Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 14 February 2022
Sec. Cognition

Working Memory and Transcranial-Alternating Current Stimulation—State of the Art: Findings, Missing, and Challenges

  • Department of Medical Biophysics and Medical Informatics, Third Faculty of Medicine, Charles University in Prague, Praha, Czechia

Working memory (WM) is a cognitive process that involves maintaining and manipulating information for a short period of time. WM is central to many cognitive processes and declines rapidly with age. Deficits in WM are seen in older adults and in patients with dementia, schizophrenia, major depression, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, etc. The frontal, parietal, and occipital cortices are significantly involved in WM processing and all brain oscillations are implicated in tackling WM tasks, particularly theta and gamma bands. The theta/gamma neural code hypothesis assumes that retained memory items are recorded via theta-nested gamma cycles. Neuronal oscillations can be manipulated by sensory, invasive- and non-invasive brain stimulations. Transcranial alternating-current stimulation (tACS) and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) are frequency-tuned non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques that have been used to entrain endogenous oscillations in a frequency-specific manner. Compared to rTMS, tACS demonstrates superior cost, tolerability, portability, and safety profile, making it an attractive potential tool for improving cognitive performance. Although cognitive research with tACS is still in its infancy compared to rTMS, a number of studies have shown a promising WM enhancement effect, especially in the elderly and patients with cognitive deficits. This review focuses on the various methods and outcomes of tACS on WM in healthy and unhealthy human adults and highlights the established findings, unknowns, challenges, and perspectives important for translating laboratory tACS into realistic clinical settings. This will allow researchers to identify gaps in the literature and develop frequency-tuned tACS protocols with promising safety and efficacy outcomes. Therefore, research efforts in this direction should help to consider frequency-tuned tACS as a non-pharmacological tool of cognitive rehabilitation in physiological aging and patients with cognitive deficits.

Introduction to Brain Oscillations and Working Memory

Working Memory

Working memory (WM) is a cognitive process that involves maintaining and processing information for a short period of time (Baddeley, 2012). WM is highly involved in goal-directed behavior (Chai et al., 2018), and was described by Baddeley and Hitch’s multicomponent model before the advent of competing models (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Daneman and Carpenter, 1980; Engle, 2002; Barrouillet et al., 2004, 2009; Cowan, 2008). Among the competing models of WM: Daneman and Carpenter’s (1980) model, which showed that the capacity of WM is reflected by the reading span task and comprehensive tests; the time-based resource sharing model proposed by several researchers, which showed that the capacity of WM is a product of attention (Barrouillet et al., 2004, 2009). Details of other relevant WM models can be found in the review article by D’Esposito and Postle (2015). The multicomponent model initially consisted of three subcomponents, namely the phonological loop (verbal WM), the central executive, and the visuospatial sketchpad (visuospatial WM) (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baars and Franklin, 2003; Cowan, 2008; Kim et al., 2015). Then, episodic memory was added to the three main subcomponents of the WM model (Baddeley, 2000; RepovŠ and Baddeley, 2006). WM consists of three main steps: (1) encoding (memory items); (2) maintenance and manipulation of information; and (3) retrieval (recalling of the information) (D’Esposito, 2007; Baddeley, 2012).

The occipital and fronto-parietal cortices, particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the parietal cortex, are thought to be highly involved in the processing of WM (Osaka et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2015; Chai et al., 2018). This involvement is clearly evident in behavioral studies in which non-invasive stimulation of DLPFC (Kehler et al., 2020), ACC (Turi et al., 2020), and parietal cortex (Wolinski et al., 2018) influences the outcomes of WM tasks. The DLPFC plays an important role in maintaining information by retrieving stored information, inhibiting irrelevant stimuli, updating information, and top-down control of memory processes (Osaka et al., 2003; Moore et al., 2013; Vartanian et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2015). ACC acts as an “attention controller” assessing the need to adapt the information received based on task requirements (Osaka et al., 2003). The parietal cortex is thought to be the “workspace” for sensory and perceptual processing (Owen et al., 2005; Andersen and Cui, 2009).

Working memory is important for supporting higher cognitive processes such as learning, planning, mathematical skills, reasoning, etc. Therefore, WM deficits underlie problems in higher cognitive processes and developmental difficulties (Baddeley, 2003; Jeffries and Everatt, 2004; Raghubar et al., 2010; Logie, 2011). It is well known that neuroplasticity in the lateral prefrontal cortex decreases with age and memory deficits are apparent in the elderly (Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004; Ziaei et al., 2017; Goldsworthy et al., 2020; Haque et al., 2021). Moreover, older people are unable to optimally ignore task-irrelevant information (Borghini et al., 2018). However, aging is not the only factor affecting WM performance. WM deficits have been observed in a number of neurological and mental disorders such as schizophrenia, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), major depressive disorder (MDD), bipolar affective disorder, Alzheimer‘s disease, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Stegmayer et al., 2015; Maehler and Schuchardt, 2016; Grot et al., 2017; Le et al., 2017), and in patients with traumatic brain injury (Dunning et al., 2016; Phillips et al., 2017). Disturbances in the prefrontal cortex have been linked to WM deficits in patients with mood disorders particularly the orbital, and medial regions of prefrontal cortex, as well as DLPFC (Drevets, 2000; Barch et al., 2003).

Brain Oscillations and Cross Frequency Coupling

Brain oscillations arise from the coordinated and synchronized activities of a large number of neuronal populations and are divided into five recognizable frequency bands: delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma, and they are involved in several functional processes in the brain (Cebolla and Cheron, 2019; Jensen et al., 2019). The anatomical locations and the functions in which the frequency bands are involved are listed in Table 1. Brain oscillations in different frequency bands have been associated with WM processes (Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014; Riddle et al., 2020b), particularly theta, gamma, and alpha bands. Theta oscillations play an essential role in the temporal organization of WM items, gamma oscillations affect the maintenance of WM information, while alpha oscillations play a role in inhibitory top-down control and the inhibition of task-irrelevant information (Klimesch et al., 2007; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The anatomical locations and the functions of the frequency bands in the cortical structure.

Cross-frequency coupling (CFC) is the interaction between brain oscillations in different frequency bands (Jirsa and Müller, 2013; Sotero, 2016; Siebenhühner et al., 2020). There are several ways in which CFC can occur: Phase-to-phase, phase-to-frequency, power-to-power, and phase-to-power interactions (Jensen and Colgin, 2007). In phase-to-phase coupling: phase locking occurs between different frequency bands and their phase relationship remains constant (Schack et al., 2002; Palva J. M. et al., 2005); in phase-to-frequency coupling: the frequency of the faster oscillations is modulated by the phase of the slower oscillations (Jensen and Colgin, 2007); in power-to-power coupling: the power variation of the faster oscillations correlates with the power variations of the lower oscillations (Bruns and Eckhorn, 2004); in phase-to-power coupling: the power of the faster oscillations is modulated by the phase of the slower oscillations (Lakatos et al., 2005; Mormann et al., 2005; Canolty et al., 2006). One of the most well-known couplings associated with WM is theta/gamma phase-amplitude coupling (PAC). This coupling provides the neural substrate for the representation and maintenance of multiple WM items, which is called theta/gamma neural code. The hypothesis behind theta/gamma neural code assumes that retained memory items are recorded via theta-nested gamma cycles (Bahramisharif et al., 2018). Thus, theta/gamma PAC is considered to be the neurophysiological signature of WM (Axmacher et al., 2010; Lisman and Jensen, 2013). In this context, two models have been adopted; one model states that each gamma wave represents a single memory item and only a finite number of gamma waves can fit into a given theta cycle, which may limit the capacity of WM (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Jensen and Lisman, 1996). The second model assumes that the entire gamma burst that fits into the theta cycle encodes a single memory item (Herman et al., 2013; Van Vugt et al., 2014). After a certain time, the memory items must be refreshed by the new gamma bursts. This reactivation occurs after a few theta cycles, which could explain the limited capacity of WM (Van Vugt et al., 2014). The causal relationship between memory and oscillations can be tested by modulating neuronal oscillations to causally alter behavior (Hanslmayr et al., 2019). Oscillatory entrainment refers to the modulation of neuronal oscillations and can be achieved via three main approaches: sensory entrainment (Adrian and Matthews, 1934; Romo and Salinas, 2003; Whittingstall and Logothetis, 2009; Ng et al., 2013), non-invasive electric/magnetic stimulation (Marshall et al., 2006; Sahlem et al., 2015; Bueno-Lopez et al., 2019), and invasive electrical stimulation (Borchers et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2013; Riva-Posse et al., 2018).

Non-invasive Brain Stimulation

Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques refer to the use of transcranial stimulation to modulate brain excitability with an excellent safety profile when properly performed (Boes et al., 2018). They include two modalities: transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (Fregni et al., 2005; Boes et al., 2018; Polanía et al., 2018). The best-known forms of tES are transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS), transcranial alternating-current stimulation (tACS), and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS). These methods induce reversible alterations in cortical activities (Paulus et al., 2016). In particular, tDCS uses static and weak direct current (Hanslmayr et al., 2019), while the electric current continuously changes its strength and reverses its direction, forming a sinusoidal wave in tACS (Marshall et al., 2006; Kuo and Nitsche, 2012; Sahlem et al., 2015). In tRNS, weak direct currents with random frequencies and amplitudes are applied (Widhalm and Rose, 2019). TMS briefly induces focal and strong currents in brain tissues by generating electromagnetic fields (Mishra et al., 2011). TMS can be administered as single pulse, pairs of pulses, or repeated pulses (Rossini et al., 2015; Valero-Cabré et al., 2017). Repetitive transcranial-magnetic stimulation (rTMS) refers to a sequence of TMS pulses administered at a specific intensity and frequency (Mishra et al., 2011).

