Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 22 July 2022
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Changes in Touch Avoidance, Stress, and Anxiety During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Italy

  • 1Institute for Educational Technology, National Research Council of Italy, Genoa, Italy
  • 2Associazione Centro di Psicologia e Psicoterapia Funzionale, Istituto SIF di Padova, Padua, Italy
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli,” Caserta, Italy
  • 4Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy

In the present study we analyzed how attitudes toward touch have changed during the COVID-19 pandemic in an Italian sample, through two different studies: in the first we contacted participants of the Italian validation study of the Touch Avoidance Questionnaire, asking them to take part in a follow-up study (N = 31, 64.5% women, age 42.58 ± 15.15); in the second we recruited a new sample of 717 people (73.92% women, age 34.25 ± 13.11), comparing it to the full validation sample of the Touch Avoidance Questionnaire (N = 335, 64.48% women, age = 35.82 ± 14.32) to further investigate the relationship between the pandemic, stress responses, fear of contagion, anxiety, and attitudes toward touch. Overall, we found higher post-pandemic scores for touch avoidance toward strangers and family members and lower scores in touch avoidance toward friends of either gender, along with a slight increase in anxiety and stress. Touch avoidance was also positively related to anxiety and/or stress levels except for touch avoidance toward same-sex friends, for which the relationship with anxiety was negative. Surprisingly, we found that young people were the most anxious, despite older people being more at-risk of dying from COVID-19. Women were slightly more stressed out. COVID-19-related fears were significant predictors of touch avoidance toward partners, friends and strangers, but not of touch avoidance toward family. The results suggest that touch avoidance increased during the pandemic (except toward same-sex friends), together with anxiety and stress levels, but the change was relatively small.

Introduction

The novel coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 emerged in December 2019, quickly spreading throughout the world. In Italy, one of the first nations hit by the pandemic, the government swiftly imposed a nation-wide lockdown and social distancing measures to contain the outbreak, lasting from March 9 to the beginning of May 2020. Partial lockdown measures, including social distancing, were imposed again in the autumn following the second outbreak.

During the pandemic, several studies focused on the psychological consequences of the pandemic and related prevention measures. An Italian study highlighted high rates of anxiety, distress, and symptoms related to post-traumatic stress disorder in a group of 2,291 respondents (Casagrande et al., 2020). During the second wave of contagion in Italy (November–December 2020), there was a strong increase in sleep disorders, which were related to depression, stress, and anxiety (Salfi et al., 2021a), although the prospect of a vaccination campaign did seem beneficial (Salfi et al., 2021b). Another study analyzed the effects of the lockdown period among Italian late adolescents and elderly people, finding that adolescents displayed higher levels of depression and perceived stress than older people (Amicucci et al., 2021).

Analyzing the international literature about the pandemic’s effects, one may argue that the psychological distress could be interpreted as the result of several factors: fear of contagion, loss of loved ones, financial insecurity, and feelings of isolation. The latter could be a consequence of social distancing measures, radically limited social interactions, especially physical closeness (Every-Palmer et al., 2020).

This study aims to investigate both how attitudes toward physical touch changed in response to the pandemic and how they relate to anxiety and stress, as the lack of physical touch may have had an impact on psychological wellbeing. Physical touch, a primary need for humans and other primates, is related to social bonding, feelings of safety (Dunbar, 2010) and, ultimately, wellbeing (Jakubiak and Feeney, 2019; Packheiser et al., 2021). Prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, social touch was one of the ways people regulated their stress, sought proximity to close others (especially romantic partners) and disclosed their problems and distress in an effort to regulate their emotions (Hazan and Shaver, 1987; Mikulincer et al., 2002; Zaki and Craig Williams, 2013). Moreover, touch seems to be especially important for fostering intimacy (App et al., 2011) and exhibiting love and sympathy (Hertenstein et al., 2009).

Social touch has been categorized into three types: (1) simple, described as a touch of short duration and applied on a restricted part of the body; (2) protracted, involving longer and mutual contact, such as hugging or holding hands; and (3) dynamic, comprising continuous and repetitive movements over the skin, such as caressing (Morrison et al., 2010; Vieira et al., 2016).

The physiological effects of social touch have been studied in recent decades. For example, daily hugging behaviors over a 14-day period have been shown to be significantly and inversely related to proinflammatory cytokines (van Raalte and Floyd, 2021). Similarly, in women, the reported frequency of hugs with partners is correlated with elevated oxytocin levels and lowered blood pressure (Light et al., 2005). Moreover, touch has been linked to lower blood pressure, lower heart rate, and higher oxytocin level (Field, 2010), which is itself related to lower levels of inflammation (Jankowski et al., 2010).

