Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 March 2022
Sec. Cognition

Introversion and High Spatial Ability Is Associated With Origami Proficiency

  • Department of Complex and Intelligent Systems, Future University Hakodate, Hakodate, Japan

This study examined the relationship between origami performance, personality traits, and spatial ability. The researchers asked 43 Japanese university students (19 women and 24 men) to fold three models of origami (paper folding). Their performance was assessed by the number of successes in correctly folding the paper to make the models. They also answered the personality inventory NEO-FFI and completed the block-design test of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV, which measures the spatial ability of people. The results showed that although origami performance demonstrated no significant relation with neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness, or conscientiousness, it improved as introversion tendency and spatial ability increased. There were no differences based on sex in origami performance. The findings suggest that performing origami requires spatial ability, which supports the view that origami is a potential educational material for training and enhancing spatial ability, and that introversion is advantageous to origami performance.

Introduction

Origami is well known as the traditional Japanese practice of paper folding, although paper folding has been traditionally performed in other regions as well, such as China, Spain, and Germany (McArthur and Lang, 2013). Traditionally, there are a limited number of origami models; however, recently, a number of new origami models have been introduced. Origami is currently practiced worldwide. It is generally used for amusement, especially for children. There are many beautiful and complex origami figures that can be now considered a type of art (Lang, 2011; McArthur and Lang, 2013, 2017).

Origami has also been analyzed mathematically, and the computational aspects of origami, such as the computational complexity of flat folding, have been studied in the field of computational geometry (Demaine and O’Rourke, 2007). Origami is also used in engineering applications. For example, Miura-ori is famous for rigid origami folding, which has been proposed for the folding of solar panels in space (Hull, 2013).

Origami is also used in kindergarten schools and childcare in Japan to build a basic understanding of geometric shapes and concepts among children (Akimaru et al., 2007). Origami is popular in Japanese childhood education, probably because Fröbel, a German educator, who created the foundation of childhood education, included origami in his educational materials, and childhood education in Japan has been influenced by his education theory. He considered that origami can help children understand geometrical concepts intuitively (Igarashi, 2012). Furthermore, a number of attempts have been made to use origami for education in elementary and middle schools (Higginson and Colgan, 2001; Robichaux and Rodrigue, 2003; Boakes, 2009; Golan and Jackson, 2009; Wares, 2011, 2016; Arıcı and Aslan-Tutak, 2015; Burte et al., 2017), as well as in colleges (Boakes, 2011). Improvement of geometrical understanding by origami have been reported (Boakes, 2009, 2011; Arıcı and Aslan-Tutak, 2015; Burte et al., 2017). Origami has been considered to stimulate and train spatial ability while folding paper, that is, geometrically transforming a paper’s shape and making geometrical figures, which can contribute to the development of geometrical concepts (Boakes, 2009, 2011; Taylor and Hutton, 2013; Cakmak et al., 2014; Burte et al., 2020).

Although recent mathematical and technological analyses of origami have been actively explored, and origami has been used for education, psychological studies of origami have been rather scarce. However, several studies have examined the effects of origami on the improvement of spatial ability as well as geometrical understanding, and the developmental effect of spatial abilities were demonstrated for students in elementary schools (Taylor and Hutton, 2013; Burte et al., 2017), children with mathematical difficulty (Krisztián et al., 2015), middle- and high-school students (Boakes, 2009; Arıcı and Aslan-Tutak, 2015) and college students (Boakes, 2011). Moreover, training effects of origami on spatial ability was demonstrated on a study of neuroscience (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2012). Thus, origami has positive developmental and training effects on spatial ability. However, it is still unknown whether people with high spatial ability perform origami well. Physical exercise has good effects on cognitive abilities (Hötting and Röder, 2013), but good performers of physical exercise do not mean people with high cognitive abilities. Similarly, positive relationship between origami performance and spatial ability is not logically derived from positive effects of origami on spatial ability. Although aspects of spatial ability were measured by mental paper folding (Ekstrom et al., 1976), spatial ability may not be critical for origami because origami is a task of actually, but not mentally, folding paper. Thus, this study examined the relationship between origami performance and spatial ability. A positive relationship between origami performance and spatial ability would strengthen the claim that spatial ability is needed for origami and that origami is a potential educational material for training spatial ability.

Although origami is said to raise good emotional characteristics and personality such as carefulness and perseverance, very few studies have experimentally examined the psychological aspects related to origami. Hence, this study also examined the relationship between origami performance and personality. One of the famous factor models of personality is the Big Five model, which describes the personality of human beings using the five factors: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Digman, 1990; Goldberg, 1990). The Big Five model is supported for people from various cultural backgrounds, although variations across cultures have been noted (De Raad et al., 2010). The Big Five personality traits are related to creativity (Batey and Furnham, 2006; Furnham and Bachtiar, 2008; Furnham et al., 2008), and academic performance (O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007).

