Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Psychol., 25 April 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Validation of Polish Version of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 and Flow State Scale-2 Questionnaires

  • 1The Faculty of Information Technology, Polish-Japanese Academy of Information Technology, Warsaw, Poland
  • 2The Faculty of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland

The aim of this study is to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) and Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2), for use with Polish adults and young adults. Currently, there are no tools that would allow us to study flow among Polish speakers. At the same time, due to the great interest in flow and its potential importance for effectiveness, cooperation, and learning, it is worth ensuring that reliable validated measurement questionnaires are available for people studying the Polish population. Study participants completed 856 questionnaires, of which 496 individuals (with an average age of 36.31 years) participated in the DFS-2 study and 360 individuals (with an average age of 33.46 years) participated in the FSS-2 study. The maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) was selected for the CFA analysis. Model fit was assessed using: χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and standardized root mean square of residuals (SRMR), and root mean square of approximation error (RMSEA). For both questionnaires, formative first-level models with nine factors and second-level models with nine factors loaded on a higher-order flow factor were compared using the Satorra-Bentler Scaled difference χ2 test. The ω coefficient was used to estimate the reliability of the FFS-2 and DFS-2 models tested in the CFA method. Confirmatory factor analysis of both DFS-2 structural models showed satisfactory model fit. Most of the fit indices for the hierarchical 2nd order FSS-2 model presented satisfactory values, except for SRMR. Both DFS-2 and FSS-2 factors tested in the analysis showed good reliability (ω ≥ 0.7). Our findings confirmed the reliability and validity of the Polish versions of DFS-2 and FSS-2 scales. The scales are reliable when applied to Polish adults and young adults.

Introduction

Essence of Flow

Flow is a construct defining how the subject describes the subjective experience of engaging in challenging activity. It represents a basic psychological state that allows one to experience a full life. It can occur in any area of life (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990; Moneta and Csíkszentmihályi, 1996) and is related to the satisfaction derived from performing various activities. Dealing with the challenge was made real through a series of goals, receiving continuous feedback on progress and modifying actions based on this information (Nakamura and Csíkszentmihályi, 2009).

From the perspective of positive psychology, an understanding of the concept of flow can be considered crucial, because being completely absorbed in what one is doing is a desirable state of human functioning and the essence of the “good life” (Nakamura et al., 2002), and experiencing flow leads to personal growth (Seligman and Csíkszentmihályi, 2000).

Dimensions of Flow

Csíkszentmihályi (1990) and Moneta and Csíkszentmihályi (1996) distinguished and described nine dimensions (three conditions and six characteristics), each of which represents a distinctive conceptual dimension of the flow experience, while together they represent the flow experience (Jackson and Eklund, 2002). Csikszentmihalyi considered the balance between the challenge of the task and the skills of the individual to be the primary “universal precondition for flow” (Csíkszentmihályi and Csíkszentmihályi, 1992), thus broadening the perspective, as it was originally thought that optimal experience depends mainly on this condition (Engeser and Rheinberg, 2008; Landhäußer and Keller, 2012). For the balance to be maintained, clarity of goals and direct feedback are essential. So there are three conditions:

1. Challenge-Skill Balance (a sense of the adequacy of one’s competence and ability and the level of challenge in what one is doing).

2. Clear Goals (feeling confident about the action one is taking),

3. Unambiguous Feedback (immediate and clear feedback).

If these conditions are met it becomes possible to:

1. Merging of Action and Awareness (deep involvement makes one act spontaneously and almost automatically),

2. Concentration on the Task at Hand (a sense of complete focus),

3. Sense of Control (feeling that you can handle the situation),

4. Loss of Self-Consciousness (lack of a sense of concern for oneself and one’s performance),

5. Transformation of Time (feeling that the way time passes is distorted),

6. Autotelic Experience (experience of an activity in which the mere fact of doing that activity is satisfying).

Flow Research. A Historical Perspective

Research on the flow as a subjective state characterized by the intensity of the level of attention experienced by people while performing intrinsically motivated activities began in the mid-seventies (Csíkszentmihályi, 1975). So far, flow has been studied mainly in sports, music, chess, dance (Csíkszentmihályi and Larson, 1987; Csíkszentmihályi and Csíkszentmihályi, 1992; Csíkszentmihályi, 1993, 2000, 2014), or recently also in the context of video games (Nah et al., 2014).

