Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 04 April 2022
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Safety Learning in Anxiety, Pavlovian Conditioned Inhibition and COVID Concerns

Meghan D. Thurston,
Meghan D. Thurston1,2*Helen J. Cassaday
Helen J. Cassaday1*
  • 1School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Clinical Psychology, The Royal Derby Hospital, Derby, United Kingdom

Experimental studies of fear conditioning have identified the effectiveness of safety signals in inhibiting fear and maintaining fear-motivated behaviors. In fear conditioning procedures, the presence of safety signals means that the otherwise expected feared outcome will not now occur. Differences in the inhibitory learning processes needed to learn safety are being identified in various psychological and psychiatric conditions. However, despite early theoretical interest, the role of conditioned inhibitors as safety signals in anxiety has been under-investigated to date, in part because of the stringent test procedures required to confirm the demonstration of conditioned inhibition as such. Nonetheless, the theoretical implications of an inhibitory learning perspective continue to influence clinical practice. Moreover, our understanding of safety signals is of additional importance in the context of the increased health anxiety and safety behaviors generated by the COVID-19 pandemic.

Introduction

Fear and anxiety are normal, albeit subjectively negative emotions and motivations, that arise in response to perceived threat, and which are typically experienced under particular circumstances. Fear has been distinguished from anxiety as more phasic (time-limited) state, whereas the term anxiety is used to refer to a tonic state of readiness (Adolphs, 2013). Like fear, anxiety can be restricted to specific situations in state anxiety, but high trait anxiety reflects a more general tendency or predisposition to perceive a wider range of situations as threatening. In either case, adrenalin is released to help the individual prepare with the fight-flight response, which is reflexive and hardwired in phylogenetically old areas of the midbrain, and includes for example increased heart rate in readiness for action.

This perspective article first outlines the theoretical framework provided by evolutionary perspectives on anxiety. We then focus on recent evidence confirming the importance of safety learning for cognitive behavioral approaches in cases of anxiety. Third, we consider the theoretical context provided by animal learning studies. In conclusion, we consider the implications of our understanding of safety learning for the mental health impact of the developing (and sometimes rapidly changing) situation presented by the COVID-19 pandemic.

Evolutionary Perspectives on Anxiety

In general, we can be slow to learn to fear new objective threats such as those arising on our roads or the health effects of cigarette smoking and excess alcohol consumption. More common causes of fear and anxiety make better sense from a phylogenetic rather than a learning perspective. However, the rapid spread of COVID-19 infections should have triggered our phylogenetic predispositions. Fear of infection is evolutionarily prepotent and prepared, hence also automatic and resistant to cognitive control (Öhman and Mineka, 2001). Moreover, adaptive avoidance of illness and death produced by infectious disease, mediated by feelings of disgust as well as fear, can be seen to provide us with a proactive behavioral immune system, complementary to the reactive defences provided by the physiological immune system (Troisi, 2020).

Within an evolutionary framework, the fight-flight response has been suggested to be a key component of a survival optimization system, involving a cascade of cognitive and affective responses to threats of increasing magnitude (Mobbs et al., 2015). Without doubt, fear can be adaptive to the extent the resulting anticipatory responses are proportionate to the level of environmental threat. However, we need to understand the functional role of fear and anxiety in relation to the environmental context (Adolphs, 2013). Actions that lead to the removal of valid threats enhance our survival and are reinforced, as well as selected for evolutionarily. Indeed taking the evolutionary perspective has already underscored the point that too little anxiety accompanied by the loss of normal defensive responses would also be harmful, amounting to hypophobic disorder (Marks and Nesse, 1994).

Conversely, it is well-recognized that excessive anxiety impacts wellbeing. For example, adrenalin causes a range of physiological changes which typically include sweating, heavy breathing and shaking as well as increased heart rate. Moreover, for some individuals these physical changes are over-interpreted, resulting in further increased anxiety, which is experienced as aversive and thus leads to the development of avoidance behaviors. To the extent these behaviors are disproportionate to the objective level of threat, and the underlying fear is irrational and excessive, there is the risk of anxiety disorders and maladaptive avoidance behaviors, undermining our health and well-being. From a clinical perspective, how should we in practice judge when levels of anxiety and the resulting anticipatory avoidance responses are proportionate to the level of environmental threat and hence adaptive? For example, in the circumstances of the COVID-19 pandemic, where the threat of serious illness is real, the failure to adopt appropriate safety behaviors can be maladaptive, potentially endangering health and even survival.