Despite the overall psychophysiological benefits of touch, contextual and personal factors influence each person’s attitude toward touching and being touched, and these could include the response to the pandemic.

Regarding contextual factors, receiving a gentle stroke from an undesirable toucher, for example, changes the pleasant experience into disgust (Ellingsen et al., 2016). Touching behavior is also an expression of dominance associated not only with social status, but also with gender, especially in countries where men are still in clearly dominant positions (Dibiase and Gunnoe, 2004). Additionally, attitude toward touch could be a general individual predisposition. Andersen and Leibowitz (1978) defined a person’s attitude toward touching and being touched in terms of touch avoidance (Ozolins and Sandberg, 2009; Russo et al., 2020). Touch avoidance is influenced by gender and by attachment processes built during infancy and adulthood and is a stable personality trait (Johansson, 2013; Ekeberg, 2016).

When considering individual attitudes toward touch, compared to men, women generally show more positive attitudes toward touch with friends and romantic partners and greater willingness to engage with such touch. However, women are more likely to perceive touch from opposite-sex strangers as unpleasant (Ozolins and Sandberg, 2009; Russo et al., 2020; Passarelli et al., 2021). Age is another factor influencing the pleasantness of touch: affective touch is perceived as more pleasant at a young age (Sehlstedt et al., 2016; Croy et al., 2019). Psychophysiological factors that influence touch avoidance include chronic pain, depression, and anhedonia, all conditions that reduce the hedonic experiences connected to affiliative touch (Pizzagalli et al., 2008; Elvemo et al., 2015; Rømer Thomsen, 2015). Lastly, anxiety seems to be related to touch avoidance: individuals with high trait anxiety appear to be more touch avoidant (Vieira et al., 2016), and social anxiety is accompanied by heightened aversion toward social situations involving touch (Wilhelm et al., 2001).

During the pandemic, social distancing was imposed in an effort “to decrease/interrupt transmission in a population by minimizing contact between potentially infected and healthy individuals” (ECDPC, 2020). These social distancing measures were mostly successful at first, but their emotional costs were not considered. During the COVID-19 pandemic, such policies prompted a change in the attitudes toward social touch, where culturally encouraged touch conveying care, affection and love such as hugging, hand-shaking or just sitting close together while comforting a dying relative was prohibited and labeled as “dangerous” for the transmission of the virus. One consequence of defining social touch as a way to convey the contagion was that it reframed a behavior associated with care and love to a possible threat to oneself and to loved ones (Green and Moran, 2021).

The forced behavioral changes toward touch during the pandemic may have deprived people of a main strategy for managing their distress; it also may have a long-term influence on individual attitudes toward touch by reframing social touch as a dangerous behavior. However, our current understanding of the pandemic’s dynamics does not address the degree to which fear of contagion has changed the individual meaning that people attribute to social touch and whether this possible change influences people’s psychological wellbeing.

Given these premises, we aimed to investigate three different research questions (RQs): (RQ1) whether the COVID-19 pandemic increased touch avoidance (i.e., personal aversion toward touch) in a sample of Italian subjects; (RQ2) how touch avoidance is related to anxiety and distress; and (RQ3) whether touch avoidance is associated with COVID-related fears and risk factors for COVID-19 (i.e., being male, being over 60 years old, being unvaccinated, knowing someone who died from COVID-19).

Those aims were examined in two different studies. The first one took advantage of the sample recruited by Casetta et al. (2020) for the validation of a touch avoidance measure conducted just prior to the pandemic. Re-recruiting some participants from the study allowed us to directly compare attitudes toward touch in a pre-post study design. This study focuses on RQ1 and RQ2, but the small sample size does not allow us to investigate RQ3.

Research questions 1–3 were investigated in Study 2, which recruited a larger sample toward the end of the second Italian outbreak by proposing an online survey. These data were analyzed in comparison with the full Touch Avoidance Questionnaire validation sample, which also measured anxiety and stress prior to the pandemic.