Flow experience is both relevant and ephemeral and it is a key construct of positive psychology (Nakamura et al., 2002). Currently, researchers are continuing to search and construct testing of measurement tools most relevant to flow measurement. These include: The Flow Questionnaire (Nakamura and Csíkszentmihályi, 2009; Moneta, 2012), Experience Sampling Method (ESM) (Nakamura and Csíkszentmihályi, 2009; Moneta, 2012; Csíkszentmihályi and Larson, 2014), Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2), Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) (Jackson and Eklund, 2002; Moneta, 2012), Flow Short Scale (Delle Fave et al., 2011b; Engeser, 2012), Flow Scale Mayersa (Nakamura and Csíkszentmihályi, 2009), Optimal Experience Survey (Cuestionario de Experiencia Óptima; CEO) (Delle Fave et al., 2011a).

Dispositional Flow Scale-2, Flow State Scale-2

The most frequently used and approved are the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) and the Flow State Scale (FSS-2) (Kawabata et al., 2008).

For their study of sport and physical activity, Jackson and Marsh (1996) transformed Csikszentmihalyi’s nine-factor flow model into the Flow State Scale (FSS). Composed of 36 questions, the FSS was designed to assess the experience of flow during a specific activity, with data collected immediately after the activity (Jackson and Marsh, 1996).

Flow as a trait and tendency to experience flow was measured by the Dispositional Flow Scale (DFS), which was an extension of the Flow Trait Scale (TFS), also developed for measurement among athletes (Jackson et al., 1998). The DFS is a dispositional measure of flow and is used to assess the typical frequency of experiencing flow during participation in a particular activity.

The results of preliminary analyzes of the psychometric properties of DFS and FSS showed that both tools showed a satisfactory level of factor validity (Jackson and Marsh, 1996; Marsh and Jackson, 1999) and reliability (e.g., 1–4 0.72–0.91 for FSS and 1/4 0.70–0.88 for DFS, Jackson et al., 1998). Middleton et al. (2004) also reported an acceptable reliability (1/4 0.71–0.86) of DFS.

Reacting to the fact that some items in the original tool did not give satisfactory results (from a conceptual and/or statistical point of view), Jackson and Eklund (2002) revised both tools, replacing the problematic items and developed new versions of the flow scales: Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2) and Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2). Nine-dimensional conceptualization of flow, and the nine-factor structure was supported by confirmatory factor analyses (Jackson and Eklund, 2002). The higher order model, representing the global flow construct, also received reasonable support from confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

For the nine subscales of the DFS and FSS adequate reliability has been demonstrated, with the exception of time and self-awareness, which showed lower internal consistency (Jackson and Marsh, 1996; Marsh and Jackson, 1999; Tenenbaum et al., 1999; Kawabata et al., 2008; Gouveia et al., 2012).

Finally, Jackson et al. (2008b) published FSS-2 and DFS-2 in two versions: a. a long version with 36 items, divided equally into nine factors, corresponding to the nine dimensions of Csíkszentmihályi (1990) and b. a short version consisting of nine items, one for each dimension.

FSS-2 and DFS-2 have been translated and validated in various languages, especially often in the context of sports (Doganis et al., 2000, Fournier et al., 2007; Calvo et al., 2008; Kawabata et al., 2008; Gonzalez-Cutre et al., 2009; Gouveia et al., 2012; Crust and Swann, 2013; Riva et al., 2017; Nojavan et al., 2017), but also learning, recreation, well-being (Whitmore and Borrie, 2005; Asakawa, 2010, Rufi et al., 2014, Souza Costa Correia et al., 2020), gameplay and internet games (Wang et al., 2009; Procci et al., 2012; Hamari and Koivisto, 2014), mental diseases (Huang et al., 2019).

Most previous studies have used exploratory factor analysis rather than CFA. Kawabata et al. (2008) point out the difficulty of interpreting the expression of subjective experience without cross-cultural and cross-linguistic biases. The present study aimed to develop psychometrically valid Polish versions of the FSS-2 and DFS-2 (PFSS-2 and PDFS-2).

Currently, there are no tools to measure the flow in Polish culture. The purpose of this study is to remedy this deficiency. Providing Polish researchers with access to reliable tools for flow measurement seems to be particularly important due to the possibility of filling gaps in the analysis of processes related to the functioning of individuals and teams in various contexts, with particular emphasis on the processes of learning and development. A promising indication here are research on flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying (Cermakova et al., 2010), as a mediator between psychological ownership and employees’ subjective happiness (Fan et al., 2019), or considering the essence of flow as a mediator or moderator in the context of the impact of flow on relationship between resources and organizational outcomes (Seifert, 2015).