The theoretical context to the study of fear conditioning and safety learning is key to the nature of the inhibitory processes in play and now highly topical in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Fear of infection with COVID-19 has understandably greatly increased health anxiety and safety behaviors, in some cases perhaps disproportionately. On the other hand, individuals failing to adopt appropriate safety behaviors may have put themselves and their close contacts at risk. Pre-pandemic approaches to managing fear, anxiety and avoidance behaviors, presupposed that the wider environment was objectively safe. Now our presumptions to the effect that such anxiety-related behaviors should be eliminated using cognitive-behavioral therapy, must be constantly re-evaluated in relation to the objective health risks posed by COVID-19, in different phases of the pandemic, and in relation to the financial and social costs of COVID-19 safety behaviors for other aspects of our well-being.

Safety Learning in Anxiety

Although there are effective treatments, the underlying mechanisms of the various anxiety disorders are still not fully understood and aetiological models are multi-factorial. Such models incorporate specific environmental triggers, ranging from discrete cues such as dirt, to the diffuse range of stimuli engendered by social activities (Ehlers and Clark, 2000; Rachman, 2012). Here, we focus on trigger cues and the counter-cues provided through safety learning.

Controlled animal studies confirm that safety signals moderate stress reactivity, buffering the effects of uncontrollable stressors (Christianson et al., 2012). Patients with anxiety disorders report awareness of a variety of safety signals, from the presence of a trusted individual, to the availability of their phone and medications (Craske et al., 2014). Health-related specific phobias are also characterized by safety behaviors such as apparently excessive medical check-ups, body scanning, excessive cleaning or other hygiene-related safety behaviors such as the use of sanitized wipes (Rachman, 2012). Yet paradoxically deficient safety discrimination has been reported to characterise individuals high in trait anxiety (Gadenzam et al., 2013; Haaker et al., 2015) and some patients with anxiety disorders (Duits et al., 2021).

The evidence-based recommended choice of treatment for anxiety disorders is cognitive behavioral therapy. This approach focuses on breaking identified associations between maladaptive cognitions and unwanted behaviors. Notably patients may be specifically encouraged to drop safety behaviors. For example, a driver with who after an accident excessively checked his mirrors would be encouraged to return to his pre-accident driving pattern (Wells et al., 2016). Clinically there is some mixed evidence as to the role of safety signals in the progress of exposure treatments, but the gradual removal of safety signals is the generally favored approach. Removal may be combined with strategies to promote the recognition and consolidation of the non-occurrence of the expected aversive event, for example using discussions with the therapist to encourage reflection on the absence of the feared outcome (Craske et al., 2014).

Pavlovian Conditioned Inhibition

The establishment of safety cues provides an example of Pavlovian inhibitory learning (Figure 1). In order to demonstrate that conditioned inhibition has occurred and that the cue presumed inhibitory is a true inhibitor, responding to this unreinforced CS- must be clearly distinguishable from that elicited by the equivalent stimulus in the reinforced CS+ role. Summation and retardation tests have been considered definitive. The summation test involves taking a CS+, pre-trained but not previously presented in conjunction with any CS-, and examining the reduction in excitatory responding seen when this CS+ is now paired together with an established CS- (pre-trained in conjunction with a different CS+). A true inhibitor should reduce the responding which would otherwise be elicited by any subsequent CS+ for the same unconditioned stimulus (US) with which it is subsequently paired. The retardation test similarly relies on the notion that a CS- develops negative associative strength. Retardation is assessed when the previously trained inhibitor (CS-) is now paired with a US. Compared with the rate of acquisition seen with a neutral CS, conversion to a CS+ is slowed or retarded for a CS-.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Positive prediction error is seen when the probability (p) of the unconditioned stimulus (US) is increased on presentation of the CS+ and generates excitatory conditioning. Negative prediction error is seen when the probability of the US is decreased on presentation of the CS- and generates inhibitory conditioning. When p (US/CS) = p (US/no CS) along the diagonal trend line there can be no new learning. In the example shown, soil provides excitatory stimuli that generate fear of contamination. However, the stimuli provided by exposure to soil could become inhibitory (safety) signals if the probability of contamination were perceived as higher in contexts where soil-related cues are absent (in which case gardening might feel like a safer exercise than swimming at the public baths). Positive and negative prediction error are depicted as Pavlovian phenomena arising from specified and predictable relationships between (CS and US) environmental events. In addition, stimuli generated by instrumental responses (both external and internal to the agent) can have Pavlovian properties, for example successful avoidance responses generate inhibitory (safety) signals.