Frequency-tuned NIBS is a recent approach in neuroscience in which the frequency of the externally applied entraining stimulus matches naturally occurring oscillations and is mainly used as a modulator of cognitive abilities (Vosskuhl et al., 2018). Moreover, entrainment of ongoing oscillations is strongest when the frequencies of the entraining stimulus match the endogenous frequencies (Ali et al., 2013; Vossen et al., 2015). Indeed, tACS and rTMS have been used to entrain endogenous brain oscillations in a frequency-specific manner and in turn cognitive functions (Balconi, 2013; Dayan et al., 2013; Fröhlich et al., 2015; Tavakoli and Yun, 2017). tACS can be administered online or offline; online tACS is applied during cognitive tasks, whereas offline tACS is administered immediately before or between tasks. The sustained brain activity following stimulation is referred to as the “aftereffect” (Veniero et al., 2015). Indeed, successful tACS produces long-lasting aftereffects, as has been shown with offline tACS (Pozdniakov et al., 2021). The aftereffect demonstrates changes in synaptic plasticity rather than entrainment per se (Bland and Sale, 2019). The prolonged aftereffects of tACS may be related to a phenomenon called spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) (Elyamany et al., 2021). When input action potentials occur immediately before output action potentials, the synapse is strengthened, as suggested by the STDP phenomenon. Thus, if tACS is administered at a frequency close to the resonance frequency during stimulation, synapses in the stimulated region could be strengthened according to the STDP mechanism (Chang et al., 2021). Multiple sessions of tACS may consolidate the neuroplasticity effects, thus eliciting long-lasting effects (Chang et al., 2021; Elyamany et al., 2021). In general, endogenous oscillations are stimulated by tACS through several mechanisms: (1) Oscillatory entrainment, i.e., when tACS is applied at a frequency above that of an endogenous oscillation, the frequency of that oscillation is accelerated; (2) increasing endogenous oscillation amplitude by applying tACS at the same frequency as that of the intrinsic target; (3) cross-frequency coupling by applying predetermined stimulation frequencies, which could affect oscillations in other frequency bands; and (4) synchronization or desynchronization of specific endogenous oscillations by stimulating the electrodes, which could affect phase coherence in the stimulated regions (Antal and Herrmann, 2016). However, the optimal stimulation frequency is task-specific; beta-tACS improves short-term memory, gamma-tACS affects fluid intelligence, and theta-tACS improves WM, etc. (Santarnecchi et al., 2013; Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014; Feurra et al., 2016; Violante et al., 2017).

Working memory declines are distressing for the elderly and patients with neurological disorders. Although some pharmacological interventions might improve or prevent WM deficits in a particular group of patients, they can have serious side effects (Bogdanov and Schwabe, 2016). Thus, identifying techniques to prevent WM deficits that can be safely applied in humans is a pivotal and hot topic in neuroscience. Compared to rTMS, tACS demonstrates superior cost, tolerability, portability, and safety profile, making it an attractive potential tool for improving cognitive performance (Tavakoli and Yun, 2017; Bland and Sale, 2019). Although cognitive research with tACS is still in its infancy compared to rTMS, a number of studies have shown a promising WM enhancement effect, especially in the elderly and patients with cognitive deficits (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019; Kehler et al., 2020; Chang et al., 2021). This review highlights the effect of tACS on WM, by presenting various methods and results of frequency-tuned tACS in healthy and unhealthy human adults, discussing established findings, unknowns, challenges, and perspectives important for developing stimulation protocols with promising safety and efficacy outcomes.

Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation and Working Memory in Healthy Participants

Theta-Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation

Among the models that have been addressed to understand the underlying mechanisms behind WM, one states that each gamma wave represents a single memory item (Sauseng et al., 2019). Therefore, the length of the theta cycle determines the number of gamma waves that can fit into a given theta cycle, and thus the WM capacity (Jensen and Lisman, 1996; Lisman and Jensen, 2013). It was found that the application of 4 Hz theta-tACS over the right parietal cortex improved the quality of memory representations, while 7 Hz theta-tACS impaired the quantity of WM (Guo et al., 2021). It has been demonstrated that theta cycles with low frequencies have a greater capacity to encode more gamma waves (Tseng et al., 2018). Wolinski et al. (2018) tested this hypothesis by slowing down and speeding up parietal theta frequencies by tACS to 4 and 7 Hz, respectively. They found that 4Hz theta-tACS enhanced WM capacity, whereas 7Hz theta-tACS had an unfavorable effect (Wolinski et al., 2018). The same findings were obtained by Jones et al. where theta-tACS of 4.5 Hz applied over fronto-parietal area improved WM outcomes better than 7 Hz (Jones et al., 2019). In addition, Vosskuhl et al. (2015) stimulated a wide network of the fronto-parietal cortices with below individual theta frequencies-tACS to probe the effect of such stimulation on memory performance. This protocol enhanced the capacity of STM but not that of WM (Vosskuhl et al., 2015). Bender et al. (2019) used the same experimental setup up utilized by Wolinski et al. (2018) but instead, the reference electrodes were positioned over Oz, Cz, and T8 (equidistant from the stimulation electrode P4), in this way, high-focal stimulation of the right posterior parietal cortex was achieved. Bender et al. (2019) aimed to achieve high-focal tACS over the parietal lobe and prevent the possible co-stimulation of the prefrontal or subcortical areas directly caused by the reference electrodes placed at frontal (Vosskuhl et al., 2015) or supraorbital site (Wolinski et al., 2018), respectively. The results of Bender et al. (2019) study are consistent with the results of the studies mentioned above that used different reference electrode montages (Wolinski et al., 2018; Jones et al., 2019).

Theta oscillations play an essential role in local processing and interregional functional connectivity, particularly in fronto-parietal regions (Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014; Fröhlich et al., 2015). Therefore, the cognitive effect of manipulating theta oscillations by the means of tACS has been extensively studied (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014; Jaušovec et al., 2014; Meiron and Lavidor, 2014; Gonzalez-Perez et al., 2019; Abellaneda-Pérez et al., 2020; Sahu and Tseng, 2021). Two studies looking at the effect of theta-tACS on WM outcomes showed that theta-tACS administered to the right or left parietal cortex improved WM performance during backward recall task, while theta-tACS administered over left parietal cortex improved WM outcomes during visual-array comparison task (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014; Jaušovec et al., 2014). The positive effect of online theta-tACS on WM performance was reported by Meiron and Lavidor (2014) who administered theta-tACS over right and left prefrontal cortices during verbal WM task. Sahu and Tseng (2021) pointed out that the WM enhancement effect of theta-tACS applied over the right fronto-parietal network was more pronounced in low-performers. Contrary to expectations, Chander et al. (2016) found that frontal midline theta tACS impaired WM outcomes in 2-back task, while Gonzalez-Perez et al. (2019) showed no cognitive enhancement effect of theta tACS administered over the occipital cortex for the perception and memory of facial and object stimuli. On the other hand, Röhner et al. (2018) compared the effect of tDCS, tACS, and sham stimulation administered over the fronto-parietal cortex in three separate sessions on the results of WM (2-back task) using a within-subject crossover design, where tasks were performed before, during, and after stimulation. The results of the study showed no differences in WM outcomes (reaction times and accuracy) according to the stimulation type. In an attempt to eliminate the practice effect (resulting from task repetition) on task results. A subgroup analysis was conducted with 10 participants who had received sham stimulation in the first session and showed similar cognitive performance on tasks administered before session 2 and session 3 (before tDCS and tACS). The results of the subgroup analysis indicated a cognitive improvement effect in favor of tACS stimulation (Röhner et al., 2018). The details of the studies are summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

Synchronization between brain oscillations of different brain cortices is considered an essential step in cognitive and behavioral processes (Klimesch et al., 2010; Siegel et al., 2012; Kleinert et al., 2017). Therefore, the effect of manipulating the phase synchrony of theta oscillations between cerebral cortices known to be involved in WM processes has been extensively addressed in the literature (Polanía et al., 2012; Alekseichuk et al., 2017; Kleinert et al., 2017; Violante et al., 2017; Tseng et al., 2018), the results of the studies can be outlined as follows: in-phase theta tACS over right and left posterior parietal cortices enhanced visual WM task scores in low-performers, while anti-phase theta-tACS had a detrimental effect on high-performers’ memory abilities (Tseng et al., 2018); Polanía et al. (2012) suggested that, in contrary to the anti-phase setting, in-phase theta tACS (between the left prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex) significantly reduced reaction times in a delayed letter discrimination task; Alekseichuk et al. (2017) induced fronto-parietal intra-hemispheric desynchronization and synchronization (theta-phase) and assessed the outcomes of 2-back visuospatial WM. They found that theta phase desynchronization had a negative impact on WM outcomes (reaction time and performance) and on phase connectivity (frontal and parietal cortices), but the synchronization of theta phases between the areas of interest had no effect on WM outcomes or electroencephalography (EEG) features (Alekseichuk et al., 2017); whereas Violante et al. (2017), examined the effect of in-phase and anti-phase theta-tACS on WM in different brain cortices (right fronto-parietal middle frontal gyrus and inferior parietal cortices) and suggested that in-phase theta-tACS improved n-back task outcomes when the cognitive demands were significantly high; in contrast to the results of the above studies, Kleinert et al. (2017) found no significant effects of theta-tACS (in-phase and anti-phase) applied over right fronto-temporal regions on visuospatial WM task outcomes or EEG features. On the other hand, Reinhart and Nguyen (2019) stimulated two brain regions by the mean of theta-tACS in healthy older adults using different experimental setups. They found that frontotemporal, in-phase theta-tuned (tuned to individual brain network dynamics)-tACS enhanced local theta/gamma PAC and WM task accuracy. Interestingly, the WM enhancement effect persisted for at least 50 min after stimulation (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019). The studies are listed in Supplementary Table 1.