Origami is claimed to require cautiousness and perseverance because those engaged in it, called folders, have to carefully read origami diagrams and instructions and then follow instructions to fold models accurately. Most origami models cannot be folded if the order, location, and manner of folding are incorrect in the crucial folding steps. Cautiousness is generally regarded as a facet of conscientiousness, and perseverance is also related to this factor. If people need to be cautious and perseverant in performing origami, conscientious people would obtain a high score for origami performance. Conscientiousness is also associated with achievement-striving, and high conscientiousness is positively related to high academic success (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2003; Noftle and Robins, 2007; O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007; Poropat, 2009). Further, conscientiousness is associated with higher performance in many domains of cognitive tasks in older adults (Sutin et al., 2019). This suggests that conscientiousness is positively related to origami performance.

Origami is also said to raise children’s creativity (Pope and Lam, 2009; Gur and Kobak-Demir, 2017); however, little empirical evidence has been presented to support this. Performing origami following instructions may appear to be unrelated to creativity, but it involves some aspects of problem-solving (Tenbrink and Taylor, 2015; Burte et al., 2017), and a high level of creativity would help in these aspects of problem-solving in origami. Studies have found that openness to experience has been related to creativity (Furnham et al., 2005) and cognitive flexibility (McCrae, 1987; Costa and McCrae, 2010). A study has found that openness is also associated with higher cognitive performance in older adulthood (Sutin et al., 2019). If creativity and cognitive flexibility help in performing origami, the performance of origami would be related to openness.

Extraversion/inversion is also related to cognitive performance; extraverts tend to be better at dividing attention to various tasks (Eysenck, 1982), resistance to distraction (Dornic and Ekehammar, 1990; Campbell, 1992; Dobbs et al., 2011), and retrieval from memory (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1979), whereas introverts perform better at vigilance (Shaw et al., 2010) and reflective problem-solving (Kumar and Kapila, 1987; Koelega, 1992). It is suggested that these associations of extraversion/introversion with cognitive performance are mediated through several psychological mechanisms, such as arousal and attentional control (Eysenck, 1982; Matthews, 2009). Although extraversion seems to be associated with better performance than introversion as a whole (Matthews, 2009), some studies reported that extraversion was negatively related to academic performance (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2003; Furnham et al., 2003). Paper folding consists of many physical and mental operations, and extraversion/introversion is related to many cognitive functions, which may be related to extraversion/introversion in some ways, although it cannot be properly said which way it is.

Neuroticism has been reported to be related to poor cognitive performance (Eysenck, 1983; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985) and was sometimes negatively associated with academic performance (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2003; Poropat, 2009), mainly due to susceptibility to high anxiety and stress in people with high neuroticism (Eysenck, 1967; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985; O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007). People with high neuroticism may feel stressed and anxious when they have to perform a task in the presence of an experimenter or examiner, and when their performance is going to be evaluated. Thus, those with high neuroticism might perform origami more poorly, although it would not be specific to origami. Agreeableness is not generally related to cognitive or academic performance (O’Connor and Paunonen, 2007). Hence, agreeableness can be said to be unrelated to origami performance.

The present study examined the relationship between origami performance and spatial ability and the Big Five personality traits. It formulated the following hypotheses that consider the above-discussed arguments: (1) As spatial ability increases, origami performance increases, (2) Conscientiousness and openness would be positively, and neuroticism would be negatively, related to origami performance; extraversion/introversion might be related to origami performance in some way, and agreeableness would not be related to origami performance. To test these hypotheses, we conducted an experiment in which participants answered questions on a Big Five personality inventory, took a test of spatial ability, and performed origami tasks. We also collected data on participants’ self-evaluations of school subjects and origami experiences to help interpret the data on the relationship between spatial ability, personality, and origami performance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 43 Japanese undergraduate and graduate students (20–24 years old, 19 women and 24 men) majoring in information sciences, participated in the experiment. The purpose of the experiment was withheld from the participants.

Materials

Intelligence Test of Spatial Ability

The present study used the block-design test included in the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) IV (Wechsler, 2008) to measure spatial ability. In the task, participants arranged red and white blocks to match a displayed pattern. This test is considered to require spatial visualization ability and motor skills. It has been used for assessing spatial skills (Serbin and Connor, 1979; Casey et al., 2014).

Personality Inventory

This study used the Japanese version of the NEO-FFI Personality Inventory as a measure of participants’ personalities. The NEO-FFI is a short version of the revised NEO personality inventory (Costa and McCrae, 2010). It measures the Big Five personality traits of neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness using 60 questions (12 for each trait).