Flow in Video Games

One of the areas in which flow has been studied is games and gamification, i.e., the application of game mechanics to non-game areas (Wang et al., 2009; Procci et al., 2012; Hamari and Koivisto, 2014). Games, due to the complexity of activities, are a kind of flow base. The player is in a positive mental state, so focused on the game that nothing else matters. The activity is not necessarily aimed at a specific benefit. It is a sense of total presence, of detachment from reality–without fear or hope. It is an autotelic action, that is, it provides value to the player in itself. In complex activities that require a lot of cognitive, motor, or temporal input, flow seems to be a particularly important and quite natural state. On the other hand, the level of involvement of the person and the autotelicity that this phenomenon generates lead researchers to look at flow in the context of processes that are desirable for learning, building motivation and engagement in the activities undertaken. The interest in flow in games can be evidenced by the numerous studies on the topic that have emerged in the last year alone (Gao and Lu, 2021; Gutierrez, 2021; Cai et al., 2022; Jogo et al., 2022, etc.).

In addition to its general purpose of providing Polish researchers with a reliable tool for studying such an important phenomenon as flow, the present study was prompted by the specific need to allow for the examination of participants’ flow while learning a complex video game in a large study focused on the effects of video games on cognitive function.

Materials and Methods

The Validation Process

After verifying the presence of tools to measure the state and predisposition of flow in the Polish language, we found that at the moment there are no tools measuring flow in the Polish culture. As part of the initial consultation, we looked at several questionnaires (Nakamura and Csíkszentmihályi, 2009; Delle Fave et al., 2011; Engeser, 2012; Moneta, 2012; Csíkszentmihályi and Larson, 2014). The most frequently used and approved are the Dispositional Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) and the Flow State Scale (FSS-2), a reliable and validated tool with a long tradition in many countries, for instance: France (Fournier et al., 2007), Spain (Rufi et al., 2014), Italy (Riva et al., 2017), China (Huang et al., 2019), Greece (Stavrou and Zervas, 2004).

Flow–both as a disposition and a state–can affect both the learning process and the work of individuals (Cermakova et al., 2010; Seifert, 2015; Fan et al., 2019) and teams (Hout et al., 2017). Providing Polish researchers with access to tools for studying flow (as a moderator and mediator) is a step that is not only needed but also a necessity.

The translation process was carried out in three stages: 1. Establishment of a committee of experts, 2. translation into Polish, 3. backwards translation. The FSS-2 and DFS-2 questionnaires in the English version were translated by two translators, verified, and then translated backwards by a native speaker and re verified by psychologists.

The next step was to assess the psychometric properties of the questionnaire, in which 496 people (DFS-2) and 360 people (FSS-2) were used in such Polish language versions.

For the purposes of the study, a license was obtained to use both questionnaires (contact the authors of the questionnaires, Mind Garden, Inc. 18 March 2019) and the guidelines for their use “The Flow Scales Instrument and Scoring Guide” by Jackson et al. (2008a), published by Mind Garden, Inc.

Materials

Dispositional Flow Scale 2 (DFS-2) and Flow State Scale 2 (FSS-2) were developed by Jackson and Eklund (2002, 2004), based on previous tools developed by Jackson and Marsh (1996). These 36-point scales were developed to assess the experience of flow at disposition and state level based on the Csíkszentmihályi (1990) nine-dimensional flow concept.

Both scales are a set of 36 items, consisting of 4 items for each of the 9 flow dimensions: Challenge skill balance, Merging of Action and Awareness, Clear goals, Unambiguous feedback, Concentration on the Task at Hand, Sense of control, Loss of self-consciousness, Transformation of time, Autotelic experience.

Participants were asked to indicate the degree of their agreement with each of the elements as characterizing their disposition (DFS-2) and their experience with the activity just ended (FSS-2) on a “Likert-type” scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagreeing) to 5 (strongly agreeing).

Participants

Two independent groups of participants took part in the study: a. participants who agreed to participate in the DFS-2 study, b. participants who agreed to participate in the FSS-2 study.

Some participants agreed to both measurements.

Both questionnaires were completed by Polish-speaking people from all over Poland (big city, town, village) over 18 years old. Due to the limitations of COVID-19, the survey was conducted in an online format, so participants had to have access to the Internet. For FSS-2, they were ready to participate in a 1.5-h workshop where they played the online game Symbols. Participants, having previously read the study agenda, signed an informed consent, and then proceeded to play the game. For the DFS-2, the task was simpler, requiring only the completion of a questionnaire. Participants made their own decisions about which of the two studies they chose to engage in.