Study 1

Methods

Participants and Procedure

For this study, we contacted participants from the Italian validation study of the Touch Avoidance Questionnaire (TAQ; Casetta et al., 2020), asking them if they wanted to take part in this follow-up study. Only participants who expressly reported interest in participating in further studies were contacted, and 31 participants (20 women, 64.5%; age 42.58 ± 15.15) responded positively. The data for the TAQ validation were collected in the first half of 2019, prior to the COVID-19 pandemic (protocol approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli,” Department of Psychology, 04/2019). While the pre-pandemic data collection was performed using paper-and-pencil questionnaires, the second round was conducted online using Google Forms due to social distancing measures. After providing informed consent and some socio-biographical information such as age, gender, and educational level, participants were asked a series of questions regarding the context in which they experienced the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., whether they knew someone who died from COVID-19, whether they lived alone during the lockdown, whether they worked remotely, etc.). Afterward, they completed a series of questionnaires described in the following section. The Ethics Committee of Policlinico “Paolo Giaccone” in Palermo, Italy, approved this second round of data collection (06/2020). Recruiting and testing conformed with the local Ethics Committee requirements and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measures

Touch Avoidance Questionnaire

The TAQ (Ozolins and Sandberg, 2009; Casetta et al., 2020) is a 37-items questionnaire that investigates attitudes toward touch. Items are responded to on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (“fully disagree”) to 5 (“fully agree”). The questionnaire comprises 5 subscales, corresponding to 5 different categories of individuals with whom touch could be experienced: romantic partners (10 items), same-sex friends (6 items), opposite-sex friends (6 items), family (6 items), and strangers (3 items). For all subscales, higher scores indicate higher touch avoidance (i.e., aversion to touch). The Italian version of the scale has an ordinal alpha ranging 0.59–0.92 for the five subscales and 1-month retest reliability ranging 0.67–0.90. The “stranger” subscale has a lower alpha (0.59), partly due to the low number of items in it, but high test-retest correlation (0.82 after 1 month in the Italian validation study).

Mesure de Stress Psychologique

The Mesure de Stress Psychologique (MSP) (Lemyre and Tessier, 1988; Di Nuovo et al., 2000) is a 49-item self-report questionnaire that measures responses to stress. The participant is asked to self-evaluate how much they felt, in the last 4–5 days, some typical stress responses (e.g., “I feel fatigued,” “I cry easily,” or “I take more than half an hour to fall asleep”). Items consider thoughts, somatic symptoms, emotions, and behaviors. The response scale for each item ranges from 1 (“not at all”) to 4 (“to a great extent”). Higher scores indicate higher levels of stress responses. The Cronbach’s alpha for the scale is 0.94.

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y) (Pedrabissi and Santinello, 1989) is a 40-item questionnaire investigating state and trait anxiety. The response scale for each item ranges from 1 (“not at all”) to 4 (“extremely”). Higher scores indicate higher levels of anxiety. In this study, we only included the state anxiety subscale, which has Chronbach’s alpha of 0.93.

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were conducted using R 4.0.2. Missing data (0.2%) were treated using pairwise deletion. When performing multiple tests, p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using Benjamini-Hochberg’s correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). All tests were two-tailed.

RQ1 was examined by Wilcoxon signed-rank tests comparing (sub)scale scores before and during the pandemic. RQ2 was investigated using a single maximum likelihood path analysis in which gender, age and the post-pre difference on TAQ scores simultaneously predict STAI and MSP scores during the pandemic.

R codes are available at the following repository: https://github.com/M-Pass/TouchAvoidance_Covid/blob/main/Study1.R.

Results

RQ1

Descriptive statistics and a correlogram of (sub)scale scores are available in the Supplementary Material. Results for the Wilcoxon signed-rank tests are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Pre-post comparisons for the scores on each subscale.

The only significant difference was in touch avoidance toward strangers, which greatly increased during the pandemic. Touch avoidance toward other categories of people was unchanged, although the effect size for touch avoidance toward same-sex friends was not negligible, suggesting that a study involving a larger sample may find an effect.

RQ2

Results are reported in Table 2. R2 for the MSP is 0.33; R2 for the STAI-Y1 is 0.27. The residual correlation between MSP and STAI-Y1 scores is 0.74.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Path analysis predicting MSP and STAI-Y1 scores.

The only significant predictor was age, as older people had, on average, less stress and anxiety compared to younger individuals. No other predictor was significant, suggesting that both stress and anxiety are unrelated to change in attitudes toward social touch. However, as the small sample size would make any interpretation of these results tentative, we ran a larger-scale study to further investigate the relationship between the pandemic, stress responses, anxiety, and touch avoidance.