Origami

The participants were asked to fold the following three origami models: Crane, Iris, and Windmill. Crane: The crane is a traditional Japanese origami model known worldwide as a symbol of peace. Iris: The iris is also a traditional Japanese origami model, albeit not as popular as the crane, even among the Japanese. Windmill: This model was created by Haga (2014), who is famous for origamics (Haga, 2008), mathematical analysis of origami, and the application of origami to geometry education. The windmill is difficult to fold because it includes tato folding, a folding technique that requires the application of substantial tension on the paper [The windmill origami model of Haga (2014) is different from the well-known traditional windmill model and is much more difficult to fold.]. Images of the folded origami models are shown in Figure 1. The participants folded these models by looking at origami diagrams with Japanese instructions for each model. The diagrams and instructions for Haga’s windmill were copied from Haga’s book (Haga, 2014), whereas the diagrams for the crane and iris were taken from an Internet site (Takeuchi, n.d.).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Photographs of origami figures for the origami tasks. The crane and iris are traditional Japanese origami models. The windmill was folded following the diagram and instructions in Haga’s book (Haga, 2014).

Questionnaires

At the beginning of the experiment, the participants reported demographic information, such as age and sex, by answering a questionnaire. After completing the origami tasks, the participants filled out another questionnaire with the following questions (by selecting from the provided options): When were the first and last times you performed origami? (4–6 years old, 7–12 years old, 13–16 years old, 19–22 years old, never performed origami, or not remember when); How often do you fold origami? (3–4 days per week, 1–2 days per week, 1–2 times per month, 1–2 times per year, or never); What origami models are you good at folding? How well did you fold each of the origami models? (1: very poor to 7: very good), and How much did you enjoy folding each of the models (1: not at all to 7: very much). They were also asked about their proficiency in school subjects [Japanese, mathematics, foreign language (English), science, social studies, physical education, art, and music]. The participants responded by choosing the appropriate number (1: very poor to 7: very good), whether they like having fun outdoors (yes or no), and whether they like jigsaw puzzles (yes, no, neither yes nor no).

Procedure

The experiment was conducted in a partitioned area of a quiet office room, and the participants completed the tasks and evaluations in the following order. (1) Answering questionnaire on demographic information; (2) Taking up WAIS block-design test; (3) Answering questions on Personality Inventory NEO-FFI; (4) Folding origami models in the following order: crane, iris, and windmill; and (5) Answering the final questionnaire. The participants folded the crane and iris models until they reported the completion of the folding or gave up on the completion of the models. A time limit of 20 min was set for folding the windmill model; otherwise, the experiment would have overextended if the participants were given as much time as they needed to decide to discontinue the process of folding the model. Origami performance was recorded using a video camera, although the data were not analyzed. The experiment lasted from 50 min to 2 h.

Data Analysis

The participants were categorized into groups according to their performance level (i.e., the number of successes in the origami tasks). The study conducted MANOVA using Wilks Λ, with the scores in the personality trait scales and block-design test as dependent variables, and the origami performance group as an independent variable. Furthermore, to determine differences between the groups in detail, we conducted an ordered logistic regression analysis (Agresti, 2003), with the standardized scores in the personality trait scales and block-design test as the independent variables, and the performance group as the dependent variable, thus reversing the independent and dependent variables from those for the MANOVA. The data on self-evaluation of school subjects were analyzed similarly. The cross-tables of the origami performance and the results of the questionnaires were tested using Fisher’s exact test, with multiple comparisons corrected using Bonferroni’s method. All of the above analyses were conducted using R (R Core Team, 2020).

Results

The cases in which the participants finished folding the origami models without any omitted steps in the instructions, regardless of the quality of the finished work, were categorized as successful folding, and those in which they gave up folding the paper to achieve the desired model or finished it incorrectly, were categorized as unsuccessful folding. Five types of success were identified: no success, success for only the crane model, success for only the iris model, success for the crane and iris models, and success for all three models. The number of participants for each of the five types is presented in Table 1. Eight participants (19%) correctly folded all the models. Twenty-four participants (56%) successfully folded the crane and iris, but not the windmill, and 11 (26%) successfully folded at most one model. Thus, we categorized the participants into three groups based on the number of successes in folding, namely, at most one, two, and three, to represent unskilled, ordinary, and proficient folders, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Result patterns of the origami tasks.

