participants (a) to allow for expected attrition, (b) to further increase our statistical power to detect even very small effects, and (c) because the sample size would be comparable to other recent research on fear of COVID-19 (Mertens et al., 2020).

We collected data exclusively from New York City citizens because New York City was heavily affected by the COVID-19 outbreak (Armstrong et al., 2020), which led to a number of government regulations. At the same time, this limited sampling approach allowed us to ensure comparability of participants by ruling out local differences in government regulations to prevent the spread of the virus. We recruited participants using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), which has been shown to be as reliable as traditional data collection methods for social research (Buhrmester et al., 2011). After a predetermined 20-day collection period that began on March 13, 2021, data collection was stopped to prevent uncontrolled systematic variability in light of the rapidly changing pandemic situation, including government regulations. We finally reached a sample size of 313 participants from New York City (Mage = 37.65, 52.72% women), thereby meeting the sample size requirement calculated in our power analysis. In line with our preregistration, we excluded two participants on the basis of Mahalanobis distance. Note that the results reported below remained the same even when outliers were included in the analyses.

Online Questionnaire and Materials

Data were collected with an online questionnaire on SoSci Survey. The study met relevant ethical guidelines, including adherence to the local legal requirements. After participants gave their informed consent to the terms and conditions of the study, they were presented with the questionnaires. The survey questions followed the same sequence as listed below. At the end of the survey, we asked for biographical data (age, gender, education, profession, and income).

Death Anxiety (Independent Variable)

We measured death anxiety with Templer’s Death Anxiety Scale (Templer, 1970), a “reliable and valid measure of death anxiety” (Iverach et al., 2014, p. 584) in its original 15-item version, according to Zuccala et al. (2019), “the most widely used measure” (p. 2) of death anxiety. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample statement is, “I often think about how short life really is” (α = 0.86, M = 3.27, SD = 0.68).

Fear of COVID-19 (Mediator)

We assessed fear of COVID-19 using a recently developed and validated seven-item measure, namely, the Fear of COVID-19 Scale (Ahorsu et al., 2020). Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample statement is, “I cannot sleep because I’m worrying about getting coronavirus-19” (α = 0.93, M = 2.61, SD = 1.06).

Behavioral Compliance With COVID-19 Regulations (Dependent Variable)

We measured participants’ behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations using a scale we developed in close reference to Roma et al.’s (2020) behavioral compliance scale. Six items were extracted from the ‘‘Social Distancing and Face Covering Rules’’ issued in March 2021 by the City of New York2 that were, at that time, identical to the precautions recommended by the ‘‘Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’’3 (CDC). Participants were asked, “Please indicate how often you have shown the recommended behavior during the last 3 months.” A sample item is, “I avoided public gatherings of more than 10 people.” Answers were given on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Psychometric properties of the scale were good (α = 0.86, M = 4.16, SD = 0.82).

Acceptance of COVID-19 Regulations (Dependent Variable)

We measured acceptance of COVID-19 regulations analogously to the behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations scale, but the wording was altered slightly. Participants were asked, “Now, we would like to know how appropriate you find the suggestions made by the City of New York.” A sample item is, “To avoid public gatherings of more than 10 people.” Answers were given on a 5-point Likert scale that ranged from 1 (too strict) to 5 (not strict enough). The scale was reverse-coded so that higher scores represented greater acceptance of COVID-19 regulations. Psychometric properties of the six-item scale were good (α = 0.93, M = 3.03, SD = 0.75).

Fear of Losing Loved Ones (Control Variable)

We measured fear of losing loved ones because (a) research has shown that fear of one’s own death should be distinguished from fear of losing significant others (see Cuniah et al., 2021, for a recent publication) and (b) risk for loved ones has been found to be one of the strongest predictors of COVID-19-related fears (Mertens et al., 2020). Thus, we used the eight-item subscale The Death of Others from The Collett-Lester’s Fear of Death Scale Version 3.0 (Lester, 1990; Lester and Abdel-Khalek, 2003) to control for potential influences on our focal variables. Participants were asked to indicate “how disturbed or anxious” they felt after reading statements about the deaths of others on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not) to 5 (very). A sample item is, “Losing someone close to you” (α = 0.86, M = 3.50, SD = 0.94).