The two test method to confirm conditioned inhibition (by both summation and retardation tests) has been widely adopted in animal studies, but has been harder to implement in human studies. In human studies, there have been some reports of conditioned inhibition confirmed by summation test (He et al., 2013; Heym et al., 2014; Thurston and Cassaday, 2015) but these have been relatively few (Sosa and dos Santos, 2019; Sosa and Ramίrez, 2019). Indeed, it has been argued that studies of feature negative inhibitory discrimination learning of the kind typically used to successfully demonstrate conditioned inhibition, but which do not include (or do not subsequently pass) the formal tests to confirm conditioned inhibition should also be considered (Sosa and dos Santos, 2019; Sosa and Ramίrez, 2019). Feature negative discriminations of the form A+/AX- introduce the conflict engendered by responding or refraining to respond, depending on the presence or absence of the inhibitory cue (X), and on this basis can be considered a good proxy for conditioned inhibition.

Translational studies of inhibitory learning have provided new insights into improving the efficacy of exposure-based interventions, particularly those which explicitly include response prevention. Laboratory studies of fear extinction suggest that the associations are not eliminated during such treatments but rather exposure is effective because of the learning of new competing associations. Where non-threat is signalled these associations are inhibitory (Jacoby and Abramowitz, 2016). Inhibitory learning approaches to exposure therapy assume that the originally aversive CS enters into competing associations, now with the absence of the feared unconditioned stimulus (US) through extinction learning (Bouton et al., 2001; Craske et al., 2014). This line of thinking generates the perhaps paradoxical prediction that exposure therapy should be more effective when multiple fear-provoking cues are combined to increase the discrepancy when the predicted outcome does not in fact occur (Craske et al., 2014; Jacoby and Abramowitz, 2016).

Instrumental Avoidance Learning

Beyond their role in the narratives of the mental landscape, from awareness of contingencies and the accompanying feelings, simple associations drive actions. The focus of this perspective has been on Pavlovian (respondent) rather than instrumental (operant) conditioning, and Pavlovian studies of conditioned inhibition have not been designed with an instrumental avoidance response in mind. However, avoidance learning has been extensively studied in animal experiments, usually conducted using laboratory rats, which when given a signal of foot shock rapidly learn to make a response to avoid experiencing any further shock. The two-process theory of avoidance proposed that anticipatory anxiety is initially learned through Pavlovian conditioning of the signal for foot shock. Through the second process, instrumental learning, negatively reinforced avoidance responses are produced in response to the anxiety signal (Mowrer, 1956). This theory was later developed to include a key role for the safety signals generated by the successful execution of an avoidance response, through consideration of the likely role of inhibitory learning (Gray, 1975). There is experimental evidence to show how a safety signal may protect the classically conditioned fear response from extinction and thus account for the persistence of the avoidance behavior (Soltysik et al., 1983). Moreover, safety signals have been shown to be emotionally “relieving” in such studies: rats showed a strong correlation between the rate of deep breaths (“sighs of relief”) and the presentation of the signal for the non-occurrence of the otherwise expected shock (Soltysik and Jelen, 2005). Similarly, in a free-operant lever press avoidance procedure, rats preferentially responded on the lever that produced an explicit safety signal (in addition to the omission of the footshock) as feedback (Fernando et al., 2014). Thus, in addition to the basic behavioral responses, effective avoidance learning can include further actions and thoughts to ease anxiety such that the individual can remain in and cope with the situation. In other words, successful avoidance generates safety signals which inhibit anxiety.