The mean standardized scores of the five personality traits in NEO-FFI and the block-design test of WAIS IV for each of the three groups regarding origami performance are shown in Figure 2. The MANOVA on the standardized scores for the NEO-FFI and block-design test showed that the main effect of the groups was significant [Λ = 0.565, F(12,70) = 1.92, p = 0.046, ηp2 = 0.248], indicating differences among the three groups. The results of the ordered logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 2. The order of the three groups was set according to the number of successes: “zero or one success,” “two successes,” and “three successes.” The coefficients for the extraversion and block-design test scores were found to be significant: the less extroverted (i.e., the more introverted) the participants were, the more they tended to succeed; the higher the score for the block-design test, the more they tended to succeed. These results were consistent with the mean scores of the groups (Figure 2). As the number of successes increased, the score for extraversion decreased, and the scores for the block-design test increased. The coefficients for neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness in the logistic regression analysis were not significant (p > 0.1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean standardized scores for personality traits in NEO-PPI and the block-design test of the WAIS-IV for each group, categorized based on the number of origami successes. The error bars represent standard errors. N, neuroticism; E, extraversion; O, openness to experience; A, agreeableness; C, conscientiousness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Ordinary logistic regression analysis of the origami successes with the scores for NEO-FFI and block-design test of the WAIS-IV.

The sample size may be small for this logistic regression analysis. Maximum likelihood estimates of logit model coefficients have substantial bias in small samples. The number of events per variable (EPV), which indicates the number of outcome events per the number of candidate predictors, are generally recommended to be 10 or greater (Peduzzi et al., 1996). However, this rule would not be applied in that analysis. The expiatory variables for that regression analysis are not correlated so much. The correlation coefficients between expiatory variables were less than 0.3 except the correlation between conscientiousness and openness (0.359). It is unsurprising, because the Big-Five personality traits are known to be fairly independent of each other, and spatial ability. In the case that explanatory variables are uncorrelated, we can approximately estimate the effect of each variable separately, with one explanatory variable. For just one explanatory variable, a much smaller number of samples is required. Thus, the required sample size would be much smaller than that suggested by the EPV of 10 for that analysis. Still, the number of the samples are fairly small. Since it was reported that Firth (1993) penalized maximum likelihood estimator [equivalently, maximum penalized likelihood with powers of the Jeffreys (1946) prior as penalty] instead of maximum likelihood estimator eliminates most of the bias in the logistic regression due to small samples (Rainey and McCaskey, 2021). Hence, the ordered logistic regression was conducted by the method of Firth’s maximum penalized likelihood using R package brglm2 (Kosmidis et al., 2020; Kosmidis, 2021). The results were similar to those by the maximum likelihood method, though the regression coefficients were a little smaller than the ones shown in Table 2 as a whole. The regression coefficient of extraversion was still significant (p = 0.012), and that of spatial ability was almost significant (p = 0.052), while the other regression coefficients were not significant (p > 0.10). Thus, the results of this logistic regression were fairly robust.

The scores for the self-evaluation of the school subjects were similarly analyzed. The mean standardized scores for each of the three groups regarding origami performance are shown in Supplementary Figure 1. The MANOVA of the standardized scores for school subjects showed a significant main effect of the group [Λ = 0.443, F(16,66) = 2.07, p = 0.020, ηp2 = 0.32]. The results of the ordered logistic regression with the standardized scores as independent variables and the grouping as the dependent variable are shown in Supplementary Table 1. Only the coefficient for physical education was found to be significant: as the self-evaluation scores for physical education decreased, the origami performance improved. The ordered logistic regression was also conducted by the method of Firth’s maximum penalized likelihood to this data. Although the regression coefficients became smaller on the whole, the results were similar to that of the maximum-likelihood logistic regression. Only the coefficient of physical education was significant (p = 0.031).

Table 3 shows the cross-tabulation of performance (zero or one, two, and three successes) × sex of the participants (female and male), frequency of performing origami, and the last time the participants performed origami. As some cells in the tables had very small numbers, some response categories regarding the frequency of performing origami and the last time of origami folding were integrated. Fisher’s exact test correcting multiple comparisons using Bonferroni’s method, which tested the independence of origami performance, sex, frequency, and the last time of origami folding, showed that sex and frequency of performing origami were not significantly related to origami performance (p > 0.1). Meanwhile, the last time of origami folding was related to origami performance (p < 0.05); skilled origami performers tended to have performed origami more recently.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Number of origami successfully folded crossed by sex, frequency of performing origami, and last time of origami folding.

Discussion

This study examined the relationship between origami performance, personality traits, and spatial ability. The participants folded three models of origami, and their performance was assessed by the number of successes in folding the models correctly. They also took the personality inventory NEO-FFI and completed the block-design test of the WAIS IV, which measures spatial ability. The results showed that origami performance improved as introversion tendency and spatial ability increased; however, it demonstrated no significant relationship with neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness, or conscientiousness. The findings suggest that performing origami requires spatial ability and that introversion is advantageous to origami performance.