Alpha, Beta, Gamma Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation

Gamma oscillations play an essential role in the maintenance and processing of WM (Honkanen et al., 2015). Therefore, the effect of gamma-tACS on WM outcomes has been extensively tested in healthy participants (Hoy et al., 2015; Tseng et al., 2016; Möller et al., 2017; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018; Misselhorn et al., 2020). The results of the studies can be summarized as follows: gamma-tACS delivered over DLPFC selectively improved participants’ performance in tasks with higher cognitive load (3-back task), as suggested by Hoy et al. (2015). In three studies comparing the effect of theta-tACS and gamma-tACS on WM outcomes, it was found that neither theta-tACS nor gamma-tACS delivered over the left middle frontal gyrus (Santarnecchi et al., 2016), fronto-parietal cortex (Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018), or occipital cortex (Gonzalez-Perez et al., 2019) had reliable cognitive enhancement effect on WM outcomes. On the other hand, Tseng et al. (2016) suggested that unlike theta-tACS, left temporo-parietal anti-phase gamma-tACS boosted visual WM-binding outcomes in low-performers. Thompson et al. (2021) compared the effect of parietal gamma-tACS and parietal alpha-tACS on the outcomes of probabilistic retrospective cues task, with results indicating a cognitive enhancement effect in favor of gamma-tACS. Contrary to expectation, parietal gamma-tACS together with cognitive training (participants performed WM training with stimulation ∼25 min/day for five consecutive days) significantly impaired WM training-related gains (Möller et al., 2017). The details of the studies are summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

Alpha and beta oscillations are involved in different cognitive domains. Alpha oscillations are involved in the functional inhibition of visual processing areas, silencing task-irrelevant information and consequently improving the focus on task-relevant information processing (Klimesch et al., 2007; Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012; Popov et al., 2018). Due to the inhibitory function of alpha and beta oscillations, entrainment of these frequency bands would be beneficial for WM (Sauseng et al., 2009; Borghini et al., 2018). However, desynchronization of alpha and beta bands in the information processing regions while performing WM tasks could improve WM outcomes (Hanslmayr et al., 2019). Alpha-tACS administered over the parietal cortex improved target responses in retro-cue tasks in older participants (Borghini et al., 2018), whereas parietal alpha-tACS had no cognitive enhancement effect in young adults (Thompson et al., 2021). In a study that investigated the effect of beta, alpha, theta, and gamma tACS in young and middle-aged adults on forward memory span for digits, it was found that beta-tACS delivered over left posterior parietal cortex enhanced forward memory span for digits in young adults only (Feurra et al., 2016). Moreover, Misselhorn et al. (2020) examined the effect of beta-tACS and gamma-tACS administered over parietal cortex on delayed match-to-sample task outcomes in young and older adults, they found that gamma-tACS delayed the responses during the task, while beta-tACS speeded up the responses and that the effect was more pronounced in older adults. The details of the studies are summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

Cross Frequency Coupling Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation and Working Memory

The coupling between theta phase and gamma amplitude has been hypothesized as a mechanism underpinning the WM process (theta/gamma neural code) (Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014). The CFC-tACS (cross-frequency coupling-transcranial-alternating current stimulation) protocols have been investigated in several cognitive domains (e.g., WM, learning, long term memory) (Lara et al., 2018; Turi et al., 2020; Riddle et al., 2021). Turi et al. (2020) found that theta/gamma trough-coupled tACS in the frontal cortex impaired cognitive performance in a Go/NoGo instrumental learning task based on monetary reward and punishment. On the other hand, Riddle et al. (2021) showed that delta/beta peak-coupled tACS and theta/gamma peak-coupled tACS in prefrontal cortex modulated cognitive task outcomes. Lara et al. (2018) examined the effect of theta/gamma CFC-tACS during encoding of long-term verbal memory. The results of this study were consistent with Turi et al. (2020) results (Lara et al., 2018). With regards to WM, Alekseichuk et al. (2016) investigated the effect of theta/gamma cross-frequency tACS, which was adapted to the intrinsic continuous theta and repetitive gamma waves in the prefrontal cortex, on the WM performance. The results of the study showed that theta/gamma tACS had a greater benefit on WM performance than theta tACS alone. Gamma bursts above theta peaks significantly improved WM compared to gamma bursts above theta troughs, and the gamma frequencies associated with the optimal results were in the range of 80–100 Hz (Alekseichuk et al., 2016).

Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation and Working Memory in Patient Population

Transcranial alternating-current stimulation modulates cognitive processes in the brain by entraining endogenous oscillations and inducing long-lasting synaptic plasticity (Markram et al., 1997). tACS has only recently been introduced into psychiatric clinical trials and studied for its potential therapeutic role in the treatment of patients with schizophrenia (Hoy et al., 2016; Kallel et al., 2016; Sreeraj et al., 2017, 2019, 2020; Mellin et al., 2018; Ahn et al., 2019; Force et al., 2021), depression (Alexander et al., 2019; Wilkening et al., 2019; Riddle et al., 2020a), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Klimke et al., 2016), ADHD (Dallmer-Zerbe et al., 2020), Alzheimer’s disease (Benussi et al., 2021), and dementia (Naro et al., 2016; Moussavi et al., 2021). Abnormal gamma activity has been observed in various neuropsychiatric disorders (Herrmann and Demiralp, 2005; Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006). Therefore, the gamma-tACS effect on WM outcomes has been examined in patients with these disorders (Sreeraj et al., 2017; Haller et al., 2020b,a; Kehler et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2021). Specifically, the effect of tACS on WM outcomes in patients with neurological disorders has been investigated in the following disorders: schizophrenia 6 studies, summarized as follows: in-phase theta-tACS administered over fronto-parietal cortex (Sreeraj et al., 2017, 2019; Chang et al., 2021) improved WM outcomes and the effect was more sustained (50 days) when theta-tACS was administered daily (20 min/day) for 5 days. Contrary to expectations, no enhancement effect on memory was observed in two studies when single-session gamma-tACS was applied over the DLPFC (Hoy et al., 2016; Papazova et al., 2020), or when in-phase gamma-tACS was administered over the fronto-parietal cortex (Sreeraj et al., 2017). Whereas multiple sessions of gamma-tACS administered over several days produced positive cognitive results (Haller et al., 2020a). In patients with cognitive impairment and dementia (four studies), all studies investigating the effect of gamma-tACS delivered over the DLPFC or left angular gyrus showed an improvement in cognitive outcome from either single or multiple stimulation sessions, and the effect persisted for 1 month when multiple gamma-tACS sessions were applied over the DLPFC (30 min/day–5 days/week for 4 weeks) and combined with cognitive exercises (Kehler et al., 2020; Bréchet et al., 2021; Kim et al., 2021; Moussavi et al., 2021). In addition, the application of gamma-tACS over the prefrontal cortex for 10 days improved WM in patients with MDD (1 study) (Haller et al., 2020b). In a study conducted to evaluate the effect of tACS at 35 and 90 Hz on the visual and pitch memory outcomes in patients with congenital amusia, 35 Hz-tACS was found to selectively improve pitch memory in patients with congenital amusia (Schaal et al., 2015). The details of the studies are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summaries of the studies investigating the effect of frequency-tuned transcranial alternating-current stimulation on cognitive outcomes in unhealthy participants.

Interestingly, Bréchet et al. (2021) investigated the feasibility, efficacy, and safety of patient-tailored, home-based gamma-tACS delivered over the left angular gyrus and administered over 14 weeks (70 sessions in total) in two patients with Alzheimer’s disease-related dementia. In this study tACS sessions were delivered by trained caregivers and the administration can be remotely monitored by the study team. Results from this study suggest that both participants’ cognitive abilities improved from baseline on tests administered every 2 weeks during the 14 weeks of the study and that the overall protocol is safe and feasible. The results of the study provide an incentive for future randomized-controlled trials with large sample sizes to investigate the effect of tACS in a real-world setting (Bréchet et al., 2021). The details of the study are summarized in Table 2.

Discussion

Frequency-Tuned Non-invasive Brain Stimulation and Working Memory

Non-invasive brain stimulation, particularly frequency-tuned stimulation, is a recent approach that relies on the entrainment of endogenous oscillations to causally alter cognition and behavior. According to the dynamic systems theory, entrainment of the endogenous oscillations is strongest when the frequencies of the entraining stimulus match the ongoing brain frequencies (Ali et al., 2013; Vossen et al., 2015). The resonance phenomenon implies that matching the ongoing brain oscillations responsible for a given cognitive task with the frequency of the stimulus leads to an increase in the activity and coherence of these networks-neuronal synchronization — (Herman et al., 2013), which in turn leads to behavioral changes in activities supported by these neuronal networks (Borghini et al., 2018). rTMS and tACS have been used to modulate ongoing oscillation in a frequency-specific manner. In this review, we have focused on the effect of tACS on WM outcomes as it is less expensive, more tolerable, more portable, and safer compared to rTMS.