Recruitment for the study was conducted through online channels, student, volunteer, business, educational, and development networks. In addition to those included in the study, 17 individuals (12K, 5M) who did not meet the basic eligibility requirements were not included in the study: individuals under 18 years of age (11K, 2M), non-Polish speakers (1K, 3M). Participants were recruited for research using social media and information on websites.

Demographic Data

The research was attended by 856 persons (Table 1), of which 496 participants (female N = 286, male N = 210) were qualified for the DFS-2 tool, and 360 persons (female N = 190, male N = 169, other N = 1) for the FSS-2 questionnaire survey tool.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of the participants.

The participants of the survey are residents of:

for DFS-2: villages (N = 134), towns (N = 137), and cities (N = 225),

for FSS-2: villages (N = 75), towns (N = 201), and cities (N = 84).

Due to the large disproportion between the number of applications from people with higher education and primary education (98,2%–higher education), it was decided to limit the group to people with higher education or in the process of learning only (secondary education).

Course of the Study

Participants belonging to the first group (a.), having been previously informed about the study, signed the informed consent and filled in the Polish version of the DFS-2 questionnaire.

Participants belonging to the second group (b.), having previously read the study agenda, signed the informed consent and then proceeded to play the game “Symbols”, in groups of 15–30 people, online. After completing the task, participants completed the FSS-2 questionnaire, indicating flow as a state.

Participants played “Symbols” online. Each participant received (virtually) two cards with symbols. Only the owner of the cards could see their cards. The group’s goal was to arrange the set of cards in the only possible position on the game board. The first step was to figure out, using only verbal communication, how to do it (45 min), the second step was to guide the instructor to put the cards in the right place according to the participants’ instructions (3 min). Each participant was responsible for telling the instructor where to place each card.

The game “Symbols” was chosen among several games that were considered because of the intensity and fluctuating involvement of the participants.

Standard informed consent procedures were followed during data collection and institutional approval for the study was obtained (Ethical Committee Opinion No: 6/2021 issued by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology, SWPS University in Warsaw).

Statistics

Confirmatory Factor Analyses

The robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) was chosen for the CFA as it was shown to produce adequate estimation of factor loadings for 5-point Likert scales (Rhemtulla et al., 2012). Model fit was evaluated based on the following indices: χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The following thresholds were considered metrics of a satisfactory model fit: CFI > 0.9, TLI > 0.9, SRMR < 0.8, and RMSEA < 0.08 (Hair et al., 2010). Preferably, the χ2 test should not be significant. Nevertheless, it is not necessary since the χ2 test is based on sample size (Russell, 2002).

For both questionnaires, we compared the formative first level models with nine factors and second level models with nine factor loaded by a higher order flow factor (Jackson and Eklund, 2002) with the use of a Satorra-Bentler Scaled difference χ2 test (Satorra and Bentler, 2010). All analyses were calculated using R software (R Core Team, 2021), with the use of lavaan (Rosseel, 2012), and semTools (Jorgensen et al., 2021) packages.

Reliability Analyses

Coefficient ω was used to estimate the reliability of FFS-2 and DFS-2 models tested in CFA ((Dunn et al., 2014; Flora, 2020).

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2

Both structural models of DFS-2 presented satisfactory model fit (Table 2), however, the metrics were better for DFS-2 first order model. Moreover, the Satorra-Bentler Scaled difference χ2 showed a significant difference between the models, χ2 difference = 162.91, p < 0.001, favoring the formative first level model with nine factors. The factor loadings of the model are presented in the Table 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fit indices for two tested models of DFS questionnaire.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Factor loadings and reliability measures for the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 models tested in the study.

Reliability Analysis of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 Questionnaire

All factors tested in the analysis showed good reliability, as indicated by ω ≥ 0.7 (Table 3).

Additionally, intercorrelations of items of the DFS-2 questionnaire are presented in the Supplementary Material.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Flow State Scale-2

Most of the fit indices for the second order hierarchical FSS-2 model presented satisfactory values, with an exception of the SRMR (Table 4). The parameters of the model were explored to find what caused the misfit. For the hierarchical model, the factor “Transformation of time” was not significantly loaded by the global flow factor (r = 0.10, p = 0.150). The FSS-2 first order model showed better values of all fit indices. Additionally, the Satorra-Bentler Scaled difference χ2 showed a significant difference between the models, χ2 difference = 92.00, p < 0.001, favoring the formative first level model with nine factors. The factor loadings of the model are presented in the Table 5.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Fit indices for the models of the Flow State Scale-2.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Factor loadings of the Flow State Scale-2 questionnaire.