The study did not score the quality of the folded origami owing to the difficulty of achieving an objective scoring. We tried to make the standard for evaluating the quality of folding for each model, and scoring the folded figures. However, it was found to be difficult to rate the quality of the folded figures objectively and there remained some subjective elements in the evaluation. Moreover, the quality of almost all the correctly folded origami figures was fairly high, and was not varied so much. On the other hand, whether origami folding is correctly accomplished can be determined objectively to a fair degree. Therefore, this measure of performance evaluation for origami folding would be appropriate. However, relationship between the quality of folded origami figures and spatial ability/personality would be an interesting issue. How to evaluate the folded figures objectively is an issue for the future.

Spatial ability was positively related to origami performance; good/poor origami performers have higher/lower spatial abilities. The need and use of spatial ability while folding paper physically, but not mentally following origami instructions, is yet unknown and there is a lack of clear empirical evidence. The positive relationship between origami performance and spatial ability shown in this study strongly suggests that spatial ability is needed to perform origami correctly. Prior studies have found that origami has been used to improve spatial ability and geometrical understandings (Higginson and Colgan, 2001; Robichaux and Rodrigue, 2003; Boakes, 2009, 2011; Golan and Jackson, 2009; Wares, 2011, 2016; Burte et al., 2017). The findings of this study provide the basis for the use of origami to improve spatial ability, although further studies should be conducted on how to use origami as educational materials, and actual training effects of origami on the improvement of spatial ability.

The study hypothesized that extraversion/introversion might be associated with origami proficiency in some way. The results indicated that the more introverted the participants were, the better they were at folding the origami models. Since the reported relations of extraversion/introversion to performance of cognitive tasks depend on the task itself, extraverts are better at dividing attention to various activities (Eysenck, 1982), resisting distraction (Dornic and Ekehammar, 1990; Campbell, 1992; Dobbs et al., 2011), and retrieving from memory (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1979), whereas introverts perform better in vigilance (Shaw et al., 2010) and reflective problem-solving tasks (Kumar and Kapila, 1987; Koelega, 1992). The origami tasks of this study should not demand divided attention and retrieval from memory so much, because the participants folded paper reading the instructions. However, some vigilance would be required for folding paper, and some amount of reflective problem solving is needed to translate the origami instructions to actual physical folding. Thus, introverts may benefit from high vigilance and reflective problem-solving abilities. Alternatively, the difference in origami performance between introverts and extraverts may be explained by the different ways in which they engage in social interactions. Extraverts like communicating with other people, whereas introverts tend to prefer activities without social interaction (Kirkcaldy and Furnham, 1991). Hence, introverts may be more familiar with activities performed alone, such as reading books compared with extraverts (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1964). Origami is an activity that is usually performed alone, which may be ideal for introverts. In addition, introverts may have more experience in origami folding, probably because it can be performed alone, whereas extroverts may have fewer experiences, probably because they prefer social activities such as team sports to origami (Eagleton et al., 2007). This explanation is consistent with the results shown in Table 3 that skillful folders tended to have performed origami more recently. Introverted people may be relatively poor at social interactions, but good at non-social activities, and thus, their introversion would prove to be advantageous for performing origami. It is noteworthy that this relation between origami performance and extraversion cannot be explained by the correlation between spatial ability and extraversion as the correlation between the scores of the block-design test and extraversion trait was extremely weak (r = −0.073), and the effect of extraversion on origami performance shown in the ordered logistic regression (Table 2) did not include those of the other explanatory variables, including extraversion. Furthermore, no significant relationship between spatial intelligence and extraversion was reported (Roberts, 2002).

The study also hypothesized that openness and conscientiousness are related to origami performance. The results showed no correlations, which is rather surprising, considering the clearly observed relationship between introversion and origami performance. The lack of relationship between conscientiousness and origami performance may be explained by the long-term nature of personality; carefulness and perseverance, which are components of conscientiousness, and was hypothesized to contribute to origami performance, would be rather long-term tendencies of behaviors (Duckworth et al., 2007), and it might not reflect the short-term mental concentration needed to perform our origami task. Alternatively, lack of care and perseverance may not be a primary cause of origami failure; those traits would not contribute to high origami performance if a participant did not understand how to fold the origami models in the first place.