Cognitive deficits are evident in elderly and patients with schizophrenia, ADHD, MDD, MCI, and Alzheimer’s disease. Patients with dementia and Alzheimer’s disease are thought to have functional impairments in the central executive component of the WM model (Baddeley et al., 1991). Older adults are unable to optimally ignore task-irrelevant information, which is considered a fundamental step in WM processing (Borghini et al., 2018). Slowing, preventing, and reversing cognitive decline and dementia are challenging (Yao et al., 2020). Recent studies have tested the effects of lifestyle changes, cognitive training, exercise, and diet on cognitive functions, the results of the studies are mixed. Although a positive effect was found, large, well-conducted studies are needed to draw a conclusion (Bhatti et al., 2020). NIBS techniques induce long-lasting changes in cortical excitability by enhancing synaptic plasticity (Terranova et al., 2019). Responses to NIBS may vary depending on several factors, such as exercise, as regular aerobic exercise can modify plasticity and improve the response to NIBS (Kramer and Erickson, 2007). Aging alters the capacity of processes important for synaptic plasticity, so that NIBS-induced plasticity decreases with age in both healthy and neurologically impaired individuals (Müller-Dahlhaus et al., 2008). Furthermore, NIBS-induced plasticity can be significantly modulated by many neuropharmacological agents (Möhler, 2006; Ridding and Ziemann, 2010). Due to differences in brain dynamics and cortical activities, healthy participants and patients with neurological disorders respond differently to NIBS. Moreover, impaired long-term plasticity in patients with chronic diseases lead them to respond differently to NIBS aftereffects compared to patients with acute illnesses (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2012; Hoy et al., 2015, 2016; Dallmer-Zerbe et al., 2020).

Transcranial Alternating-Current Stimulation and Working Memory

The effect of frequency-tuned tACS on WM outcomes in healthy participants has been addressed in many studies (Santarnecchi et al., 2016; Tseng et al., 2016; Möller et al., 2017; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018), but a limited number of studies investigated the cognitive effects of tACS in the elderly, patients with neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders (Hoy et al., 2016; Kehler et al., 2020; Papazova et al., 2020). The effects of frequency-tuned tACS on WM are inconsistent, especially when tACS is applied to young, healthy participants or in a single-session (Hoy et al., 2015; Sreeraj et al., 2017; Papazova et al., 2020). Such a substantial diversity in results could be due to the inter-and intra-individual variability as well as differences in the experimental setups. Inter-individual variability could be partly due to individual differences in the theta/gamma ratio, theta and gamma frequencies, and the shape of the theta peak (sharp or broad), and participants’ baseline characteristics (e.g., low-performer versus high-performer to cognitive task administered prior to stimulation), elderly versus young, healthy versus unhealthy, etc. On the other flip, variability in task type and cognitive load explains intra-individual differences in sensitivity to the tACS effect (Wolinski et al., 2018). In addition, the relative release of sex hormones at different phases of the menstrual cycle has been suggested to influence women’s cognitive responses (Berman et al., 1997; Amin et al., 2006). Therefore, in two studies conducted by Jaušovec and Jaušovec (2014) and Jaušovec et al. (2014) the subjects participated in two sessions—a sham session and a tACS session. The sham and verum sessions were 28 days apart (on the same day of the menstrual cycle) to minimize the confounding effect of hormones on the measured cognitive outcomes. In addition, the cortical excitability of females is similar to males only during follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (Inghilleri et al., 2004). Thus, Fertonani et al. (2011) and Gonzalez-Perez et al. (2019) examined the effect of tACS in females only during this phase. However, the cognitive effects of sex hormones need to be studied in-depth before they can be considered as a factor influencing clinical outcomes. Differences in the experimental setups such as the number and duration of tACS sessions, stimulation parameters (frequency, intensity, duration, etc.) and the specific sub-region of the cortex that is stimulated are factors that influence the tACS entrainment effect (Liu et al., 2017).

The WM enhancement effect of tACS is more pronounced when applied over the prefrontal or parietal cortex in the theta or gamma frequency range (Hoy et al., 2015; Tseng et al., 2016; Sreeraj et al., 2019; Kehler et al., 2020) and when applied to participants performing tasks with relatively high cognitive demands (e.g., the enhancement effect was more pronounced in the 3-back task than in the 2-back task) (Hoy et al., 2015; Papazova et al., 2020). In addition, the cognitive effect of tACS is more evident in low-performers such as the elderly and patients with neurodegenerative diseases, who benefited from tACS administered either in one or in multiple sessions (Borghini et al., 2018; Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019; Kehler et al., 2020; Misselhorn et al., 2020; Bréchet et al., 2021; Moussavi et al., 2021). On the other hand, patients with schizophrenia or MDD may require multiple tACS sessions administered over several days to achieve cognitive benefits (Sreeraj et al., 2019; Haller et al., 2020a,b; Chang et al., 2021). The long-term cognitive effect (e.g., the effect lasting one month after stimulation) of tACS could be achieved by multiple tACS sessions administered over several days or weeks (e.g., 20 min/day for five consecutive days/week for 4 weeks) (Sreeraj et al., 2019; Chang et al., 2021; Moussavi et al., 2021). However, most tACS studies that have targeted neurological patients are case reports and case series conducted with small numbers of participants (Sreeraj et al., 2017, 2019; Haller et al., 2020a,b). Interestingly, there are currently several ongoing randomized, double-blind studies investigating the long-term safety and efficacy of tACS in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease (TRANSFORM-AD study) (Xing et al., 2020), depression after stroke (SPIRIT Compliant study) (Wang H. et al., 2020), and MDD (Wang H. X. et al., 2020; Huang et al., 2021). Follow-up in these studies will continue for up to several weeks after the end of stimulation (Wang H. et al., 2020; Wang H. X. et al., 2020; Xing et al., 2020; Huang et al., 2021). Among the tACS protocols that have recently gained popularity among researchers is the CFC-tACS. During cognitively demanding tasks, endogenous gamma bursts nest into theta peaks in the frontal cortex (Peak-coupled CFC) (Smith et al., 2015). Thus, manipulating this endogenous coupling between theta and gamma frequencies by the mean of tACS could affect WM outcomes. The causal role of CFC in cognitive processes has been investigated in several studies (Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Lara et al., 2018; Berger et al., 2019; Bramson et al., 2020; Hermiller et al., 2020; Turi et al., 2020). In the context of WM, the causal role of CFC was tested by Alekseichuk et al. (2016) by matching exogenously applied theta and gamma CFC-tACS to intrinsic continuous theta and repetitive gamma waves in the prefrontal cortices of healthy participants. They found that theta/gamma peak-coupled tACS enhanced WM more than theta-tACS alone, and the effect was more evident when gamma bursts (range of 80–100 Hz) were coincided into theta peaks (peak-coupled tACS) (Alekseichuk et al., 2016). The effect of CFC-tACS on cognitive functions varies depending on the brain area stimulated, the cognitive domain tested (WM, learning, long-term memory, etc.), the frequency bands targeted, and the experimental design used (Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Lara et al., 2018; Turi et al., 2020; Riddle et al., 2021). However, CFC-tACS needs further investigation in healthy individuals and patients with cognitive deficits due to its promising cognitive effects.

Future Directions and Limitations

The current studies have a number of limitations, summarized as follows: (1) Most studies currently reporting the cognitive effects of tACS are case reports, case series, and pilot studies conducted on a small number of participants, indeed, randomized, placebo-controlled, and double-blind studies on a large number of volunteers are needed to elucidate the WM enhancement effect of tACS on WM outcomes. (2) Only one study conducted by Bréchet et al. (2021) examined the effect of home-based tACS on cognitive outcomes in two patients with dementia. Although the effect of tACS on WM performance is promising, translating tACS effect to the real-world is challenging. This is because real-world conditions are dynamic, complex, and generally noisier than in the laboratory. However, mobile EEG devices that allow EEG recordings outside the laboratory would be beneficial, besides behavioral assessments, to evaluate the effect of tACS in realistic situations in future studies (Debener et al., 2015; Bleichner and Debener, 2017). (3) Most studies did not use personalized stimulation parameters. The use of personalized tACS parameters such as stimulation intensities and frequencies would lead to more effective stimulation (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014; Jaušovec et al., 2014; Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019) with minimal side effects (Thut et al., 2011b; Deng et al., 2013) and reduce the heterogeneity of results due to the inter-individual variability (section “tACS and Working Memory”). (4) There are a limited number of studies in the literature on the tACS effects on patients with neuropsychiatric disorders and cognitive deficits. (5) Lack of consistent aftereffect. Although multiple sessions of tACS delivered over several weeks may elicit long-lasting cognitive effects (e.g., at least 1-month post-stimulation), more and more studies are needed to replicate the results and determine the maximum duration of the cognitive effect.

Interestingly, all NIBS studies that targeted two regions in healthy older adults significantly improved WM outcomes (Park et al., 2014; Arciniega et al., 2018; Borghini et al., 2018; Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019), while those that targeted one region showed mixed results (Goldthorpe et al., 2020). Therefore, it is reasonable to call for studies targeting two regions, both in healthy participants and in patients with schizophrenia, depression, dementia, MCI, etc. using tACS.

In summary, recent studies suggest that participants with lower WM capacity due to advanced age or neurological disorders tend to derive greater cognitive benefit from tACS stimulation. These participants benefited from theta-tACS and/or gamma-tACS delivered over the prefrontal cortex. Schizophrenic patients may require multiple stimulation sessions to benefit from the cognitive effects of tACS. Elderly people and patients with neurodegenerative diseases, on the other hand, may benefit from one or more stimulation sessions. In addition, multiple tACS sessions may result in long-lasting cognitive improvement in participants with cognitive deficits. However, the current studies have been limited by small sample sizes, heterogeneous methods, inconsistent long-term effects, and so on. Further large sample sizes and well-designed studies are needed to confirm the cognitive effects of tACS and to determine optimal stimulation protocols for administration to patients with cognitive impairment. Comprehensive studies aimed at: (1) targeting more and more neuropsychiatric patients and patients with cognitive impairment; (2) achieving consistent aftereffects; (3) using personalized stimulation parameters; (4) translating the effects of tACS into realistic situations; (5) targeting two regions simultaneously (e.g., the frontal and parietal cortices); (6) investigating the effect of CFC-tACS on WM using different experimental setups are needed before frequency-tuned NIBS could alter cognitive impairments in everyday life.