Simple associations drive actions and both Pavlovian (respondent) and instrumental (operant) learning mechanisms should be considered (Rajaraman et al., 2022). The link between fear-related associations and actions has been further explored in studies of Pavlovian to instrumental transfer. This term refers to the experimental procedure and underling phenomenon whereby a Pavlovian CS influences instrumental actions by increasing or decreasing motivational salience (Cartoni et al., 2013). In the example shown in Figure 2, the stimuli provided by exposure to stimuli such as those produced by exposure to soil result in an outcome representation of contamination which may in turn result in the avoidance of activities likely to increase the risk of contamination; however, the availability of an antibacterial hand gel in the pocket can effectively inhibit the outcome representation, reduce the fear of contamination and suppress the associated avoidance responses.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. After Pavlovian conditioning the excitatory stimulus elicits not only a conditioned emotional response but also an outcome representation. In turn, through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer, the rate of an ongoing instrumental response can be moderated. If conditioned inhibitors are assumed to act by depressing the outcome representation this can explain both their effects in summation and retardation tests as well as their capacity to reduce Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. In the example shown, the excitatory stimulus provided by exposure to soil results in an outcome representation of contamination which in turn increases avoidance of outdoor activities; however, antibacterial hand gel cues (e.g., the feel of the container in the pocket) can effectively inhibit the outcome representation, reducing both the fear of contamination and the associated avoidance responses. Thus, in addition to the capacity to generate inhibitory (safety) signals, instrumental avoidance responses are bi-directionally controlled by Pavlovian excitors and inhibitors.

Avoidance behaviors are known to be very persistent and are negatively reinforced through the decrease in anxiety and relief from perceived threat. Neutral stimuli that accompany successful avoidance responses are in effect trained as conditioned inhibitors. Similarly, feedback from the successful execution of an avoidance response generates stimuli which reliably predict US omission. Thus theoretically, avoidance can also be secondarily reinforced by the safety signals and hence fear reduction generated by the instrumental response (Cook et al., 1987; Dinsmoor, 2001). Thus, when avoidance responses are possible, more complex Pavlovian-instrumental interactions also emerge. In addition to the capacity to generate inhibitory (safety) signals, instrumental avoidance responses are bi-directionally controlled by Pavlovian excitors and inhibitors (Figure 2).

When the need to generate safety signals, through safety behaviors, starts to impinge on normal daily routines this can suggest the presence of an anxiety disorder: the anxiety disorders are as much a group of behavioral disorders characterized by unwanted actions as cognitive disorders characterized by unwanted thoughts. Indeed, obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) has been suggested to arise from excessive stimulus–response habit formation resulting in enhanced avoidance habits (Gillan et al., 2014). Impulsivity in the wider sense also features inhibition deficits and conditioned inhibition has started to be used to characterize components of these deficits (Sosa and dos Santos, 2019).

COVID Concerns

To our knowledge, the safety learning approach has yet to be applied to our understanding of the mental health impact of the COVID-19 pandemic. However, its relevance is self-evident. People may initially wash and sanitize their hands to reduce the risk of COVID-19 infection, but the safety signals provided by the smell of soap and alcohol will come to secondarily reinforce these behaviors. Similar arguments apply to mask wearing and social distancing, and it may be difficult to reverse these now ingrained behaviors as the risk reduces. As might be expected, individuals with OCD have been identified as particularly susceptible to the mental health impact of the COVID-19 pandemic (Jassi et al., 2020), particularly those with contamination-related OCD (Jelinek et al., 2021). Moreover, there is evidence that COVID-19 has resulted in anxiety symptoms and some experimental evidence of attentional bias for virus-related stimuli, related to pre-existing levels of health anxiety in a normal population (Cannito et al., 2020). Trait health anxiety and cyberchondria are risk factors for poor mental health in consequence of the pandemic (Jungmann and Witthöft, 2020). Internet research (e.g., of infection symptoms) and media exposure can amount to safety-seeking behaviors but also generate potential triggers for further safety-seeking behavior depending on how the findings are interpreted (Starcevic and Berle, 2013; Brown et al., 2019; Garfin et al., 2020).

Discussion

Remote cognitive-behavioral therapy for severe anxiety in response to the pandemic has been found to be variably effective based on the perceived quality of the early interactions and in particular the focus on solutions to problems arising (Malins et al., 2020). We suggest that, granted relaxed restrictions and/or recommendations and relatively low case numbers, cognitive-behavioral interventions for persistent anxiety should include the identification of safety signals and reflection on the broader range of solutions needed to ensure wellbeing in the wider sense. As we have seen, safety signals protect conditioned fear from extinction and their systematic removal has been a focus of therapeutic intervention (Craske et al., 2014).