Study 2

Methods

Participants and Procedure

In this study, a new sample of participants took part in a single data collection conducted from April to May 2021 (a period in which Italy was still experiencing a peak of infections and travel restrictions and curfew were still in place on a nation-wide level). Participants were recruited online through snowball sampling (Parker et al., 2019). After providing informed consent, participants were asked to complete the same materials described in Study 1 (i.e., socio-biographical information, COVID-related questions, TAQ, MSP, STAI-Y1), as well as the Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19—Related Fears (MAC-RF; see below). A total of 717 individuals took part in the study (530 women, 73.92%; 182 men, 25.38%; 5 undisclosed, 0.42%; age 34.25 ± 13.11; range, 18–78). The sample was unevenly distributed geographically (35% from North Italy, 7% from central Italy, 58% from South Italy/islands). Data collected from this sample were compared with data collected for the Italian validation of the TAQ (Casetta et al., 2020; N = 335, 216 women, 64.48%; 113 men, 33.63%; 6 undisclosed, 1.79%; age = 35.82 ± 14.32; range, 16--74; 35% from North Italy, 35% from central Italy, and 31% from South Italy/islands).1 The two samples differed in gender composition [X 2(1) = 8.12, p = 0.004], with the validation (pre-COVID) sample having significantly fewer women than the one collected for this study. The age difference was not significant [t(595.55) = 1.68, p = 0.093]. The Ethics Committee of Policlinico “Paolo Giaccone” in Palermo, Italy, approved this study (02/2021). Recruiting and testing conformed with the local Ethics Committee requirements and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measures

Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19—Related Fears

In addition to the measures employed in Study 1, this survey included the Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19—Related Fears (MAC-RF) (Schimmenti et al., 2020). This test is an eight-item, self-report scale that has been developed to assess clinically relevant domains of fear during the COVID-19 pandemic. The MAC-RF is based on a comprehensive theoretical model conceptualizing fears during the pandemics as resulting from an interaction of bodily, interpersonal, cognitive, and behavioral experiences. The authors have suggested four dialectical elements of COVID-19 fear: fear of and for one’s body, as one is both a potential vector and victim; fear of and for others, also related to the tension of prescribed social distancing from important interpersonal relationship; fear of ignorance of the virus as well as knowledge, as information is required for protection but can also be overwhelming and anxiety-inducing; and fear of both personal action and inaction, related to the behavioral consequences of fear. Respondents rate all 8 items on a 5-point Likert scale (from 0 = very unlike me to 4 = very like me). By adding all items’ rates, a total score can be obtained. The higher it is, the stronger COVID-19 related fears are. Cronbach’s α for the whole scale is 0.77.

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were conducted using R 4.0.2. Missing data (1.4% for the pre-pandemic sample, 0.5% for the pandemic sample) were treated using pairwise deletion. When performing multiple tests, p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using Benjamini-Hochberg’s correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). All tests were two-tailed.

RQ1 was examined with independent samples t-tests comparing (sub)scale scores between the samples collected before and during the pandemic. RQ2 and RQ3 were investigated using a maximum likelihood path analysis in which COVID-related fears and COVID risk factors (gender, being over 60, being vaccinated and knowing people who died from COVID-19) predict TAQ, MSP and STAI scores and in which TAQ scores are used in turn as predictors of MSP and STAI scores (although actual causality could be reversed or even bidirectional). Results for RQ2 and RQ3 are presented separately, but they refer to the same model.

R codes are available at the following repository: https://github.com/M-Pass/TouchAvoidance_Covid/blob/main/Study2.R.

Results

RQ1

Descriptive statistics and correlograms for all (sub)scale scores before and during the pandemic are reported in the Supplementary Material. Results for the independent samples t-tests are reported in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pre-post comparisons for the scores on each subscale.

The findings show higher post-pandemic scores for stress, anxiety and touch avoidance toward family members and strangers. However, the results indicate lower scores in touch avoidance toward friends of either gender. No change was found for touch avoidance toward romantic partners. All effect sizes were small or negligible, suggesting that while the pandemic affected attitudes toward touch, stress, and anxiety, the effect was very limited.

RQ2

Results are reported in Table 4. R2 was 0.35 for the MSP and 0.34 for the STAI-Y1. Residual correlation between the two measures is 0.167.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Path analysis predicting MSP and STAI-Y1 scores.

Males presented lower stress (MSP) scores. Both STAI-Y1 and MSP scores were lower for participants over 60 years old and were positively related with touch avoidance toward romantic partners, family, and strangers. Additionally, STAI scores were related to both same-sex and opposite-sex touch avoidance, although with contrasting signs, as the relationship between same-sex touch avoidance and anxiety is negative. Regarding COVID-related fears, behavioral difficulties were found to correlate with both anxiety and stress, while difficulties in cognitive monitoring of concerns correlated with distress, but not anxiety. Lastly, fears related to meaningful relationships correlated with anxiety, but not distress.

RQ3

Results are reported in Table 4. R2 was 0.024 for TAQ partner, 0.027 for TAQ family, 0.109 for TAQ same sex, 0.067 for TAQ opposite sex, and 0.081 for TAQ stranger.