Reliability Analysis of the Flow State Scale-2

All factors tested in the analysis showed good reliability, as indicated by ω ≥ 0.7 (Table 4).

Additionally, intercorrelations of items of the FSS-2 questionnaire are presented in the Supplementary Material.

The results of the analyses in this study provided strong support for the validity and reliability of the Polish version of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (PDFS-2) and the Polish version of Flow State Scale-2 (PFSS-2) in assessing the experience of flow in the activity of playing games as entertainment among adult Poles.

As hypothesized, the psychometric properties obtained with the validated Polish versions of the DFS-2 and FFS-2 overlap with the responses of adult Polish speakers.

The maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) was selected for CFA analysis. Model fit was assessed using indices: χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and standardized root mean squares of residuals (SRMR) and root mean squares of approximation errors (RMSEA).

For both the DFS-2 and FSS-2 questionnaires, formative first-level models with nine factors and second-level models with nine factors loaded on a higher-order flow factor were compared using the Satorra-Bentler Scaled difference χ2 test.

The ω coefficient was used to estimate the reliability of the FFS-2 and DFS-2 models tested using the CFA method.

DFS-2

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2

Both DFS-2 structural models showed satisfactory model fit, but the metrics were better for the DFS-2 first order model. Furthermore, the scaled Satorra-Bentler χ2 difference showed a significant difference between the models, χ2 difference = 162.91, p < 0.001, favoring the formative first order model with nine factors.

Reliability Analysis of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 Questionnaire

All factors examined in the analysis showed good reliability, as indicated by ω ≥ 0.7.

FSS-2

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Dispositional Flow Status Scale-2

Most of the fit indices for the hierarchical FSS-2 second order model presented satisfactory values, except for SRMR. The model parameters were examined to find the cause of the misfit. For the hierarchical model, the “Time Transformation” factor was not significantly loaded by the global flow factor (r = 0.10, p = 0.150). The first order FSS-2 model showed better values for all fit indices. Additionally, the scaled Satorra-Bentler χ2 difference showed a significant difference between the models, χ2 difference = 92.00, p < 0.001, favoring the formative first order model with nine factors.

Reliability Analysis of the Flow State Scale-2

All factors examined in the analysis showed good reliability, as indicated by ω ≥ 0.7.

Intercorrelations of DFS-2 and FSS-2 questionnaire items are presented in the Supplementary Material.

Discussion

The main aim of the present study was to assess the psychometric properties of the Polish version of DFS-2 and FSS-2 for use with Polish adults. For this purpose, following the principles of test adaptation proposed by Gawlik and Kurpas (2014), a diversified group of participants from all over the country, from large and small towns and villages of various ages, was invited to the study. Due to the limitations of COVID-19, the recruitment and the study itself took place online, hence the participation required Internet access, which is a limitation.

Thanks to the research described above, it was possible to create a solid adaptation of PDFS-2 and PFFS-2 for adults speaking Polish.

The study of flow allows us to understand experiences during which individuals are fully engaged in the present moment of a task. Flow is a state that occurs during full engagement, in which the individual is operating at a level consistent with the demands of the task and is fully focused on the present moment, which positive psychology defines as one of the key determinants of a good, happy life. Flow, as a phenomenon potentially present in every area of life, has been studied in sports, music, chess, dance and games, including video games, among others. This tool validation was prepared for the research project “Player playability. Correlation between the dynamics of changes in cognitive functioning resulting from complex skill learning through play and player flow, motivation and perceived playability.” The purpose of this project is, among other things, to verify how a person’s flow influences their learning process of a complex game. The flow measurement tools will allow for a better understanding of the influence of video games on cognitive functions and the mediators that accompany this process. The starting point for this project is the concept of flow, which is the assumption that humans function optimally when the challenges they face are balanced (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990; Moneta and Csíkszentmihályi, 1996). This allows us to assume that the development of cognitive functions will proceed more efficiently in a higher flow state.

Limitations and Methodological Considerations

The results of the study may be affected by the fact that the participants of the study carried out their activity with the awareness of the participation in the study–this was the first activity carried out in a group of people who did not know each other.