Although we hypothesized a positive relationship because openness is related to creativity, it was found that openness was not related to origami performance. Openness was reported to be related to divergent thinking (McCrae, 1987), which is considered the basis of creativity. Performing origami following instructions should be a type of problem- solving in which well-defined solutions exist. Hence, convergent, but not divergent thinking, is important for origami. Since openness is not a good indicator of convergent thinking ability, it might not reflect origami performance efficiently. The lack of a relationship between openness and origami performance may also be explained by the view that creativity is not a single construct and may consist of several aspects of creativity, which are related to different cognitive abilities and styles. A study reported that artistic, scientific, and verbal creativity can be separated, and object visualization is related to artistic creativity and spatial visualization is associated with scientific creativity (Kozhevnikov et al., 2013). Artists seem to have higher openness than scientists, although highly creative scientists tend to have higher openness than non-creative ones (Feist, 1998). Hence, openness is related more to artistic creativity than to scientific creativity. Since origami performance following instructions is associated with spatial ability, it may relate to scientific creativity, but it may not be related to verbal and artistic creativity (However, artistic creativity should be needed for the creation of new origami models.). The weak relationship between openness and scientific creativity may explain the lack of a significant relationship between origami performance and openness.

We also hypothesized that neuroticism is negatively related to origami performance due to its susceptibility to mental stress. However, in this study, it was noted that neuroticism was higher for good origami performance than for poor performance (Figure 2), although this difference in origami performance was not statistically significant. Since our origami task is like a play activity, but not like a cognitive or academic test, the task may not invoke mental stress even to people with high neuroticism. Alternatively, origami may not be affected by stress levels. Measurement of the stress level during or after origami would clarify which of these explanations is correct.

Origami is becoming increasingly popular worldwide, with many exhibitions of artistic origami works in recent times (McArthur and Lang, 2013, 2017). Moreover, the mathematical and computational aspects of origami have been analyzed in the fields of mathematics and technology by various studies (Demaine and O’Rourke, 2007; Hull, 2013). Meanwhile, studies on origami in the field of psychology have been scarce. The present study showed that introversion and spatial ability are related to origami performance. In the area of education, origami is popular in Japan’s kindergarten and childcare facilities, and it is attracting attention in other countries. Many attempts have been made to improve spatial ability and geometric understanding using origami as a mathematical material (Boakes, 2009, 2011; Taylor and Hutton, 2013; Cakmak et al., 2014). Introverted people are observed to be good at origami; they may be more interested in the geometry of paper folding than extraverts. Mathematical education by origami may be more appropriate for introverted people than for extraverts. Whether the efficacy of geometrical education using origami differs between extraverts and introverts would be an issue to be addressed through future studies.

This study had several limitations. First, all of the participants were young university students; the findings on the relationship between origami performance, extraversion, and spatial ability may not apply to children and older populations. Second, the participants were Japanese, and almost all Japanese had some experience in origami folding during their childhood. Thus, the present results may not be generalizable to people from different cultural backgrounds and with different education levels. Third, the study used three origami models for origami tasks, but there may be more appropriate origami models for studying the relation between origami performance, spatial ability, and personality. Although further studies are required to examine these issues, the present work provides the first step in elucidating the psychological aspects of origami.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Future University Hakodate. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank the members of my laboratory who assisted in the preparation of the experimental materials and data acquisition.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.825462/full#supplementary-material

References

Agresti, A. (2003). Categorical Data Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/0471249688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akimaru, T., Niwa, T., and Teshi, C. (2007). The knowledge and use of “traditional games” by nursery teachers in japan. Stud. Hum. Cult. 7, 57–78.

Google Scholar

Arıcı, S., and Aslan-Tutak, F. (2015). The effect of origami-based instruction on spatial visualization, geometry achievement, and geometric reasoning. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 13, 179–200. doi: 10.1007/s10763-013-9487-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batey, M., and Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 132, 355–429. doi: 10.3200/mono.132.4.355-430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boakes, N. (2009). “The impact of origami-mathematics lessons on achievement and spatial ability of middle-school students,” in Origami4: Fourth International Meeting of Origami Science, Mathematics, and Education, ed. R. J. Lang (Natick, MA: A K Peters, Ltd), 471–481.

Google Scholar

Boakes, N. (2011). “Origami and spatial thinking of college-age students,” in Origami5: Fifth International Meeting of Origami Science, Mathematics, and Education, eds M. Yim, P. Wang-Iverson, and R. J. Lang (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 173–187.

Google Scholar

Burte, H., Gardony, A. L., Hutton, A., and Taylor, H. A. (2017). Think3d!: improving mathematics learning through embodied spatial training. Cogn. Res. 2, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s41235-017-0052-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burte, H., Gardony, A. L., Hutton, A., and Taylor, H. A. (2020). Elementary teachers’ attitudes and beliefs about spatial thinking and mathematics. Cogn. Res. 5, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s41235-020-00221-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cakmak, S., Isiksal, M., and Koc, Y. (2014). Investigating effect of origami-based instruction on elementary students’ spatial skills and perceptions. J. Educ. Res. 107, 59–68. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2012.753861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, J. B. (1992). Extraversion and noise sensitivity: a replication of Dornic and Ekehammar’s study. Pers. Individ. Differ. 13, 953–955. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90015-H

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. M., Dearing, E., Dulaney, A., Heyman, M., and Springer, R. (2014). Young girls’ spatial and arithmetic performance: the mediating role of maternal supportive interactions during joint spatial problem solving. Early Childhood Res. Q. 29, 636–648. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2003). Personality traits and academic examination performance. Eur. J. Pers. 17, 237–250. doi: 10.1002/per.473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (2010). The NEO Personality Inventory: 3. Odessa, FL: Psychological assessment resources.