Financially Affected by COVID-19 (Control Variable)

Because the financial situation of many individuals has gotten worse during the pandemic (Horowitz et al., 2021), which is likely to affect COVID-19-related perceptions and behaviors, we used a single self-designed item and asked participants to indicate “How did the pandemic affect your financial situation?” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (worsened strongly) to 5 (improved strongly) (M = 3.27, SD = 0.90). We reverse-coded this variable so that higher values indicate a greater financial impact of COVID-19.

Professionally Affected by COVID-19 (Control Variable)

Because many individuals’ job situations have gotten worse during the pandemic (Horowitz et al., 2021), which is likely to affect COVID-19-related perceptions and behaviors, we used a single self-designed item and asked participants to rate “How did the pandemic affect your job opportunities?” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (worsened strongly) to 5 (improved strongly) (M = 3.36, SD = 0.98). We reverse-coded this variable so that higher values indicate a greater financial impact of COVID-19.

Media Exposure (Control Variable)

Because media exposure related to the COVID-19 outbreak has been found to be associated with an increased fear of COVID-19 (Mertens et al., 2020), we used a single-item scale that has been applied in previous research (Mertens et al., 2020) and asked participants, “Have you looked up any extra information regarding the COVID-19 outbreak? (not taking into account coincidentally seeing/reading about it in the news)” using a Yes or No answer format (M = 0.71, SD = 0.45).

Political Orientation (Control Variable)

Because conservatism has been shown to be associated with less perceived personal vulnerability to COVID-19 and less perceived severeness of the pandemic (Calvillo et al., 2020), we assessed political orientation using the scale by Kanai et al. (2011)4. Participants were asked to indicate their political orientation on a single-item scale with the levels: very conservative (1), conservative (2), middle-of-the-road (3), liberal (4), and very liberal (5) (α = 0.93, M = 3.36, SD = 1.07).

Relationship to Conspiracy Theories (Control Variable)

Because COVID-19-related conspiracy theories were associated with resistance to preventive behaviors (Romer and Jamieson, 2020), we used the single-item scale by Lantian et al. (2016) to assess participants’ relationship to conspiracy theories. Participants were asked to indicate whether the item, “I think that the official version of the events given by the authorities very often hides the truth” was true or false on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely false) to 9 (completely true), M = 5.47, SD = 2.00.

General Health (Control Variable)

Because bad general health is related to higher mortality from COVID-19 (e.g., Zhou et al., 2020), high-risk individuals may perceive a greater personal threat in the COVID-19 pandemic (Mertens et al., 2020). Thus, we measured general health by using a single-item scale that has been applied in previous research (Mertens et al., 2020). Participants were asked to answer the item “Overall, I would rate my general health as” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (extremely bad) to 5 (extremely good), M = 3.89, SD = 0.82.

Perceived Disease Vulnerability (Control Variable)

We measured perceived disease vulnerability for two reasons: First, because greater perceived vulnerability to disease was found to be related to a greater belief that public health measures protect the population (De Coninck et al., 2020), and second, and more importantly, because heightened perceived disease vulnerability has been associated with increased virus anxiety (Jungmann and Witthöft, 2020). We used the 15-item Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Scale (Duncan et al., 2009). Participants indicated the extent to which they believed they were vulnerable to diseases on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A sample item is, “In general, I am very susceptible to colds, flu, and other infectious diseases” (α = 0.72, M = 4.38, SD = 0.80).

Statistical Analyses

In line with and Preacher and Hayes (2004, 2008) we conducted two mediation analyses to test our main mediation hypotheses that fear of COVID-19 mediates the effect of death anxiety on behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations. For the analyses, we entered death anxiety as the predictor, fear of COVID-19 as the mediator, and behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations as outcome variables. Additionally, we added the variables that were specified as control variables to our mediational models as covariates to statistically control for their effects on the dependent variables. We computed 95% confidence intervals from 10,000 bootstrapping resamples for each indirect effect to test for whether the indirect effects of the focal predictor on the outcome variables through the mediator were significantly different from zero (Shrout and Bolger, 2002). We used the IBM SPSS (Version 28) PROCESS macro (Model 4; Hayes, 2018) for all the analyses. Using the same statistical procedure, we also tested all of our hypotheses without the control variables.

Additionally, we estimated equivalent models to test the alternative possibility that death anxiety mediates the effect of fear of COVID-19 on behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations. In these equivalent models, we reversed the predictor and the mediators, so that fear of COVID-19 became the predictor and death anxiety became the mediator.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables can be found in Table 1. We found that only 2% of our sample reported intense death anxiety (i.e., mean plus one standard deviation). Further, we found that women reported more death anxiety than men (correlation between death anxiety and gender; r = -0.20, p < 0.01). We also found that age, level of education, and income were not significantly correlated with death anxiety (r = −0.06, r = −0.05, and r = −0.09, respectively, all ps > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations between all study variables.