According to the results, individuals over 60 had higher touch avoidance scores toward friends of either gender (and a difference in touch avoidance toward partners bordering on significance). Male participants had higher touch avoidance toward family and same-sex friends. Vaccinated individuals had lower touch avoidance toward partners and family members, although touch avoidance toward friends and strangers was the same for vaccinated and unvaccinated individuals. Lastly, COVID-related fears seem to have different associations with the measures of touch avoidance: fears related to the body and meaningful relationships affected touch avoidance toward strangers, difficulties in monitoring cognitive concerns were correlated with touch avoidance toward opposite-sex friends and behavioral difficulties were correlated with touch avoidance toward friends of any gender. Lastly, fears related to meaningful relationships were negatively correlated with touch avoidance toward partners.

Discussion

While several studies have been conducted on general wellbeing during the pandemic, to the best of our knowledge, none have focused on how social distancing changed people’s attitudes toward touch and its psychological consequences. The aim of this study was to analyze how attitudes toward touch have changed in the last year due to a strong natural stressor like the COVID-19 pandemic, which reconceptualized social touch as a threat. We investigated this question through two different studies. In the first, we contacted participants from the Italian validation study of the Touch Avoidance Questionnaire (TAQ; Casetta et al., 2020), asking them to take part in a follow-up study. In the second, we recruited a new sample of 717 people and compared results to the full validation sample of the TAQ (N = 335) to further investigate the relationship between the pandemic, stress responses, anxiety, and attitudes toward touch.

The first study revealed a change in attitudes toward touch involving strangers, while the larger sample of the second study highlighted a significant change in touch avoidance for family members (higher post-pandemic) and friends of either gender (lower post-pandemic), with slightly higher post-pandemic scores in the subscale measuring touch avoidance for strangers. These results suggest that the reconceptualization of touch as a source of contagion plays a role in the change of attitudes toward touch. Specifically, the higher touch avoidance toward family members could be related to fear of contaminating loved ones. Conversely, touch avoidance toward strangers could be due to the fear of being contaminated by others. The lower scores for touch avoidance toward friends during the pandemic are somewhat surprising, and may suggest a need to compensate for touch deprivation by getting physically closer to peer friends, an explanation consistent with the use of touch as an important way to deal with stress and to satisfy the need for connection (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). In accordance with a previous study (Passarelli et al., 2021), we found that, compared to men, women behave differently in relation to touch, being less touch avoidant toward same-sex friends (Ozolins and Sandberg, 2009; Hielscher and Mahar, 2017; Casetta et al., 2020). Their inclination to help others and to use social support to cope with negative emotions (Matud, 2004; Eisenbarth, 2019) is a reasonable explanation of the further decrease in women’s touch avoidance for same-sex friends during the pandemic. All changes in attitudes toward touch during the pandemic seem to be significant but small, suggesting that the behavioral changes forced by social distancing measures only had a limited impact on internal attitudes.

We then analyzed, in both data sets, how touch avoidance relates to stress and anxiety, finding that touch avoidance toward romantic partners and family members was associated with higher anxiety and stress levels, while touch avoidance toward strangers was only associated with stress, and touch avoidance toward opposite-sex friends was associated only with anxiety. All relationships were in the direction that suggested that touch avoidant individuals experience more stress/anxiety, with touch avoidance toward same-sex friends as the sole exception (for which the relationship was very weak, but negative). One possible interpretation of this result is that people who use social touch as a strategy to manage stress could be negatively affected by social distancing. However, it could also mean that anxious individuals—more affected by fear of contagion—tend to avoid social touch during a pandemic. Therefore, causality could be bidirectional. Differently from other research who highlighted a higher level of touch avoidance in elders (Ozolins and Sandberg, 2009) in this study the older people responded to the pandemic and social distancing with less anxiety and touch avoidance. This unexpected result can contribute to the field of social touch because highlights the importance of touch in managing stress independently from the fear of contagion. Even if elders are the most vulnerable category at risk of developing severe COVID related symptoms (Leung, 2020), our study has shown that the longing for social touch can help to go beyond the fear of contagion.

The reason for this phenomenon could be related to a greater wisdom and acceptance regarding death (Wysokiński et al., 2019), but further investigation is necessary to answer this question. Additionally, the fact that anxiety is higher for younger individuals may suggest that it relates more to spreading the contagion to loved ones, rather than fear for one’s own health.