Author Contributions

WA conducted the literature search and the summaries of previous published studies. MA and WA wrote the first and the final version of the manuscript. EK contributed to the progress of the manuscript and provided guidance with expert perspective. All authors contributed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.822545/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

WM, working memory; STM, short-term memory; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; Hz, hertz; CFC, cross-frequency coupling; PAC, phase-amplitude coupling; NIBS, non-invasive brain stimulation; tES, transcranial electrical stimulation; TMS, transcranial magnetic stimulation; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; tRNS, transcranial random noise stimulation; rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; TBS, theta burst stimulation; cTBS, continuous theta burst stimulation; iTBS, intermittent theta burst stimulation; FDA, Food and Drug Administration; STDP, spike-timing-dependent plasticity; EEG, electroencephalography; EPR, event related potential; MEG, magnetencephalography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; CFC-tACS, cross-frequency coupling- transcranial alternating current stimulation.

References

Abellaneda-Pérez, K., Vaqué-Alcázar, L., Perellón-Alfonso, R., Bargalló, N., Kuo, M.-F., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2020). Differential tDCS and tACS Effects on Working Memory-Related Neural Activity and Resting-State Connectivity. Front. Neurosci. 13:1440. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2019.01440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adrian, E. D., and Matthews, B. H. C. (1934). The interpretation of potential waves in the cortex. J. Physiol. 81, 440–471. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1934.sp003147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahn, S., Mellin, J. M., Alagapan, S., Alexander, M. L., Gilmore, J. H., Jarskog, L. F., et al. (2019). Targeting reduced neural oscillations in patients with schizophrenia by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Neuroimage 186, 126–136. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.10.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alekseichuk, I., Pabel, S. C., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2017). Intrahemispheric theta rhythm desynchronization impairs working memory. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 35, 147–158. doi: 10.3233/RNN-160714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alekseichuk, I., Turi, Z., Amador de Lara, G., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2016). Spatial Working Memory in Humans Depends on Theta and High Gamma Synchronization in the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr. Biol. 26, 1513–1521. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, M. L., Alagapan, S., Lugo, C. E., Mellin, J. M., Lustenberger, C., Rubinow, D. R., et al. (2019). Double-blind, randomized pilot clinical trial targeting alpha oscillations with transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) for the treatment of major depressive disorder (MDD). Transl. Psychiatry 9, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0439-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, M. M., Sellers, K. K., and Fröhlich, F. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates large-scale cortical network activity by network resonance. J. Neurosci. 33, 11262–11275. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5867-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amin, Z., Epperson, C. N., Constable, R. T., and Canli, T. (2006). Effects of estrogen variation on neural correlates of emotional response inhibition. Neuroimage 32, 457–464. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2006.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amzica, F., and Steriade, M. (1998). Electrophysiological correlates of sleep delta waves. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 107, 69–83. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00051-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, R. A., and Cui, H. (2009). Intention, Action Planning, and Decision Making in Parietal-Frontal Circuits. Neuron 63, 568–583. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2009.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Transcranial Alternating Current and Random Noise Stimulation: possible Mechanisms. Neural Plast. 2016:3616807. doi: 10.1155/2016/3616807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arciniega, H., Gözenman, F., Jones, K. T., Stephens, J. A., and Berryhill, M. E. (2018). Frontoparietal tDCS Benefits Visual Working Memory in Older Adults With Low Working Memory Capacity. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:57. doi: 10.3389/FNAGI.2018.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Henseler, M. M., Jensen, O., Weinreich, I., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2010). Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 3228–3233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911531107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, B. J., and Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 166–172. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00056-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working Memory: theories, Models, and Controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., and Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study. Brain 114, 2521–2542. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.6.2521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. Adv. Res. Theory 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahramisharif, A., Jensen, O., Jacobs, J., and Lisman, J. (2018). Serial representation of items during working memory maintenance at letter-selective cortical sites. PLoS Biol. 16:e2003805. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2003805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M. (2013). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, working memory and episodic memory processes: insight through transcranial magnetic stimulation techniques. Neurosci. Bull. 29, 381–389. doi: 10.1007/S12264-013-1309-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barch, D. M., Sheline, Y. I., Csernansky, J. G., and Snyder, A. Z. (2003). Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: specificity to schizophrenia compared with major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 376–384. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01674-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., and Camos, V. (2004). Time Constraints and Resource Sharing in Adults’ Working Memory Spans. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 83–100. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrouillet, P., Gavens, N., Vergauwe, E., Gaillard, V., and Camos, V. (2009). Working Memory Span Development: a Time-Based Resource-Sharing Model Account. Dev. Psychol. 45, 477–490. doi: 10.1037/A0014615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, M., Romei, V., and Sauseng, P. (2019). Slow Theta tACS of the Right Parietal Cortex Enhances Contralateral Visual Working Memory Capacity. Brain Topogr. 32, 477–481. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00702-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benussi, A., Cantoni, V., Cotelli, M. S., Cotelli, M., Brattini, C., Datta, A., et al. (2021). Exposure to gamma tACS in Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled, crossover, pilot study. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodulation 14, 531–540. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2021.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, B., Griesmayr, B., Minarik, T., Biel, A. L., Pinal, D., Sterr, A., et al. (2019). Dynamic regulation of interregional cortical communication by slow brain oscillations during working memory. Nat. Commun. 101:4242. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12057-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, K. F., Schmidt, P. J., Rubinow, D. R., Danaceau, M. A., Van Horn, J. D., Esposito, G., et al. (1997). Modulation of cognition-specific cortical activity by gonadal steroids: a positron-emission tomography study?in?women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 94, 8836–8841. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.94.16.8836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhatti, G. K., Reddy, A. P., Reddy, P. H., and Bhatti, J. S. (2020). Lifestyle Modifications and Nutritional Interventions in Aging-Associated Cognitive Decline and Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:369. doi: 10.3389/FNAGI.2019.00369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bland, N. S., and Sale, M. V. (2019). Current challenges: the ups and downs of tACS. Exp. Brain Res. 237, 3071–3088. doi: 10.1007/s00221-019-05666-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleichner, M. G., and Debener, S. (2017). Concealed, Unobtrusive Ear-Centered EEG Acquisition: cEEGrids for Transparent EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:163. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2017.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boes, A. D., Kelly, M. S., Trapp, N. T., Stern, A. P., Press, D. Z., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2018). Noninvasive Brain Stimulation: challenges and Opportunities for a New Clinical Specialty. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 30, 173–179. doi: 10.1176/APPI.NEUROPSYCH.17110262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogdanov, M., and Schwabe, L. (2016). Transcranial stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex prevents stress-induced working memory deficits. J. Neurosci. 36, 1429–1437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3687-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnefond, M., and Jensen, O. (2012). Alpha oscillations serve to protect working memory maintenance against anticipated distracters. Curr. Biol. 22, 1969–1974. doi: 10.1016/J.CUB.2012.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borchers, S., Himmelbach, M., Logothetis, N., and Karnath, H. O. (2012). Direct electrical stimulation of human cortex-the gold standard for mapping brain functions? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 63–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn3140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghini, G., Candini, M., Filannino, C., Hussain, M., Walsh, V., Romei, V., et al. (2018). Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing. J. Neurosci. 38, 4418–4429. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1285-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bramson, B., den Ouden, H. E. M., Toni, I., and Roelofs, K. (2020). Improving emotional-action control by targeting long-range phase-amplitude neuronal coupling. Elife 9, 1–19. doi: 10.7554/ELIFE.59600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Yu, W., Biagi, M. C., Ruffini, G., Gagnon, M., Manor, B., et al. (2021). Patient-Tailored, Home-Based Non-invasive Brain Stimulation for Memory Deficits in Dementia Due to Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Neurol. 12:598135. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.598135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruns, A., and Eckhorn, R. (2004). Task-related coupling from high- to low-frequency signals among visual cortical areas in human subdural recordings. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 51, 97–116. doi: 10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bueno-Lopez, A., Eggert, T., Dorn, H., and Danker-Hopfe, H. (2019). Slow oscillatory transcranial direct current stimulation (so-tDCS) during slow wave sleep has no effects on declarative memory in healthy young subjects. Brain Stimul. 12, 948–958. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canolty, R. T., Edwards, E., Dalal, S. S., Soltani, M., Nagarajan, S. S., Kirsch, H. E., et al. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cebolla, A. M., and Cheron, G. (2019). Understanding neural oscillations in the human brain: from movement to consciousness and vice versa. Front. Psychol. 10:1930. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chai, W. J., Abd Hamid, A. I., and Abdullah, J. M. (2018). Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: a review. Front. Psychol. 9:401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chander, B. S., Witkowski, M., Braun, C., Robinson, S. E., Born, J., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2016). tACS Phase Locking of Frontal Midline Theta Oscillations Disrupts Working Memory Performance. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 10:120. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C.-C., Chia-Yu Huang, C., Chung, Y.-A., Jamie Im, J., Lin, Y.-Y., Ma, C.-C., et al. (2021). Online Left-Hemispheric In-Phase Frontoparietal Theta tACS for the Treatment of Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia. J. Pers. Med. 11:1114. doi: 10.3390/JPM11111114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Prog. Brain Res. 169:323. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00020-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallmer-Zerbe, I., Popp, F., Lam, A. P., Philipsen, A., and Herrmann, C. S. (2020). Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) as a Tool to Modulate P300 Amplitude in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): preliminary Findings. Brain Topogr. 33, 191–207. doi: 10.1007/s10548-020-00752-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daneman, M., and Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 19, 450–466. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, E., Censor, N., Buch, E. R., Sandrini, M., and Cohen, L. G. (2013). Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 838–844. doi: 10.1038/nn.3422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debener, S., Emkes, R., De Vos, M., and Bleichner, M. (2015). Unobtrusive ambulatory EEG using a smartphone and flexible printed electrodes around the ear. Sci. Rep. 5:16743. doi: 10.1038/SREP16743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Z.-D., Lisanby, S. H., and Peterchev, A. V. (2013). Electric field depth–focality tradeoff in transcranial magnetic stimulation: simulation comparison of 50 coil designs. Brain Stimul. 6, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2012.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 761–772. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M., and Postle, B. R. (2015). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 115–142. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV-PSYCH-010814-015031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C. (2000). Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 813–829. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01020-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunning, D. L., Westgate, B., and Adlam, A. L. R. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in survivors of moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 30, 811–819. doi: 10.1037/neu0000285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elyamany, O., Leicht, G., Herrmann, C. S., and Mulert, C. (2021). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): from basic mechanisms towards first applications in psychiatry. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 271, 135–156. doi: 10.1007/S00406-020-01209-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fertonani, A., Pirulli, C., and Miniussi, C. (2011). Random Noise Stimulation Improves Neuroplasticity in Perceptual Learning. J. Neurosci. 31, 15416–15423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2002-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feurra, M., Galli, G., Pavone, E. F., Rossi, A., and Rossi, S. (2016). Frequency-specific insight into short-term memory capacity. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 153–158. doi: 10.1152/jn.01080.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Force, R. B., Riddle, J., Jarskog, L. F., and Fröhlich, F. (2021). A case study of the feasibility of weekly tACS for the treatment of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodulation 14, 361–363. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2021.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fregni, F., Simon, D. K., Wu, A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Non-invasive brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 1614–1623. doi: 10.1136/JNNP.2005.069849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fröhlich, F., Sellers, K. K., and Cordle, A. L. (2015). Targeting the neurophysiology of cognitive systems with transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Expert Rev. Neurother. 15:145. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2015.992782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevins, A., Smith, M. E., McEvoy, L., and Yu, D. (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cereb. Cortex 7, 374–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.4.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsworthy, M. R., Rogasch, N. C., Ballinger, S., Graetz, L., Van Dam, J. M., Harris, R., et al. (2020). Age-related decline of neuroplasticity to intermittent theta burst stimulation of the lateral prefrontal cortex and its relationship with late-life memory performance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 2181–2191. doi: 10.1016/J.CLINPH.2020.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldthorpe, R. A., Rapley, J. M., and Violante, I. R. (2020). A Systematic Review of Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Applications to Memory in Healthy Aging. Front. Neurol. 11:575075. doi: 10.3389/FNEUR.2020.575075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Perez, M., Wakui, E., Thoma, V., Nitsche, M. A., and Rivolta, D. (2019). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at 40?Hz enhances face and object perception. Neuropsychologia 135:107237. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2019.107237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J., Schmitz, F., Schnitzler, I., Kessler, K., Shapiro, K., Hommel, B., et al. (2004). Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 13050–13055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0404944101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grot, S., Légaré, V. P., Lipp, O., Soulières, I., Dolcos, F., and Luck, D. (2017). Abnormal prefrontal and parietal activity linked to deficient active binding in working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 188, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X., Li, Z., Zhang, L., and Liu, Q. (2021). Modulation of Visual Working Memory Performance via Different Theta Frequency Stimulations. Brain Sci. 11:1358. doi: 10.3390/BRAINSCI11101358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, N., Hasan, A., Padberg, F., Brunelin, J., da Costa Lane Valiengo, L., and Palm, U. (2020a). Gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation in patients with negative symptoms in schizophrenia: a case series. Neurophysiol. Clin. 50, 301–304. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUCLI.2020.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, N., Senner, F., Brunoni, A. R., Padberg, F., and Palm, U. (2020b). Gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation improves mood and cognition in patients with major depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 130, 31–34. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2020.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Axmacher, N., and Inman, C. S. (2019). Modulating Human Memory via Entrainment of Brain Oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 42, 485–499. doi: 10.1016/J.TINS.2019.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haque, Z. Z., Samandra, R., and Mansouri, F. A. (2021). Neural substrate and underlying mechanisms of working memory: insights from brain stimulation studies. J. Neurophysiol. 126, 2038–2053. doi: 10.1152/JN.00041.2021/ASSET/IMAGES/MEDIUM/JN-00041-2021R01.PNG