Additionally, we examined the zero-order correlations between predictor, mediator, and outcome variables (see Table 1). We found a significant correlation between death anxiety and fear of COVID-19 (r = 0.55, p < 0.01). We also found that death anxiety and fear of COVID-19 were both significantly correlated with behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations (r = 0.23 and r = 0.17, respectively, both ps < 0.01) and with acceptance of COVID-19 regulations (r = 0.23 and r = 0.30, respectively, both ps < 0.01).

As shown in Table 1, we found that the extents to which individuals were financially and professionally affected by COVID-19 were significantly and positively associated with behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations (financial: r = 0.23, p < 0.01; professional: r = 0.27, p < 0.01) and with acceptance of COVID-19 regulations (financial: r = 0.24 p < 0.01; professional: r = 0.29 p < 0.01).

Main Hypothesis Testing

When statistically controlling for our covariates, we found a significant relationship between death anxiety and fear of COVID-19 [b = 0.77, SE = 0.08, t(311) = 9.14, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.47]. Additionally, we found the expected significant association between fear of COVID-19 and behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations [b = 0.24, SE = 0.05, t(311) = 4.74, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.32], but not with acceptance of COVID-19 regulations [b = 0.06, SE = 0.05, t(311) = 1.34, p > 0.05; R2 = 0.27]. Further, albeit death anxiety was not significantly associated with behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations [b = 0.06, SE = 0.08, t(311) = 0.84, p > 0.05; R2 = 0.26], the predicted indirect effect of death anxiety on behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations through fear of COVID-19 was found to be significant (ab = 0.19, SE = 0.05, and 95% CI [0.10, 0.29]). Figure 1 illustrates the results. However, death anxiety was neither directly [b = 0.08, SE = 0.07, t(311) = 1.17, p > 0.05; R2 = 0.27] nor indirectly (via fear of COVID-19) related to acceptance of COVID-19 regulations (ab = 0.05, SE = 0.05, and 95% CI [−0.04, 0.15]). Additionally, we tested our hypotheses without controlling for the covariates, and found that all aforementioned study results remained largely intact.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediation model for the influence of death anxiety on compliance with COVID-19 regulations through fear of COVID-19. N = 313. The value in parentheses represents the direct effect of death anxiety on compliance with COVID-19 regulations without controlling for fear of COVID-19. **p < 0.01.

We also estimated equivalent models to test the alternative possibility that death anxiety mediated the effects of fear of COVID-19 on behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations, respectively. Results showed that although fear of COVID-19 was significantly associated with death anxiety [b = 0.29, SE = 0.03, t(311) = 9.14, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.53], death anxiety did not significantly predict either behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations [b = 0.12, SE = 0.08, t(311) = −1.45, p > 0.05; R2 = 0.32] or acceptance of COVID-19 regulations [b = 0.03, SE = 0.08, t(311) = 0.41, p > 0.05; R2 = 0.27]. Consequently, the indirect effects of fear of COVID-19 on behavioral compliance with or acceptance of COVID-19 regulations through death anxiety were non-significant (ab = −0.03, SE = 0.03, and 95% CI [−0.09, 0.02]; ab = 0.01, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [−0.03, 0.05], respectively).

Discussion

The results of this study show that only 2% of the sample showed intense death anxiety which is in line with the literature (Agras et al., 1969). We also found that, in general, the demographic correlates of death anxiety were similar to findings reported in the death anxiety literature (Kastenbaum, 2000; Furer et al., 2007). The examination of the relationships of the covariates with the outcome variables revealed that the extents to which individuals were financially and professionally affected by COVID-19 were positively associated with their behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations, findings that support the previous literature (e.g., Han et al., 2021).