Another limitation may be the fact that the research was performed online due to the limitations of COVID-19.

Conclusion

The current research has provided indications that both the Polish versions of DFS-2 and FSS-2 are valid and reliable questionnaires. During the study, several aspects emerged that should be examined in future research in order to better understand both the nature of the flow phenomenon and the questionnaire tools themselves.

Possible directions of exploration:

1. Criteria-related validity (Kline, 2005) using self-report tools of other psychological constructs (motivation, immersion, commitment, self-esteem, locus of control) or variables (participation and skills levels). Some of them (motivation, locus of control, skills levels) are part of the “Playability of the Player” study.

2. Verification of the stability of the questionnaires. It seems interesting to verify to what extent the measurement for a given participant is stable. While the nature of the FSS-2 tool implies that it is related to the “here and now,” it would be interesting to observe whether there are trends within a person’s measure. A study carried out by the team of authors of this article, “Playability of the player,” which assumes repeated measurement of flow as a state, spread over time, and related to learning a specific complex activity, may be helpful here. In the context of the DFS-2, it is worth looking at its stability, which Kawabata et al. (2008) undertook by measuring it twice over a 4-week interval. In the cited study, factors such as task focus, loss of self-awareness, time transformation, and autotelic experience showed slightly less stability than others. However, because the stability of responses to DFS-2 was not tested, it is not possible to determine whether these characteristics are sample specific.

3. Verification of the correlation between DFS-2 and FSS-2. Another interesting direction seems to be the implementation of group studies using both questionnaires (DFS-2 and FSS-2). Also such an activity has been planned in the framework of the project “Playability of the player,” where the participants first complete the DFS-2 questionnaire and then take part in a learning process with repeated measurement using the FSS-2 questionnaire.

The scales assessed in this study are useful tools for a variety of research purposes. They are also relevant to those studying flow in various aspects of life, including video games, which provide a safe yet compelling and variedly challenging environment for exploration (Wang et al., 2009; Procci et al., 2012; Hamari and Koivisto, 2014; Gao and Lu, 2021; Gutierrez, 2021; Cai et al., 2022; Jogo et al., 2022, etc.).

The results of the study allow for the use of the Polish version of the tool, which will make it possible to study flow using the DFS-2 and FSS-2 in both game studies and other activities where the potential for flow state is identified.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Commission on Research Ethics of the Faculty of Psychology, SWPS University in Warsaw. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JJ and NK-G conceptualized the project. JJ conducted data analyses, verified by AB and NK-G. All authors take full responsibility for the integrity and accuracy of the data analysis and approved the final version for submission.

Funding

This study was supported by the Polish National Science Centre, NCN (Narodowe Centrum Nauki) Grant 2016/23/B/HS6/03843. Open access was financed by the European Union Resources within the European Social Found no POWR.03.02.00-00-I054/16-00 and supported by a doctoral fellowship ICT&Psychology – Interdisciplinary Ph.D. Study from the University of Social Sciences and Humanities and Polish Japanese Academy of Information Technology.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Adam Wierzbicki and Elżbieta Hornowska for their support and motivation in carrying out the study. The content of this article is drawn from a larger project but data presented here are not reported elsewhere.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.818036/full#supplementary-material

References

Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? J. Happiness Stud. 11, 205–223.

Google Scholar

Cai, X., Cebollada, J., and Cortiñas, M. (2022). Self-report measure of dispositional flow experience in the video game context: conceptualisation and scale development. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 159:102746. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2021.102746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo, T. G., Jiménez Castuera, R., Santos-Rosa Ruano, F. J., Reina Vaíllo, R., and Cervelló Gimeno, E. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the flow state scale. Span. J. Psychol. 11, 660–669. doi: 10.1017/s1138741600004662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cermakova, L., Moneta, G. B., and Spada, M. M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educ. Psychol. 30, 495–511. doi: 10.1080/01443411003777697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L., and Swann, C. (2013). The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional flow. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 13, 215–220. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2011.635698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1975). Purpose and Mind. Contemp. Psychol. 20, 352–353. doi: 10.1037/0013305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. Vol. 1990. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1993). Activity and happiness: Towards a science of occupation. J. Occup. Sci. 1, 38–42. doi: 10.1080/14427591.1993.9686377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2000). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety (25th anniversary ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. [Original work published in 1975].