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmony, T., Fernández, T., Silva, J., Bernal, J., Díaz-Comas, L., Reyes, A., et al. (1996). EEG delta activity: an indicator of attention to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 161–171. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00053-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, P. A., Lundqvist, M., and Lansner, A. (2013). Nested theta to gamma oscillations and precise spatiotemporal firing during memory retrieval in a simulated attractor network. Brain Res. 1536, 68–87. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermiller, M. S., Chen, Y. F., Parrish, T. B., and Voss, J. L. (2020). Evidence for Immediate Enhancement of Hippocampal Memory Encoding by Network-Targeted Theta-Burst Stimulation during Concurrent fMRI. J. Neurosci. 40, 7155–7168. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0486-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., and Demiralp, T. (2005). Human EEG gamma oscillations in neuropsychiatric disorders. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2719–2733. doi: 10.1016/J.CLINPH.2005.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, L. E., Buchanan, R. W., Thaker, G. K., Shepard, P. D., and Summerfelt, A. (2008). Beta (∼16 Hz) frequency neural oscillations mediate auditory sensory gating in humans. Psychophysiology 45, 197–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00624.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honkanen, R., Rouhinen, S., Wang, S. H., Palva, J. M., and Palva, S. (2015). Gamma oscillations underlie the maintenance of feature-specific information and the contents of visual working memory. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3788–3801. doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHU263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N., Arnold, S., Windsor, K., John, J., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2015). The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls. Brain Cogn. 101, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Whitty, D., Bailey, N., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2016). Preliminary investigation of the effects of γ -tACS on working memory in schizophrenia. J. Neural Transm. 123, 1205–1212. doi: 10.1007/S00702-016-1554-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Shen, L., Huang, J., Xu, X., Wang, Y., and Jin, H. (2021). Efficacy and Safety of tDCS and tACS in Treatment of Major Depressive Disorder: a Randomized, Double-Blind, Factorial Placebo-Controlled Study Design. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 17:1459. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S295945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inghilleri, M., Conte, A., Currà, A., Frasca, V., Lorenzano, C., and Berardelli, A. (2004). Ovarian hormones and cortical excitability. An rTMS study in humans. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1063–1068. doi: 10.1016/J.CLINPH.2003.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N., and Jaušovec, K. (2014). Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Biol. Psychol. 96, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N., Jaušovec, K., and Pahor, A. (2014). The influence of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions. Acta Psychol. 146, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeffries, S., and Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: its role in dyslexia and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia 10, 196–214. doi: 10.1002/DYS.278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Colgin, L. L. (2007). Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 267–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Lisman, J. E. (1996). Novel lists of 7±2 known items can be reliably stored in an oscillatory short-term memory network: interaction with long-term memory. Learn. Mem. 3, 257–263. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.2-3.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., Spaak, E., and Zumer, J. M. (2019). ““Human Brain Oscillations: from Physiological Mechanisms to Analysis and Cognition,” in Magnetoencephalography, eds S. Supek and C. Aine (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 1–46. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-62657-4_17-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jirsa, V., and Müller, V. (2013). Cross-frequency coupling in real and virtual brain networks. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:78. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Arciniega, H., and Berryhill, M. E. (2019). Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits. Brain Res. 1720:146324. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2019.146324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kallel, L., Mondino, M., and Brunelin, J. (2016). Effects of theta-rhythm transcranial alternating current stimulation (4.5 Hz-tACS) in patients with clozapine-resistant negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a case series. J. Neural Transm. 123, 1213–1217. doi: 10.1007/S00702-016-1574-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kehler, L., Francisco, C. O., Uehara, M. A., and Moussavi, Z. (2020). “The effect of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on cognitive function in older adults with dementia,” in Proceedings of the Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, EMBS, (Montreal, QC, Canada: Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers Inc), 3649–3653. doi: 10.1109/EMBC44109.2020.9175903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., Calhoun, V. D., and Clark, V. P. (2015). The role of the frontopolar cortex in manipulation of integrated information in working memory. Neurosci. Lett. 595, 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.03.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., Kim, H., Jeong, H., Roh, D., and Kim, D. H. (2021). tACS as a promising therapeutic option for improving cognitive function in mild cognitive impairment: a direct comparison between tACS and tDCS. J. Psychiatr. Res. 141, 248–256. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2021.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinert, M. L., Szymanski, C., and Müller, V. (2017). Frequency-unspecific effects of θ-tACS related to a visuospatial working memory task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:367. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1997). EEG-alpha rhythms and memory processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 26, 319–340. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00773-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Freunberger, R., and Sauseng, P. (2010). Oscillatory mechanisms of process binding in memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1002–1014. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimke, A., Nitsche, M. A., Maurer, K., and Voss, U. (2016). Case Report: successful Treatment of Therapy-Resistant OCD With Application of Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS). Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodulation 9, 463–465. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2016.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knyazev, G. G. (2007). Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 377–395. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopell, N., Ermentrout, G. B., Whittington, M. A., and Traub, R. D. (2000). Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 1867–1872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., and Erickson, K. I. (2007). Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 342–348. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2007.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, M.-F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of Transcranial Electrical Stimulation on Cognition. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 43, 192–199. doi: 10.1177/1550059412444975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Shah, A. S., Knuth, K. H., Ulbert, I., Karmos, G., and Schroeder, C. E. (2005). An Oscillatory Hierarchy Controlling Neuronal Excitability and Stimulus Processing in the Auditory Cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1904–1911. doi: 10.1152/JN.00263.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lara, G. A. D., Alekseichuk, I., Turi, Z., Lehr, A., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2018). Perturbation of theta-gamma coupling at the temporal lobe hinders verbal declarative memory. Brain Stimul. 11, 509–517. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2017.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, T. M., Borghi, J. A., Kujawa, A. J., Klein, D. N., and Leung, H. C. (2017). Alterations in visual cortical activation and connectivity with prefrontal cortex during working memory updating in major depressive disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 14, 43–53. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2013). Electrical engram: how deep brain stimulation affects memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 574–584. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Idiart, M. A. P. (1995). Storage of 7 ± 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267, 1512–1515. doi: 10.1126/science.7878473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Jensen, O. (2013). The Theta-Gamma Neural Code. Neuron 77, 1002–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Leng, Y. S., Zou, X. H., Cheng, Z. Q., Yang, W., and Li, B. J. (2017). Affective processing in non-invasive brain stimulation over prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:439. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logie, R. H. (2011). The Functional Organization and Capacity Limits of Working Memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 240–245. doi: 10.1177/0963721411415340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maehler, C., and Schuchardt, K. (2016). Working memory in children with specific learning disorders and/or attention deficits. Learn. Individ. Differ. 49, 341–347. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markram, H., Lübke, J., Frotscher, M., and Sakmann, B. (1997). Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence of postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science 275, 213–215. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., Helgadóttir, H., Mölle, M., and Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444, 610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiron, O., and Lavidor, M. (2014). Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/J.CLINPH.2013.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellin, J. M., Alagapan, S., Lustenberger, C., Lugo, C. E., Alexander, M. L., Gilmore, J. H., et al. (2018). Randomized trial of transcranial alternating current stimulation for treatment of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry 51, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/J.EURPSY.2018.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishra, B. R., Sarkar, S., Praharaj, S. K., Mehta, V. S., Diwedi, S., and Haque Nizamie, S. (2011). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in psychiatry. Ann. Indian Acad. Neurol. 14, 245–251. doi: 10.4103/0972-2327.91935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misselhorn, J., Göschl, F., Higgen, F. L., Hummel, F. C., Gerloff, C., and Engel, A. K. (2020). Sensory capability and information integration independently explain the cognitive status of healthy older adults. Sci. Rep. 101:22437. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-80069-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möhler, H. (2006). GABAA receptor diversity and pharmacology. Cell Tissue Res. 326, 505–516. doi: 10.1007/S00441-006-0284-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möller, A., Nemmi, F., Karlsson, K., and Klingberg, T. (2017). Transcranial Electric Stimulation Can Impair Gains during Working Memory Training and Affects the Resting State Connectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:364. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2017.00364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, A. B., Li, Z., Tyner, C. E., Hu, X., and Crosson, B. (2013). Bilateral basal ganglia activity in verbal working memory. Brain Lang. 125, 316–323. doi: 10.1016/J.BANDL.2012.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormann, F., Fell, J., Axmacher, N., Weber, B., Lehnertz, K., Elger, C. E., et al. (2005). Phase/amplitude reset and theta–gamma interaction in the human medial temporal lobe during a continuous word recognition memory task. Hippocampus 15, 890–900. doi: 10.1002/HIPO.20117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moussavi, Z., Kimura, K., Kehler, L., de Oliveira Francisco, C., and Lithgow, B. (2021). A Novel Program to Improve Cognitive Function in Individuals With Dementia Using Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) and Tutored Cognitive Exercises. Front. Aging 2:632545. doi: 10.3389/FRAGI.2021.632545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller-Dahlhaus, J. F. M., Orekhov, Y., Liu, Y., and Ziemann, U. (2008). Interindividual variability and age-dependency of motor cortical plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 187, 467–475. doi: 10.1007/S00221-008-1319-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naro, A., Corallo, F., De Salvo, S., Marra, A., Di Lorenzo, G., Muscarà, N., et al. (2016). Promising Role of Neuromodulation in Predicting the Progression of Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 53, 1375–1388. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, B. S. W., Logothetis, N. K., and Kayser, C. (2013). EEG phase patterns reflect the selectivity of neural firing. Cereb. Cortex 23, 389–398. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osaka, M., Osaka, N., Kondo, H., Morishita, M., Fukuyama, H., Aso, T., et al. (2003). The neural basis of individual differences in working memory capacity: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 18, 789–797. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00032-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25:46. doi: 10.1002/HBM.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahor, A., and Jaušovec, N. (2018). The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:651. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, J. M., Palva, S., and Kaila, K. (2005). Phase Synchrony among Neuronal Oscillations in the Human Cortex. J. Neurosci. 25, 3962–3972. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4250-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, S., Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., Näätänen, R., and Palva, J. M. (2005). Early neural correlates of conscious somatosensory perception. J. Neurosci. 25, 5248–5258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0141-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papazova, I., Strube, W., Hoffmann, L., Schwippel, T., Padberg, F., Palm, U., et al. (2020). T54. Effects of gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on working memory in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Bull. 46, S251–S252. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa029.614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S. H., Seo, J. H., Kim, Y. H., and Ko, M. H. (2014). Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport 25, 122–126. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, W., Nitsche, M. A., and Antal, A. (2016). Application of transcranial electric stimulation (tDCS, tACS, tRNS): from motor-evoked potentials towards modulation of behaviour. Eur. Psychol. 21, 4–14. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., Andrew, C., and Edlinger, G. (1997). Foot and hand area mu rhythms. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 26, 121–135. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00760-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, N. L., Parry, L., Mandalis, A., and Lah, S. (2017). Working memory outcomes following traumatic brain injury in children: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Child Neuropsychol. 23, 26–66. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1085500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., Korman, C., Batsikadze, G., and Paulus, W. (2012). The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr. Biol. 22, 1314–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., and Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popov, T., Jensen, O., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2018). Dorsal and ventral cortices are coupled by cross-frequency interactions during working memory. Neuroimage 178, 277–286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozdniakov, I., Vorobiova, A. N., Galli, G., Rossi, S., and Feurra, M. (2021). Online and offline effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Sci. Rep. 11:3854. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83449-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghavachari, S., Lisman, J. E., Tully, M., Madsen, J. R., Bromfield, E. B., and Kahana, M. J. (2006). Theta oscillations in human cortex during a working-memory task: evidence for local generators. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 1630–1638. doi: 10.1152/jn.00409.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., Hecht, S. A., and Barnes, M. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: a review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinhart, R. M. G., and Nguyen, J. A. (2019). Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 820–827. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0371-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RepovŠ, G., and Baddeley, A. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 139, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2005.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridding, M. C., and Ziemann, U. (2010). Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects. J. Physiol. 588:2291. doi: 10.1113/JPHYSIOL.2010.190314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddle, J., McFerren, A., and Frohlich, F. (2021). Causal role of cross-frequency coupling in distinct components of cognitive control. Prog. Neurobiol. 202:102033. doi: 10.1016/J.PNEUROBIO.2021.102033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddle, J., Scimeca, J. M., Cellier, D., Dhanani, S., and D’Esposito, M. (2020b). Causal Evidence for a Role of Theta and Alpha Oscillations in the Control of Working Memory. Curr. Biol. 30, 1748–1754.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddle, J., Rubinow, D. R., and Frohlich, F. (2020a). A case study of weekly tACS for the treatment of major depressive disorder. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodulation 13, 576–577. doi: 10.1016/J.BRS.2019.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva-Posse, P., Choi, K. S., Holtzheimer, P. E., Crowell, A. L., Garlow, S. J., Rajendra, J. K., et al. (2018). A connectomic approach for subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation surgery: prospective targeting in treatment-resistant depression. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 843–849. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röhner, F., Breitling, C., Rufener, K. S., Heinze, H.-J., Hinrichs, H., Krauel, K., et al. (2018). Modulation of Working Memory Using Transcranial Electrical Stimulation: a Direct Comparison Between TACS and TDCS. Front. Neurosci. 12:761. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2018.00761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romo, R., and Salinas, E. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 203–218. doi: 10.1038/nrn1058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., et al. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1071–1107. doi: 10.1016/J.CLINPH.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F., and Uhlhaas, P. J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahlem, G. L., Badran, B. W., Halford, J. J., Williams, N. R., Korte, J. E., Leslie, K., et al. (2015). Oscillating square wave transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) delivered during slow wave sleep does not improve declarative memory more than sham: a randomized sham controlled crossover study. Brain Stimul. 8, 528–534. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahu, P. P., and Tseng, P. (2021). Frontoparietal theta tACS nonselectively enhances encoding, maintenance, and retrieval stages in visuospatial working memory. Neurosci. Res. 172, 41–50. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURES.2021.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Muller, T., Rossi, S., Sarkar, A., Polizzotto, N. R., Rossi, A., et al. (2016). Individual differences and specificity of prefrontal gamma frequency-tACS on fluid intelligence capabilities. Cortex 75, 33–43. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Polizzotto, N. R., Godone, M., Giovannelli, F., Feurra, M., Matzen, L., et al. (2013). Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials. Curr. Biol. 23, 1449–1453. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Heise, K. F., Gruber, W. R., Holz, E., Karim, A. A., et al. (2009). Brain Oscillatory Substrates of Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity. Curr. Biol. 19, 1846–1852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Peylo, C., Biel, A. L., Friedrich, E. V. C., and Romberg-Taylor, C. (2019). Does cross-frequency phase coupling of oscillatory brain activity contribute to a better understanding of visual working memory? Br. J. Psychol. 110, 245–255. doi: 10.1111/BJOP.12340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaal, N. K., Pfeifer, J., Krause, V., and Pollok, B. (2015). From amusic to musical?-Improving pitch memory in congenital amusia with transcranial alternating current stimulation. Behav. Brain Res. 294, 141–148. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schack, B., Vath, N., Petsche, H., Geissler, H. G., and Möller, E. (2002). Phase-coupling of theta–gamma EEG rhythms during short-term memory processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 143–163. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00199-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seager, M. A., Johnson, L. D., Chabot, E. S., Asaka, Y., and Berry, S. D. (2002). Oscillatory brain states and learning: impact of hippocampal theta-contingent training. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 99, 1616–1620. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.032662099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siebenhühner, F., Wang, S. H., Arnulfo, G., Lampinen, A., Nobili, L., Palva, J. M., et al. (2020). Genuine cross-frequency coupling networks in human resting-state electrophysiological recordings. PLoS Biol. 18:e3000685. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Donner, T. H., and Engel, A. K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirota, A., Montgomery, S., Fujisawa, S., Isomura, Y., Zugaro, M., and Buzsáki, G. (2008). Entrainment of Neocortical Neurons and Gamma Oscillations by the Hippocampal Theta Rhythm. Neuron 60, 683–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. H., Banks, G. P., Mikell, C. B., Cash, S. S., Patel, S. R., Eskandar, E. N., et al. (2015). Frequency-Dependent Representation of Reinforcement-Related Information in the Human Medial and Lateral Prefrontal Cortex. J. Neurosci. 35:15827. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1864-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sotero, R. C. (2016). Topology, Cross-Frequency, and Same-Frequency Band Interactions Shape the Generation of Phase-Amplitude Coupling in a Neural Mass Model of a Cortical Column. PLoS Comput. Biol. 12:1005180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreeraj, V. S., Shanbhag, V., Nawani, H., Shivakumar, V., Damodharan, D., Bose, A., et al. (2017). Feasibility of online neuromodulation using transcranial alternating current stimulation in schizophrenia. Indian J. Psychol. Med. 39, 92–95. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.198937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreeraj, V. S., Shivakumar, V., Sowmya, S., Bose, A., Nawani, H., Narayanaswamy, J. C., et al. (2019). Online theta frequency transcranial alternating current stimulation for cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: a case report and review of literature. J. ECT 35, 139–143. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreeraj, V. S., Suhas, S., Parlikar, R., Selvaraj, S., Dinakaran, D., Shivakumar, V., et al. (2020). Effect of add-on transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on persistent delusions in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 290:113106. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2020.113106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stegmayer, K., Usher, J., Trost, S., Henseler, I., Tost, H., Rietschel, M., et al. (2015). Disturbed cortico–amygdalar functional connectivity as pathophysiological correlate of working memory deficits in bipolar affective disorder. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 265, 303–311. doi: 10.1007/s00406-014-0517-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Peronnet, F., and Pernier, J. (1998). Induced γ-band activity during the delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. J. Neurosci. 18, 4244–4254. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-11-04244.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavakoli, A. V., and Yun, K. (2017). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) mechanisms and protocols. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11:214. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terranova, C., Rizzo, V., Cacciola, A., Chillemi, G., Calamuneri, A., Milardi, D., et al. (2019). Is there a future for non-invasive brain stimulation as a therapeutic tool? Front. Neurol. 10:1146. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.01146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, L., Khuc, J., Saccani, M. S., Zokaei, N., and Cappelletti, M. (2021). Gamma oscillations modulate working memory recall precision. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 2711–2724. doi: 10.1007/S00221-021-06051-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., Veniero, D., Romei, V., Miniussi, C., Schyns, P., and Gross, J. (2011b). Rhythmic TMS causes local entrainment of natural oscillatory signatures. Curr. Biol. 21, 1176–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., Schyns, P. G., and Gross, J. (2011a). Entrainment of Perceptually Relevant Brain Oscillations by Non-Invasive Rhythmic Stimulation of the Human Brain. Front. Psychol. 2:170. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, P., Chang, Y. T., Chang, C. F., Liang, W. K., and Juan, C. H. (2016). The critical role of phase difference in gamma oscillation within the temporoparietal network for binding visual working memory. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–15. doi: 10.1038/srep32138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, P., Iu, K. C., and Juan, C. H. (2018). The critical role of phase difference in theta oscillation between bilateral parietal cortices for visuospatial working memory. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18449-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsujimoto, T., Shimazu, H., and Isomura, Y. (2006). Direct recording of theta oscillations in primate prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2987–3000. doi: 10.1152/jn.00730.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turi, Z., Mittner, M., Lehr, A., Bürger, H., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2020). θ−γ Cross-Frequency Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation over the Trough Impairs Cognitive Control. eNeuro 7, 1–12. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0126-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., Haenschel, C., Nikolić, D., and Singer, W. (2008). The Role of Oscillations and Synchrony in Cortical Networks and Their Putative Relevance for the Pathophysiology of Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34:927. doi: 10.1093/SCHBUL/SBN062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2006). Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52, 155–168. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2006.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2012). Neuronal Dynamics and Neuropsychiatric Disorders: toward a Translational Paradigm for Dysfunctional Large-Scale Networks. Neuron 75, 963–980. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valero-Cabré, A., Amengual, J. L., Stengel, C., Pascual-Leone, A., and Coubard, O. A. (2017). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in basic and clinical neuroscience: a comprehensive review of fundamental principles and novel insights. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 381–404. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vugt, M. K., Chakravarthi, R., and Lachaux, J.-P. (2014). For whom the bell tolls: periodic reactivation of sensory cortex in the gamma band as a substrate of visual working memory maintenance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:696. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, O., Jobidon, M. E., Bouak, F., Nakashima, A., Smith, I., Lam, Q., et al. (2013). Working memory training is associated with lower prefrontal cortex activation in a divergent thinking task. Neuroscience 236, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2012.12.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veniero, D., Vossen, A., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2015). Lasting EEG/MEG aftereffects of rhythmic transcranial brain stimulation: level of control over oscillatory network activity. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 9:477. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vertes, R. P. (2005). Hippocampal theta rhythm: a tag for short-term memory. Hippocampus 15, 923–935. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Violante, I. R., Li, L. M., Carmichael, D. W., Lorenz, R., Leech, R., Hampshire, A., et al. (2017). Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. Elife 6:e22001. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossen, A., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2015). Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (a-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 8, 499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2015). Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Strüber, D., and Herrmann, C. S. (2018). Non-invasive Brain Stimulation: a Paradigm Shift in Understanding Brain Oscillations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:211. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H. X., Wang, K., Zhang, W. R., Zhao, W. F., Yang, X. T., Wang, L., et al. (2020). Protocol on transcranial alternating current stimulation for the treatment of major depressive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Chin. Med. J. 133, 61–67. doi: 10.1097/CM9.0000000000000589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Zhang, W., Zhao, W., Wang, K., Wang, Z., Wang, L., et al. (2020). The efficacy of transcranial alternating current stimulation for treating post-stroke depression: study Protocol Clinical Trial (SPIRIT Compliant). Medicine 99:e19671. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000019671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittingstall, K., and Logothetis, N. K. (2009). Frequency-Band Coupling in Surface EEG Reflects Spiking Activity in Monkey Visual Cortex. Neuron 64, 281–289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widhalm, M. L., and Rose, N. S. (2019). How can transcranial magnetic stimulation be used to causally manipulate memory representations in the human brain? Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 10:e1469. doi: 10.1002/WCS.1469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkening, A., Kurzeck, A., Dechantsreiter, E., Padberg, F., and Palm, U. (2019). Transcranial alternating current stimulation for the treatment of major depression during pregnancy. Psychiatry Res. 279, 399–400. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2019.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolinski, N., Cooper, N. R., Sauseng, P., and Romei, V. (2018). The speed of parietal theta frequency drives visuospatial working memory capacity. PLoS Biol. 16:e2005348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, Y., Wei, P., Wang, C., Shan, Y., Yu, Y., Qiao, Y., et al. (2020). TRanscranial AlterNating current Stimulation FOR patients with Mild Alzheimer’s Disease (TRANSFORM-AD study): protocol for a randomized controlled clinical trial. Alzheimers Dement. 6:e12005. doi: 10.1002/TRC2.12005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, S., Liu, Y., Zheng, X., Zhang, Y., Cui, S., Tang, C., et al. (2020). Do nonpharmacological interventions prevent cognitive decline? a systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 10, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0690-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Chen, Y., Bressler, S. L., and Ding, M. (2008). Response preparation and inhibition: the role of the cortical sensorimotor beta rhythm. Neuroscience 156, 238–246. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2008.06.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziaei, M., Salami, A., and Persson, J. (2017). Age-related alterations in functional connectivity patterns during working memory encoding of emotional items. Neuropsychologia 94, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial alternating-current stimulation, working memory, brain oscillations, non-invasive brain stimulation, cognitive deficits

Citation: Al Qasem W, Abubaker M and Kvašňák E (2022) Working Memory and Transcranial-Alternating Current Stimulation—State of the Art: Findings, Missing, and Challenges. Front. Psychol. 13:822545. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.822545

Received: 25 November 2021; Accepted: 19 January 2022;
Published: 14 February 2022.

Edited by:

Robert West, DePauw University, United States

Reviewed by:

Jennifer Rodger, University of Western Australia, Australia
Jessica Moretti, University of Western Australia, Australia, in collaboration with reviewer JR
Ashley Jean Scolaro, Central College, United States
Sankaraleengam Alagapan, Georgia Institute of Technology, United States

Copyright © 2022 Al Qasem, Abubaker and Kvašňák. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wiam Al Qasem, wiam.alqasem@lf3.cuni.cz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.