With respect to our hypotheses, the results show that death anxiety was significantly associated with fear of COVID-19, thus corroborating Hypothesis 1. In addition, with respect to compliance with COVID-19 regulations, the results were largely in line with predictions. Specifically, fear of COVID-19 was significantly associated with compliance with COVID-19 regulations, corroborating Hypothesis 3a. Further, and most importantly, even though we did not find the expected direct link between death anxiety and compliance with COVID-19 regulations, as predicted in Hypothesis 2a, we did find the expected indirect effect of death anxiety on compliance with COVID-19 regulations via fear of COVID-19, corroborating Hypothesis 4a, the mediator hypothesis. Conversely, with respect to acceptance of COVID-19 regulations, the results were not in line with predictions. Specifically, we did not find the expected positive relationships between death anxiety or fear of COVID-19 and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations (Hypotheses 2a and 3a). Consequently, there was no significant indirect effect of death anxiety on acceptance of COVID-19 regulations via fear of COVID-19, and, therefore, no support for Hypothesis 4b.

In sum, this study corroborates the notion that death anxiety is the fundamental fear (Iverach et al., 2014) that underlies the fear of COVID-19, in line with psychological researchers’ speculations (Menzies and Menzies, 2020). In turn, fear of COVID-19 drives behavioral compliance with government regulations to avert the spread of COVID-19. The results were obtained after statistically controlling for several prominent covariates. Notably, controlling for the influences of perceived disease vulnerability (Jungmann and Witthöft, 2020) and fear of losing loved ones (Mertens et al., 2020; Cuniah et al., 2021) allowed us to examine and highlight the unique effects of death anxiety on our dependent variables. Nevertheless, it is important to note that all study results remained intact when the covariates were not statistically controlled for. Further, our results did not speak in favor of the alternative possibility that death anxiety mediates the effect of fear of COVID-19 on behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations.

Without overinterpreting our inability to reject the null hypothesis, our findings seem to indicate that behavioral compliance with and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations need to be differentiated. In fear of COVID-19, people may behaviorally comply with restrictive measures imposed by their governments, but they do not necessarily accept them. Hence, media campaigns that are designed to increase people’s compliance with restrictive COVID-19 measures by stirring up their death anxiety (Mertens et al., 2020; Pyszczynski et al., 2021) are likely to meet their target, but they will not necessarily lead to increased public acceptance of the measures taken. In addition, researchers have cautioned that social media exposure that leads to persistent salience of the threat of COVID-19 to mortality may be associated with poorer mental health (Iverach et al., 2014; Mertens et al., 2020; Pyszczynski et al., 2021).

Clearly, this study is not without limitations. First, it is a cross-sectional study. Therefore, it is not possible to infer causality from the findings. Indeed, an alternative view on the underlying causality could be that footage portraying dead victims from COVID-19 may have directly triggered fear of COVID-19 as an antecedent of subsequently increased death anxiety. To us, the alternative causality chain seems rather unlikely. First, our empirical findings indicate that there was no significant indirect path from fear of COVID-19 to behavioral compliance via death anxiety. Second, and more importantly, death anxiety theorists have long held that death anxiety should be conceptualized as a fundamental and integral part of human existence (Becker, 1993) and that it can be the underlying cause of more specific fears of certain illnesses (Iverach et al., 2014), including fear of COVID-19 (Menzies and Menzies, 2020). In and of itself, a new virus is not necessarily threatening, but footage portraying death as the paradigmatic example of consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic may trigger death anxiety and subsequently spill over to instigate fear of COVID-19. But clearly, additional laboratory studies that manipulate death anxiety and subsequently assess behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations are essential in the future. Further, the survey was conducted online with participants from New York City so that results may have been affected by some respondent bias. Finally, although many covariates were controlled for, the possibility of unmeasured factors causing some residual confounding cannot be excluded.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://osf.io/fvmjr/?view_only=8c55d0ebeeca4798a8636a572e834d30.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the Local Legislation and Institutional Requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CB performed the data collection and analysis. HK, CB, and MJ wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the study conception, design, and material preparation, commented on previous versions of the manuscript, and read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ These hypotheses differ slightly from the preregistered hypotheses. Specifically, our preregistration contained only three hypotheses. Each of these comprised two separable dependent variables: behavioral compliance with COVID-19 regulations and acceptance of COVID-19 regulations. For this article, however, we decided it would be clearer if we formulated these three hypotheses for each of the dependent variables separately. Also, the preregistration did not specify H1, the hypothesis that death anxiety is significantly associated with fear of COVID-19, because this prediction was included in the mediation hypotheses (H4a and H4b). However, for this article, we decided to be more specific and spell out H1. Further, in our preregistration, we used “fear of death” instead of “death anxiety” for our hypotheses. In fact, in our preregistration, we employed “death anxiety” and “fear of death” interchangeably, as other researchers on death anxiety have done (e.g., Menzies and Menzies, 2020). For this article, however, we decided to use “death anxiety” throughout to be consistent. Also, we changed the labeling of “COVID-19 restrictions” to “COVID-19 regulations” because this term seems more neutral. Lastly, we added the directions of the hypothesized associations. We must clarify that we did not add or delete any content from the preregistered hypotheses, so the new hypotheses are essentially identical to the preregistered ones.
  2. ^ Retrieved from: https://portal.311.nyc.gov/article/?kanumber=KA-03297.
  3. ^ Retrieved from: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/prevention.html.
  4. ^ Note that our preregistration included three scales for political orientation, but we used only the one specified above in our analyses. We did so because the political orientation scale specified above had better psychometric properties than the others, and all study results remained basically the same with and without the other scales included in the analyses.