To further understand the relationship between fear of contamination and touch avoidance, we analyzed whether COVID-related fears, as well as some risk factors for COVID-19 (namely age, gender, being vaccinated and knowing someone who died of COVID-19), influenced attitudes toward touch. The results suggest that, overall, older people and males are more touch avoidant, which is consistent with the hypothesis that touch avoidance correlates with risk of dying from COVID-19. However, we found no difference according to risk factors for touch avoidance toward strangers, arguably the most important source of contagion, which would disconfirm this hypothesis. The inconsistency of these results should be further investigated. Regarding COVID-related fears, they mostly influenced touch avoidance toward strangers and friends, while no significant effect was found for touch avoidance toward family. Specifically, we found a relation between the fear of being either a vector or victim of contagion and touch avoidance toward strangers. This could be interpreted as a sense of one’s physical vulnerability, causing the body to be perceived as a potential source of danger and strangers to be viewed as the most relevant threat of contagion. On the other hand, while fears related to meaningful relationships were negatively related to touch avoidance toward partners, fear of knowing important information was positively related to touch avoidance toward opposite-sex friends. Moreover, behavioral consequences of fear were positively correlated with touch avoidance toward friends of any gender. These results generally support the notion that COVID-related fears lead to touch avoidance. The one relationship for which individuals higher in COVID-related fears expressed lower touch avoidance is the relationship between fears related to meaningful relationship and touch avoidance toward partners. This result may relate to the use of intimate—and possibly sensual—touch as a way to cope with negative emotions.

Lastly, being vaccinated is associated with lower touch avoidance toward family and partners, suggesting that change in attitudes toward touch—which was already relatively small—could be partially reversible when the risk of contagion is lowered.

Summing up, from our study we may state that the pandemic and consequent social distancing has changed the Italian attitude toward touch only slightly: while the difference in touch avoidance before and during the pandemic was statistically significant for most subscales, it was smaller than we expected, suggesting that the pandemic, which changed behavior substantially, did not have much impact on underlying attitudes. This could suggest both that the Italian population was resilient to the changes in their lives due to the pandemic, and that the need for touch, being of prime importance for humans, cannot be entirely disregarded due to fear of contamination. The fact that being vaccinated reduced touch avoidance suggests that the changes in attitudes we observed may be not only small, but also temporary.

This study has several limitations. The sample size of the first study is quite small, and interpretation of its results should be tentative. Nevertheless, its longitudinal nature, with data collected prior to the appearance of COVID-19, can offer insights through data collected in circumstances that unfortunately cannot be replicated. Regarding the limitations of Study 1 and Study 2, having a single measurement during the pandemic does not allow us to investigate whether the apparent increase in psychological distress, anxiety, and touch avoidance was sustained over time. The third limitation is that to answer the survey, it was necessary to have access to a computer or internet-connected mobile phone and to be able to use them, so we probably over-represented people with higher socio-economic status and education, as well as the younger population. Hence, selection bias may have influenced our results: many respondents are young and female. Despite the methodological limitations, this study suggests that attitudes toward touch have changed—if only slightly—due to a strong natural stressor such as the COVID-19 pandemic. However, results suggest that the Italian population was psychologically resilient to the effects of lockdown and social distancing (Prati and Mancini, 2021).

We hope these data may be taken into consideration by political organizations and public health providers to address new ways to help younger people in managing their anxiety. We also hope this study will help us understand future social behaviors, from both a psychological and sociological perspective.

It is likely that the consequences of the pandemic will be pervasive and prolonged, and we still do not know what is going to happen in the near future, so it would be interesting to monitor attitudes toward touch in the coming months and years, reporting on their evolution. It would also be interesting to further study the attitudes of older people, trying to understand why they feel less anxious even when they are the most exposed to mortality. Understanding their attitudes more deeply could help keep them safer, motivating them to conduct themselves conscientiously.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://github.com/M-Pass/TouchAvoidance_Covid/.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Policlinico “Paolo Giaccone,” in Palermo, Italy. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LC and LR collected the data. MP and LC organized the database. MP performed the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to conception and design of the study, wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.854110/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ In the pre-pandemic sample, geographical region was associated with touch avoidance toward family members [lower in the South than in the North; t(311) = 4.05, p < 0.001]. In the post-pandemic sample, geographical region was associated with touch avoidance toward family members [lower in the South than in the North; t(671) = −4.52, p < 0.001], touch avoidance toward strangers [higher in the South than in the North; t(671) = 4.42, p < 0.001], stress [higher in the South than in the North; t(671) = 4.05, p < 0.001], and anxiety [higher in the South than in the North; t(671) = 5.20, p < 0.001].