This situation is of course particularly complex in the case of COVID-19: given the uncertainties surrounding the efficacy of the available vaccines against new variants (which will likely continue to emerge), to continue with an appropriate range and level of safety behaviors may be entirely rational. The costs and benefits of behavioral exposure interventions for individuals experiencing COVID-related anxiety will need continual review in the light of our current understanding of the risks associated with COVID-19 infection, also taking into account individual vulnerability factors unrelated to mental health (Jassi et al., 2020). Individualized precision interventions should also be tailored to the prognostic markers provided by individual inhibitory learning profiles (Duits et al., 2021). Perhaps most pressing is the need to identify whether safety learning is increased or decreased in connection with different kinds of anxiety-related disorders and concerns (Orr et al., 2000; Jovanovic et al., 2010; Duits et al., 2021).

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council [grant number BB/S000119/1] and a School of Psychology PhD studentship.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the reviewer for very constructive suggestions to improve the presentation of the theoretical framework of the perspective article, and Rodrigo Sosa for comments on the preprint.

References

Adolphs, R. (2013). The biology of fear. Curr. Biol. 23, R79–R93. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., and Barlow, D. H. (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychol. Rev. 108, 4–32. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. J., Skelly, N., and Chew-Graham, C. A. (2019). Online health research and health anxiety: a systematic review and conceptual integration. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 27:85. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannito, L., Di Crosta, A., Palumbo, R., Ceccato, I., Anzani, S., La Malva, P., et al. (2020). Health anxiety and attentional bias toward virus-related stimuli during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 10:16476. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73599-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cartoni, E., Puglisi-Allegra, S., and Baldassarre, G. (2013). The three principles of action: A Pavlovian-instrumental transfer hypothesis. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:153. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christianson, J. P., Fernando, A. B. P., Kazama, A. M., Jovanovic, T., Ostroff, L. E., and Sangha, S. (2012). Inhibition of fear by learned safety signals: A mini-symposium review. J. Neurosci. 32, 14118–14124. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, M., Mineka, S., and Trumble, D. (1987). The role of response produced and exteroceptive feedback in the attenuation of fear over the course of avoidance learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 13, 239–249. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.13.3.239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craske, M. G., Treanor, M., Conway, C., Zbozinek, T., and Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 58, 10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generate by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 311–333. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duits, P., Baas, J. M. P., Engelhard, I. M., Richter, J., Huisman-van Dijk, H. M., Limberg-Thiesen, A., et al. (2021). Latent class growth analyses reveal overrepresentation of dysfunctional fear conditioning trajectories in patients with anxiety-related disorders compared to controls. J. Anxiety Disord. 78:102361. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernando, A. B. P., Urcelay, G. P., Mar, A. C., Dickinson, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Safety signals as instrumental reinforcers during free-operant avoidance. Learn. Mem. 21, 488–497. doi: 10.1101/lm.034603.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gadenzam, F. J., Kamphuis, J. H., and Kindt, M. (2013). Deficient safety learning characterizes high trait anxious individuals. Biol. Psychol. 92, 342–352. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfin, D. R., Silver, R. C., and Holman, E. A. (2020). The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 39, 355–357. doi: 10.1037/hea0000875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Morein-Zamir, S., Urcelay, G. P., Sule, A., Voon, V., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., et al. (2014). Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 631–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A. (1975). Elements of a Two-Process Theory of Learning. London: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Haaker, J., Lonsdorf, T. B., Schümann, D., Menz, M., Brassen, S., Bunzeck, N., et al. (2015). Deficient inhibitory processing in trait anxiety: evidence from context-dependent fear learning, extinction recall and renewal. Biol. Psychol. 111, 65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Z., Cassaday, H. J., Bonardi, C., and Bibby, P. A. (2013). Do personality traits predict individual differences in excitatory and inhibitory learning? Front. Psychol. 4:245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heym, N., Kantini, E., Checkley, H. L. R., and Cassaday, H. J. (2014). Tourette-like behaviors in the normal population are associated with hyperactive/impulsive ADHD-like behaviors but do not relate to deficits in conditioned inhibition or response inhibition. Front. Psychol. 5:946. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacoby, R. J., and Abramowitz, J. S. (2016). Inhibitory learning approaches to exposure therapy: a critical review and translation to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 49, 28–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jassi, A., Shahriyarmolki, K., Taylor, T., Peile, L., Challacombe, F., Clark, B., et al. (2020). OCD and COVID-19: a new frontier. Cogn. Behav. Therap. 13:e27. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X20000318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelinek, L., Voderholzer, U., Moritz, S., Carsten, H. P., Riesel, A., and Miegel, F. (2021). When a nightmare comes true: change in obsessive-compulsive disorder over the first months of the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Anxiety Disord. 84:102493. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovanovic, T., Norrholm, S. D., Blanding, N. Q., Davis, M., Duncan, E., Bradley, B., et al. (2010). Impaired fear inhibition is a biomarker of PTSD but not depression. Depress. Anxiety 27, 244–251. doi: 10.1002/da.20663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jungmann, S. M., and Witthöft, M. (2020). Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J. Anxiety Disord. 73:102239. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malins, S., Moghaddam, N., Morriss, R., Schröder, T., Brown, P., and Boycott, N. (2020). Predicting outcomes and sudden gains from initial in-session interactions during remote cognitive-behavioral therapy for severe health anxiety. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 28, 891–906. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, I. M., and Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethol. Sociobiol. 15, 247–261. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)90002-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Dalgleish, T., Silston, B., and Prévost, C. (2015). The ecology of human fear: survival optimization and the nervous system. Front. Neurosci. 9:55. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mowrer, O. H. (1956). Two-factor learning theory reconsidered, with special reference to secondary reinforcement and the concept of habit. Psychol. Rev. 63, 114–128. doi: 10.1037/h0040613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108, 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Peri, T., and Pitman, R. K. (2000). De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 290–298. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachman, S. (2012). Health anxiety disorders: A cognitive construal. Behav. Res. Ther. 50, 502–512. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajaraman, A., Austin, J. L., Gover, H. C., Cammilleri, A. P., Donnelly, D. R., and Hanley, G. P. (2022). Toward trauma-informed applications of behavior analysis. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 55, 40–61. doi: 10.1002/jaba.881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltysik, S., and Jelen, P. (2005). In rats, sighs correlate with relief. Physiol. Behav. 85, 598–602. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltysik, S. S., Wolfe, G. E., Nicholas, T., Wilson, W. J., and Garcia-Sanchez, J. L. (1983). Blocking of inhibitory conditioning within a serial conditioned stimulus-conditioned inhibitor compound: maintenance of acquired behavior without an unconditioned stimulus. Learn. Motiv. 14, 1–29. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(83)90010-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosa, R., and dos Santos, C. V. (2019). Conditioned inhibition and its relationship to impulsivity: empirical and theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rec. 69, 315–332. doi: 10.1007/s40732-018-0325-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosa, R., and Ramίrez, M. N. (2019). Conditioned inhibition: historical critiques and controversies in the light of recent advances. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 45, 17–42. doi: 10.1037/xan0000193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starcevic, V., and Berle, D. (2013). Cyberchondria: towards a better understanding of excessive health-related internet use. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 13, 205–213. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurston, M. D., and Cassaday, H. J. (2015). Conditioned inhibition of emotional responses: retardation and summation with cues for IAPS outcomes. Learn. Motiv. 52, 69–82. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2015.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troisi, A. (2020). Fear of COVID-19: insights from evolutionary behavioral science. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 17, 72–75. doi: 10.36131/CN20200207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A., Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P., Ludgate, J., Hackmann, A., and Gelder, M. (2016). Social phobia: The role of in-situation safety behaviors in maintaining anxiety and negative beliefs - republished article. Behav. Ther. 47, 669–674. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anxiety, conditioned inhibition, COVID-19 pandemic, safety learning, cognitive behavioral therapy

Citation: Thurston MD and Cassaday HJ (2022) Safety Learning in Anxiety, Pavlovian Conditioned Inhibition and COVID Concerns. Front. Psychol. 13:866771. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.866771

Received: 31 January 2022; Accepted: 10 March 2022;
Published: 04 April 2022.

Edited by:

Federica Biassoni, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy

Reviewed by:

Hiu Tin Leung, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Copyright © 2022 Thurston and Cassaday. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helen J. Cassaday, aGVsZW4uY2Fzc2FkYXlAbm90dGluZ2hhbS5hYy51aw==; Meghan D. Thurston, bWVnaGFuLnRodXJzdG9uMUBuaHMubmV0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.