References

Agras, S., Sylvester, D., and Oliveau, D. (1969). The epidemiology of common fears and phobia. Compr. Psychiatry 10, 151–156. doi: 10.1016/0010440X(69)90022-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahorsu, D. K., Lin, C.-Y., Imani, V., Saffari, M., Griffiths, M. D., and Pakpour, A. H. (2020). The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, D., Goldman, H., and Clukey, K. (2020). Why New York Suffered When Other Cities Were Spared by Covid-19. Bloomberg: Business Prognosis. Available online at: https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2020-05-28/why-was-new-york-hit-so-badly-with-covid-19 (accessed September 25, 2020).

Google Scholar

Becker, E. (1993). The Denial of Death. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., and Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvillo, D. P., Ross, B. J., Garcia, R. J., Smelter, T. J., and Rutchick, A. M. (2020). Political ideology predicts perceptions of the threat of COVID-19 (and susceptibility to fake news about it). Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 11, 1119–1128. doi: 10.1177/1948550620940539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuniah, M., Bréchon, G., and Bailly, N. (2021). Validation of the revised Collett–Lester fear of death scale in a French population. Front. Psychol. 12:736171. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Coninck, D., d’Haenens, L., and Matthijs, K. (2020). Perceived vulnerability to disease and attitudes towards public health measures: COVID-19 in Flanders, Belgium. Pers. Individ. Differ. 166:110220. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., and Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 541–546. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzen, A., and Wöhner, F. (2021). Coronavirus risk perception and compliance with social distancing measures in a sample of young adults: evidence from Switzerland. PLoS One 16:e0247447. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furer, P., Walker, J. R., and Stein, M. B. (2007). Treating Health Anxiety and Fear of Death: A Practitioner’s Guide. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Han, Q., Zheng, B., Cristea, M., Agostini, M., Bélanger, J. J., Gützkow, B., et al. (2021). Trust in government regarding COVID-19 and its associations with preventive health behaviour and prosocial behaviour during the pandemic: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, C. A., Satchell, L. P., Fido, D., and Latzman, R. D. (2021). Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 19, 1875–1888. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00281-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Horowitz, J. M., Brown, A., and Minkin, R. (2021). A Year into the Pandemic, Long-term Financial Impact Weighs Heavily on Many Americans. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.