Google Scholar

De Raad, B., Barelds, D. P., Levert, E., Ostendorf, F., Mlačić, B., Blas, L. D., et al. (2010). Only three factors of personality description are fully replicable across languages: a comparison of 14 trait taxonomies. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98:160. doi: 10.1037/a0017184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demaine, E. D., and O’Rourke, J. (2007). Geometric Folding Algorithms: Linkages, Origami, Polyhedra. Cambridge: Cambridge university press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511735172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 417–440. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobbs, S., Furnham, A., and McClelland, A. (2011). The effect of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 307–313. doi: 10.1080/00140130210121932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dornic, S., and Ekehammar, B. (1990). Extraversion, neuroticism, and noise sensitivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 11, 989–992. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90283-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., and Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92:1087. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagleton, J. R., McKelvie, S. J., and De Man, A. (2007). Extra version and neuroticism in team sport participants, individual sport participants, and nonparticipants. Percept. Motor Skills 105, 265–275. doi: 10.2466/pms.105.1.265-275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., and Dermen, D. (1976). Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Spring-field, IL: Thomas.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and Individual Differences. New York, NY: Plenum. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-2413-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. B. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. Sevenoaks: Hodder and Stoughton. doi: 10.1037/t02711-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and Arousal: Cognition and Performance. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-68390-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W. (1983). “Anxiety,” in Stress and Fatigue in Human Performance, ed. R. Hockey (Chichester: Wiley), 273–295.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Memory scanning, introversion-extraversion, and levels of processing. J. Res. Pers. 13, 305–315. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(79)90021-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2, 290–309. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, D. (1993). Bias reduction of maximum likelihood estimates. Biometrika 80, 27–38. doi: 10.1093/biomet/80.1.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Bachtiar, V. (2008). Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 45, 613–617. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K., and Manfield, J. (2008). Personality, hypomania, intelligence and creativity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 44, 1060–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and McDougall, F. (2003). Personality, cognitive ability, and beliefs about intelligence as predictors of academic performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 14, 47–64. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2003.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Zhang, J., and Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2005). The relationship between psychometric and self-estimated intelligence, creativity, personality and academic achievement. Imag. Cogn. Pers. 25, 119–145. doi: 10.2190/530V-3M9U-7UQ8-FMBG

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golan, M., and Jackson, P. (2009). “Origametria: a program to teach geometry and to develop learning skills using the art of origami,” in Origami4: Fourth International Meeting of Origami Science, Mathematics, and Education, ed. R. J. Lang (Natick, MA: A K Peters, Ltd), 459–469.

Google Scholar

Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: the big-five factor structure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59:1216. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.6.1216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, H., and Kobak-Demir, M. (2017). Geometry teaching via origami: the views of secondary mathematics teacher trainees. J. Educ. Pract. 8, 65–71.

Google Scholar

Haga, K. (2008). Origamics: Mathematical Explorations Through Paper Folding. Singapore: World Scientific Publishing. doi: 10.1142/7023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haga, K. (2014). Origami de Tanoshimu Kikagaku-Zukei. Tokyo: SB Creative Corp.

Google Scholar

Higginson, W., and Colgan, L. (2001). Algebraic thinking through origami. Math. Teach. Middle Sch. 6, 343–349. doi: 10.5951/MTMS.6.6.0343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hötting, K., and Röder, B. (2013). Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2243–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, T. (2013). Project Origami: Activities for Exploring Mathematics, 2nd Edn. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. doi: 10.1201/b14320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igarashi, Y. (2012). A study of history of origami and origami as childcare teaching materials. J. Urawa Jr. Coll. 46, 45–68.

Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N., and Jaušovec, K. (2012). Sex differences in mental rotation and cortical activation patterns: can training change them? Intelligence 40, 151–162. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00345-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeffreys, H. (1946). An invariant form for the prior probability in estimation problems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. A Math. Phys. Sci. 186, 453–461. doi: 10.1098/rspa.1946.0056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkcaldy, B., and Furnham, A. (1991). Extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism and recreational choice. Pers. Individ. Differ. 12, 737–745. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90229-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelega, H. S. (1992). Extraversion and vigilance performance: 30 years of inconsistencies. Psychol. Bull. 112:239. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosmidis, I. (2021). brglm2: Bias Reduction in Generalized Linear Models. R Package Version 0.8.2. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=brglm2 (accessed January 23, 2021).