References

Amicucci, G., Salfi, F., D’Atri, A., Viselli, L., and Ferrara, M. (2021). The differential impact of COVID-19 lockdown on sleep quality, insomnia, depression, stress, and anxiety among late adolescents and elderly in Italy. Brain Sci. 11:1336. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11101336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, P. A., and Leibowitz, K. (1978). The development and nature of the construct touch avoidance. Environ. Psychol. Nonverb. Behav. 3, 89–106. doi: 10.1007/BF01135607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

App, B., McIntosh, D. N., Reed, C. L., and Hertenstein, M. J. (2011). Nonverbal channel use in communication of emotion: how may depend on why. Emotion 11, 603–617. doi: 10.1037/a0023164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Statist. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casagrande, M., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., and Forte, G. (2020). The enemy who sealed the world: effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Med. 75, 12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casetta, L., Rizzi, L., Passarelli, M., Arcara, G., and Perrella, R. (2020). Italian validation of the touch avoidance measure and the touch avoidance questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 11:1673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croy, I., Sehlstedt, I., Wasling, H. B., Ackerley, R., and Olausson, H. (2019). Gentle touch perception: from early childhood to adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 81–86. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Nuovo, S., Rispoli, L., and Genta, E. (2000). Misurare lo Stress. Il test MSP e Altri Strumenti per Una Valutazione Integrata, Vol. 19. Milan: FrancoAngeli.

Google Scholar

Dibiase, R., and Gunnoe, J. (2004). Gender and culture differences in touching behavior. J. Soc. Psychol. 144, 49–62. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.144.1.49-62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ECDPC (2020). Considerations Relating to Social Distancing Measures in Response to Covid 19 – Second Update (Technical Report). Available online at: www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/documents/covid-19-social-distancing-measuresg-guide-second-update.pdf

Google Scholar

Eisenbarth, C. (2019). Coping with stress: gender differences among college students. Coll. Stud. J. 53, 151–162.

Google Scholar

Ekeberg, D. (2016). The Relationship between Interpersonal Touch and Attachment Organization. Stockholm: Stockholm University.

Google Scholar

Ellingsen, D. M., Leknes, S., Løseth, G., Wessberg, J., and Olausson, H. (2016). The neurobiology shaping affective touch: expectation, motivation, and meaning in the multisensory context. Front. Psychol. 6:1986. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elvemo, N. A., Landrø, N. I., Borchgrevink, P. C., and Håberg, A. K. (2015). Reward responsiveness in patients with chronic pain. Eur. J. Pain 19, 1537–1543. doi: 10.1002/ejp.687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Every-Palmer, S., Jenkins, M., Gendall, P., Hoek, J., Beaglehole, B., Bell, C., et al. (2020). Psychological distress, anxiety, family violence, suicidality, and wellbeing in New Zealand during the COVID-19 lockdown: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One 15:e0241658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (2010). Touch for socioemotional and physical well-being: a review. Dev. Rev. 30, 367–383. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2011.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, L., and Moran, L. (2021). Covid-19, social distancing and the ‘scientisation’of touch: exploring the changing social and emotional contexts of touch and their implications for social work. Qual. Soc. Work 20, 171–178. doi: 10.1177/1473325020973321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertenstein, M. J., Holmes, R., McCullough, M., and Keltner, D. (2009). The communication of emotion via touch. Emotion 9, 566–573. doi: 10.1037/a0016108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hielscher, E., and Mahar, D. (2017). An exploration of the interaction between touch avoidance and the pleasant touch (C-tactile afferent) system. Perception 46, 18–30. doi: 10.1177/0301006616661938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubiak, B. K., and Feeney, B. C. (2019). Hand-in-hand combat: affectionate touch promotes relational well-being and buffers stress during conflict. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45, 431–446. doi: 10.1177/0146167218788556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankowski, M., Bissonauth, V., Gao, L., Gangal, M., Wang, D., Danalache, B., et al. (2010). Anti-inflammatory effect of oxytocin in rat myocardial infarction. Basic Res. Cardiol. 105, 205–218. doi: 10.1007/s00395-009-0076-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansson, C. (2013). Views on and perceptions of experiences of touch avoidance: an exploratory study. Curr. Psychol. 32, 44–59. doi: 10.1007/s12144-012-9162-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemyre, L., and Tessier, R. (1988). Mesure de Stress Psychologique (MSP): se sentir stressé-e. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 20, 302–321. doi: 10.1037/h0079945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, C. (2020). Risk factors for predicting mortality in elderly patients with COVID-19: a review of clinical data in China. Mech. Ageing Dev. 188:111255. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2020.111255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, K. C., Grewen, K. M., and Amico, J. A. (2005). More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biol. Psychol. 69, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matud, M. P. (2004). Gender differences in stress and coping styles. Personal. Individ. Differ. 37, 1401–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., and Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment system in adulthood: threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental representations of attachment figures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 83, 881–895. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, I., Löken, L. S., and Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 305–314. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2007-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozolins, A., and Sandberg, C. (2009). Development of a multifactor scale measuring the psychological dimensions of touch avoidance. Int. J. Psychol. A Biopsychosoc. Approach 3, 33–56.