Google Scholar

Iverach, L., Menzies, R. G., and Menzies, R. E. (2014). Death anxiety and its role in psychopathology: reviewing the status of a transdiagnostic construct. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34, 580–593. doi: 10.1016/J.CPR.2014.09.0022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jungmann, S. M., and Witthöft, M. (2020). Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J. Anxiety Disord. 73:102239. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Feilden, T., Firth, C., and Rees, G. (2011). Political orientations are correlated with brain structure in young adults. Curr. Biol. 21, 677–680. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastenbaum, R. (2000). The Psychology of Death, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A. (2017). MedPower: An Interactive Tool for the Estimation of Power in Tests of Mediation [Computer software]. Available online at: https://davidakenny.shinyapps.io/MedPower/ (accessed February 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., and Douglas, K. M. (2016). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: validation of a French and English single-item scale. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 29, 1–14. doi: 10.5334/irsp.8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lester, D. (1990). The Collett-Lester fear of death scale: the original version and a revision. Death Stud. 14, 451–468. doi: 10.1080/07481189008252385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lester, D., and Abdel-Khalek, A. (2003). The Collett-Lester fear of death scale: a correction. Death Stud. 27, 81–85. doi: 10.1080/07481180302873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzies, R. E., and Menzies, R. G. (2020). Death anxiety in the time of COVID-19: theoretical explanations and clinical implications. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 13:e19. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X20000215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mertens, G., Gerritsen, L., Duijndam, S., Salemink, E., and Engelhard, I. M. (2020). Fear of the coronavirus (COVID-19): predictors in an online study conducted in March 2020. J. Anxiety Disord. 74:102258. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morbée, S., Vermote, B., Waterschoot, J., Dieleman, L., Soenens, B., Van den Bergh, O., et al. (2021). Adherence to COVID-19 measures: the critical role of autonomous motivation on a short- and long-term basis. Motiv. Sci. 7, 487–496. doi: 10.1037/mot0000250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicola, M., Alsafi, Z., Sohrabi, C., Kerwan, A., Al-Jabir, A., Iosifidis, C., et al. (2020). The socio-economic implications of the coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19): a review. Int. J. Surg. 78, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2020.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyszczynski, T., Lockett, M., Greenberg, J., and Solomon, S. (2021). Terror management theory and the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Humanist. Psychol. 61, 173–189. doi: 10.1177/0022167820959488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., and Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty years of terror management theory: from genesis to revelation. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 52, 1–70. doi: 10.1016/BS.AESP.2015.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quintana, F., and Uriburu, J. (2020). Modest international law: COVID-19, international legal responses, and depoliticization. Am. J. Int. Law 114, 687–697. doi: 10.1017/ajil.2020.65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. (2020). Covid Overload: U.S. Hospitals are Running Out of Beds for Patients. The New York Times. Available online at: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/11/27/health/covid-hospitals-overload.html (accessed November 27, 2020).

Google Scholar

Roma, P., Monaro, M., Muzi, L., Colasanti, M., Ricci, E., Biondi, S., et al. (2020). How to improve compliance with protective health measures during the COVID-19 outbreak: testing a moderated mediation model and machine learning algorithms. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:7252. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17197252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romer, D., and Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Soc. Sci. Med. 263:113356. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabat, I., Elsem Varghese, N., Neuman-Böhme, S., Pita Barros, P., Brouwer, W., van Exel, J., et al. (2020). Countering COVID-19: a European survey on acceptability of and commitment to preventive measures. Eur. J. Public Health 30(Suppl. 5). doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckaa166.619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037//1082-989x.7.4.422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a death anxiety scale. J. Gen. Psychol. 82, 165–177. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1970.9920634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, S., and Ip, E. C. (2020). COVID-19 emergency measures and the impending authoritarian pandemic. J. Law Biosci. 7, 1–33. doi: 10.1093/jlb/lsaa064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, M., Schwarzenberg, A., Nelson, R., Sutter, K. M., and Sutherland, M. D. (2021). Global Economic Effects of COVID-19 (Report No. R46270). Congressional Research Service. Available online at: https://sgp.fas.org/crs/row/R46270.pdf (accessed November 10, 2021).

Google Scholar

Zettler, I., Schild, C., Lillehot, L., and Böhm, R. (2020). Individual differences in accepting personal restrictions to fight the COVID-19 pandemic: results from a Danish adult sample. PsyArXiv [Preprints]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/pkm2a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Yu, T., Du, R., Fan, G., Liu, Y., Xiang, J., et al. (2020). Clinical course and risk factors for mortality of adult inpatients with COVID-19 in Wuhan, China: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet 395, 1054–1062. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30566-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuccala, M., Menzies, R. E., Hunt, C. J., and Abbott, M. J. (2019). A systematic review of the psychometric properties of death anxiety self-report measures. Death Stud. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2019.1699203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: death anxiety, fear of COVID-19, terror management theory, compliance with COVID-19 regulations, acceptance of COVID-19 regulations

Citation: Kehr HM, Bakaç C, Jais M, Brunner D, Voigt J and Holzemer L (2022) The Role of Death-Anxiety-Induced Fear of COVID-19 in Compliance With and Acceptance of Government-Issued COVID-19 Regulations. Front. Psychol. 13:881603. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.881603

Received: 22 February 2022; Accepted: 08 April 2022;
Published: 02 May 2022.

Edited by:

Kwok Kit Tong, University of Macau, Macao SAR, China

Reviewed by:

Ginés Navarro-Carrillo, University of Jaén, Spain
Hamid Sharif Nia, Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2022 Kehr, Bakaç, Jais, Brunner, Voigt and Holzemer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hugo M. Kehr, a2VockB0dW0uZGU=

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.