Google Scholar

Kosmidis, I., Kenne Pagui, E. C., and Sartori, N. (2020). Mean and median bias reduction in generalized linear models. Stat. Comput. 30, 43–59. doi: 10.1007/s11222-019-09860-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozhevnikov, M., Kozhevnikov, M., Yu, C. J., and Blazhenkova, O. (2013). Creativity, visualization abilities, and visual cognitive style. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 83, 196–209. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krisztián, Á, Bernáth, L., Gombos, H., and Vereczkei, L. (2015). Developing numerical ability in children with mathematical difficulties using origami. Percept. Motor Skills 121, 233–243. doi: 10.2466/24.10.PMS.121c16x1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, D., and Kapila, A. (1987). Problem solving as a function of extraversion and masculinity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 8, 129–132. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90020-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, R. J. (2011). Origami Design Secrets: Mathematical Methods for an Ancient Art. Boca Raton, FL: AK Peters/CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Matthews, G. (2009). “Cognitive processes and models,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Personality, 2nd Edn, eds P. J. Corr and G. Matthews (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.), 400–426.

Google Scholar

McArthur, M., and Lang, R. J. (2013). Folding Paper: The Infinite Possibilities of Origami. North Clarendon, VT: Tuttle Publishing.

Google Scholar

McArthur, M., and Lang, R. J. (2017). New Expressions in Origami Art: Masterworks from 25 Leading Paper Artists. North Clarendon, VT: Tuttle Publishing.

Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52:1258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noftle, E. E., and Robins, R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: big five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93:116. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, M. C., and Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performance. Pers. Individ. Differ. 43, 971–990. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peduzzi, P., Concato, J., Kemper, E., Holford, T. R., and Feinstein, A. R. (1996). A simulation study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 49, 1373–1379.

Google Scholar

Pope, S., and Lam, T. K. (2009). “Using origami to promote problem solving, creativity, and communication in mathematics education,” in Origami4: Fourth International Meeting of Origami Science, Mathematics, and Education, ed. R. J. Lang (Natick, MA: A K Peters, Ltd), 517–525.

Google Scholar

Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychol. Bull. 135:322. doi: 10.1037/a0014996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rainey, C., and McCaskey, K. (2021). Estimating logit models with small samples. Polit. Sci. Res. Methods 9, 549–564. doi: 10.1017/psrm.2021.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, M. J. (2002). The relationship between extraversion and ability. Pers. Individ. Differ. 32, 517–522. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00053-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robichaux, R. R., and Rodrigue, P. R. (2003). Using origami to promote geometric communication. Math. Teach. Middle Sch. 9, 222–229. doi: 10.5951/MTMS.9.4.0222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serbin, L. A., and Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children’s play preferences and patterns of cognitive performance. J. Genet. Psychol. 134, 315–316. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1979.10534065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, T. H., Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V. S., Silverman, L., and Costa, P. T. Jr. (2010). Individual differences in vigilance: personality, ability and states of stress. J. Res. Pers. 44, 297–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., and Terracciano, A. (2019). Five-factor model personality traits and cognitive function in five domains in older adulthood. BMC Geriatr. 19:343. doi: 10.1186/s12877-019-1362-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, H. A., and Hutton, A. (2013). Think3d!: training spatial thinking fundamental to STEM education. Cogn. Instr. 31, 434–455. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2013.828727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenbrink, T., and Taylor, H. A. (2015). Conceptual transformation and cognitive processes in Origami paper folding. J. Probl. Solving 8:1. doi: 10.7771/1932-6246.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wares, A. (2011). Using origami boxes to explore concepts of geometry and calculus. Int. J. Math. Educ. Sci. Technol. 42, 264–272. doi: 10.1080/0020739X.2010.519797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wares, A. (2016). Mathematical thinking and origami. Int. J. Math. Educ. Sci. Technol. 47, 155–163. doi: 10.1080/0020739X.2015.1070211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2008). WAIS-IV: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. doi: 10.1037/t15169-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: origami, paper folding, spatial ability, personality, big-five model, introversion, extraversion

Citation: Hanada M (2022) Introversion and High Spatial Ability Is Associated With Origami Proficiency. Front. Psychol. 13:825462. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.825462

Received: 30 November 2021; Accepted: 04 February 2022;
Published: 02 March 2022.

Edited by:

Lara L. Jones, Wayne State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Thora Tenbrink, Bangor University, United Kingdom
Ágota Krisztián, Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary

Copyright © 2022 Hanada. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mitsuhiko Hanada, hanada@fun.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.