Google Scholar

Packheiser, J., Malek, I. M., Reichart, J. S., Katona, L., Luhmann, M., and Ocklenburg, S. (2021). The association of embracing with daily mood and general life satisfaction: an ecological momentary assessment study. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/rxbcv

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, C., Scott, S., & Geddes, A. (2019). “Snowball Sampling,” in SAGE Research Methods Foundations, Eds P. Atkinson, S. Delamont, A. Cernat, J.W. Sakshaug, and R.A. Williams (London: Sage Publications), doi: 10.4135/9781526421036831710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passarelli, M., Casetta, L., Rizzi, L., and Perrella, R. (2021). Responses to stress: investigating the role of gender, social relationships, and touch avoidance in italy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:600. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18020600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedrabissi, L., and Santinello, M. (1989). Verifica della validità dello STAI forma Y di Spielberger. [Verification of the validity of the STAI, Form Y, by Spielberger.]. Giunti Organ. Spec. 191–192, 11–14.

Google Scholar

Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., and Fava, M. (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J. Psychiatr. Res., 76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prati, G., and Mancini, A. D. (2021). The psychological impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns: a review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies and natural experiments. Psychol. Med. 51, 201–211. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721000015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rømer Thomsen, K. (2015). Measuring anhedonia: impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward. Front. Psychol. 6:1409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, V., Ottaviani, C., and Spitoni, G. F. (2020). Affective touch: a meta-analysis on sex differences. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108, 445–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salfi, F., D’Atri, A., Tempesta, D., and Ferrara, M. (2021a). Sleeping under the waves: a longitudinal study across the contagion peaks of the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy. J. Sleep Res. 30:e13313. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salfi, F., Lauriola, M., D’Atri, A., Amicucci, G., Viselli, L., Tempesta, D., et al. (2021b). Demographic, psychological, chronobiological, and work-related predictors of sleep disturbances during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. Sci. Rep. 11:11416. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90993-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schimmenti, A., Starcevic, V., Giardina, A., Khazaal, Y., and Billieux, J. (2020). Multidimensional assessment of COVID-19-related fears (MAC-RF): a theory-based instrument for the assessment of clinically relevant fears during pandemics. Front. Psychiatry 11:748. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sehlstedt, I., Ignell, H., Backlund Wasling, H., Ackerley, R., Olausson, H., and Croy, I. (2016). Gentle touch perception across the lifespan. Psychol. Aging 31:176. doi: 10.1037/pag0000074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Raalte, L. J., and Floyd, K. (2021). Daily hugging predicts lower levels of two proinflammatory cytokines. Western J. Commun. 85, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/10570314.2020.1850851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vieira, A. I., Ramos, A. V., Cavalheiro, L. M., Almeida, P., Nogueira, D., Reis, E., et al. (2016). Reliability and validity of the European Portuguese version of the social touch questionnaire. J. Nonverb. Behav. 40, 363–377. doi: 10.1007/s10919-016-0239-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, F. H., Kochar, A. S., Roth, W. T., and Gross, J. J. (2001). Social anxiety and response to touch: incongruence between self-evaluative and physiological reactions. Biol. Psychol. 58, 181–202. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00113-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wysokiński, M., Fidecki, W., and Jarosz, M. (2019). Elderly people’s acceptance of death: a study of a polish cohort. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16: 3374.

Google Scholar

Zaki, J., and Craig Williams, W. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion 13, 803–810. doi: 10.1037/a0033839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: touch avoidance, COVID-19, fear of contagion, pandemic effects, attitudes toward touch

Citation: Passarelli M, Casetta L, Rizzi L, Perrella R, Maniaci G and La Barbera D (2022) Changes in Touch Avoidance, Stress, and Anxiety During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Italy. Front. Psychol. 13:854110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.854110

Received: 13 January 2022; Accepted: 21 June 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.

Edited by:

Irene Ceccato, University of Studies G. d’Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy

Reviewed by:

Yuta Ujiie, Research Organization of Open Innovation and Collaboration, Ritsumeikan University, Japan
Erlis Ruli, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Passarelli, Casetta, Rizzi, Perrella, Maniaci and La Barbera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Casetta, l.casetta@psicoterapiafunzionale.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.