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2014). Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology: The Collected Works of Mihály Csíkszentmihályi. Dordrecht: Springer Science Business Media.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M., and Csíkszentmihályi, I. S. (1992). Optimal Experience: Psychological Study of Flow in Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M., and Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the Experience Sampling Method. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 175, 526–536. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M., and Larson, R. (2014). “Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method,” in Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology: The Collected Works of Mihaly Csíkszentmihályi, ed. M. Csíkszentmihályi (Dordrecht: Springer), 35–54. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., and Wissing, M. P. (2011a). The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: qualitative and quantitative findings. Soc. Indic. Res. 100, 185–207. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delle Fave, A., Massimini, F., and Bassi, M. (2011b). “Instruments and Methods in Flow Research,” in Psychological Selection and Optimal Experience Across Cultures: Social Empowerment through Personal Growth, eds A. Delle Fave, F. Massimini, and M. Bassi (Dordrecht: Springer), 59–87.

Google Scholar

Doganis, G., Iosifidou, P., and Vlachopoulos, S. (2000). Factor structure and internal consistency of the greek version of the flow state scale. Percept. Mot. Skills 91(3 Pt 2), 1231–1240. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3f.1231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, T. J., Baguley, T., and Brunsden, V. (2014). From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 399–412. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engeser, S. (2012). Advances in Flow Research. New York, NY: Springer, 231. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engeser, S., and Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, performance and moderators of challenge-skill balance. Motiv. Emot. 32, 158–172. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9102-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, H.-L., Hou, S.-T., and Lin, Y.-H. (2019). Flow as a mediator between psychological ownership and employees’ subjective happiness. J. Manag. Psychol. 34, 445–458. doi: 10.1108/JMP-11-2017-0405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flora, D. B. (2020). Your coefficient alpha is probably wrong, but which coefficient omega is right? A tutorial on using r to obtain better reliability estimates. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 3, 484–501. doi: 10.1177/2515245920951747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fournier, J., Gaudreau, P., Demontrond-Behr, P., Visioli, J., Forest, J., and Jackson, S. (2007). French translation of the flow state scale-2: factor structure, cross-cultural invariance, and associations with goal attainment. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 8, 897–916. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, J., and Lu, L. (2021). “Influence of flow conditions in entertainment activities: is self-reported levels of flow state higher in video games than in TV Shows for 18-24 U.S. Undergraduate students,” in Proceedings of the 2021 4th International Conference on Humanities Education and Social Sciences (ICHESS 2021), (Amsterdam: Atlantis Press).

Google Scholar

Gawlik, M., and Kurpas, D. (2014). Principles of Questionnaire Validation Using the Caregiver Quality of Life-Cancer Questionnaire as an Example. Available online at: https://depot.ceon.pl/handle/123456789/5646 (accessed November 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Cutre, D., Sicilia, A., Moreno, J. A., and Fernandez-Balboa, J. M. (2009). Dispositional flow in physical education: relationships with motivational climate, social goals, and perceived competence. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 28, 422–440. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.28.4.422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouveia, M. J., Pais-Ribeiro, J. L., Marques, M., and Carvalho, C. (2012). Measuring dispositional flow in exercise. Validity and reliability of a portuguese version of the dispositional flow scale-2 (DFS-2). Rev. Psicol. Deporte 21, 81–88.

Google Scholar

Gutierrez, J. P. (2021). Do game transfer phenomena lead to flow? An investigation of in-game and out-game immersion among MOBA gamers. Comput. Hum. Behav. Rep. 3:100079. doi: 10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., and Babin, B. J. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th Editio Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Hamari, J., and Koivisto, J. (2014). Measuring flow in gamification: Dispositional Flow Scale-2. Comput. Hum. Behav. 40, 133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hout, J. J. J., Gevers, J. M. P., Davis, O. C., and Weggeman, M. C. D. P. (2017). Overcoming impediments to team flow. Chall. Organ. Soc. 6, 1165–1181.

Google Scholar

Huang, L. J., Hu, F. C., Wu, C., Yang, Y. H., Lee, S. C., Fang, Y. C., et al. (2019). Traditional-Chinese dispositional flow scale-2 and flow state scale-2 in Taiwanese subjects with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Psychiatry Res. 274, 287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: the flow state scale–2 and dispositional flow scale–2. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 24, 133–150. doi: 10.1123/jsep.24.2.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Eklund, R. C. (2004). “Relationships between quality of experience and participation in diverse performance settings http://espace.Library.Uq.Edu.Au/workflow/edit_metadata.Php?Id=382160&wfs_id=778,” in Proceedings of the 39th Conference of The Australian Psychological Society, Sydney, NSW.

Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., Martin, A. J., and Eklund, R. C. (2008b). Long and short measures of flow: the construct validity of the FSS-2, DFS-2, and new brief counterparts. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 30, 561–587. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.5.561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., Eklund, R. C., and Martin, A. J. (2008a). The Flow Scales Instrument and Scoring Guide. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden, Inc.

Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., Ford, S. K., Kimiecik, J. C., and Marsh, H. W. (1998). psychological correlates of flow in sport. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 20, 358–378. doi: 10.1123/jsep.20.4.358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: the flow state scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 18, 17–35. doi: 10.1123/jsep.18.1.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jogo, D. A., Challco, G. C., Bittencourt, I. I., Reis, M., Silva, L. R., and Isotani, S. (2022). Investigating how gamified syllabic literacy impacts learning, flow and inappropriate behaviors: a single-subject study design. Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. 33:100458. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2022.100458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., and Rosseel, Y. (2021). semTools: Useful Tools for Structural Equation Modeling.

Google Scholar

Kawabata, M., Mallett, C. J., and Jackson, S. A. (2008). The Flow State Scale-2 and Dispositional Flow Scale-2: examination of factorial validity and reliability for Japanese adults. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 9, 465–485. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, T. (2005). Psychological Testing: A Practical Approach to Design and Evaluation. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Landhäußer, A., and Keller, J. (2012). “Flow and its affective, cognitive, and performance-related consequences,” in Advances in Flow Research, ed. S. Engeser (New York, NY: Springer), 65–85. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Jackson, S. A. (1999). Flow experience in sport: construct validation of multidimensional, hierarchical state and trait responses. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 343–371. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Middleton, S. C., Marsh, M. W., Martin, A. J., Richards, G. E., and Perry, C. (2004). “Developing the Mental Toughness Inventory (MTI). Self-concept, motivation and identity, where to from here?,” in Proceedings of the 3rd International Biennial SELF Research Conference, (Sydney, NSW).

Google Scholar

Moneta, G. B. (2012). “On the Measurement and Conceptualization of Flow,” in Advances in Flow Research, ed. S. Engeser (New York, NY: Springer), 23–50. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moneta, G. B., and Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. J. Pers. 64, 275–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00512.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nah, F. F.-H., Eschenbrenner, B., Zeng, Q., Telaprolu, V. R., and Sepehr, S. (2014). Flow in gaming: literature synthesis and framework development. Int. J. Inf. Syst. Manage. 1, 83–124. doi: 10.1504/ijisam.2014.062288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, J., and Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2009). “Flow theory and research,” in Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds J. Lopez and C. R. Snyder (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 195–206.

Google Scholar

Nakamura, J., Csíkszentmihályi, M., Snyder, C. R., and Lopez, S. J. (2002). “Handbook of positive psychology,” in The Concept of Flow, 2ndedn Edn, eds C. R. Snyder, S. J. Lopez, L. M. Edwards, and S. C. Marques (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 89–105.

Google Scholar

Nojavan, A. H., Zandi, H. G., and Talab, R. H. (2017). Psychometric properties of persian version dispositional flow scale -2 (DFS-2). IOSR J. Sports Phys. Educ. 4, 4–11. doi: 10.9790/6737-04030411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Procci, K., Singer, A. R., Levy, K. R., and Bowers, C. (2012). Measuring the flow experience of gamers: An evaluation of the DFS-2. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 2306–2312. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É, and Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychol. Methods 17, 354–373. doi: 10.1037/a0029315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva, E. F. M., Riva, G., Talò, C., Boffi, M., Rainisio, N., Pola, L., et al. (2017). Measuring Dispositional flow: validity and reliability of the dispositional flow state scale 2, Italian Version. Edited by Hamari. J. PLoS One 12:e0182201. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rufi, S., Javaloy, F., Batista-Foguet, J. M., Solanas, A., and Páez, D. (2014). flow dimensions on daily activities with the spanish version of the flow scale (DFS). Span. J. Psychol. 17:E3O. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2014.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D. W. (2002). In search of underlying dimensions: the use (and abuse) of factor analysis in personality and social psychology bulletin. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1629–1646. doi: 10.1177/014616702237645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring Positiveness of the Scaled Difference Chi-square Test Statistic. Psychometrika 75, 243–248. doi: 10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seifert, A. (2015). Mediator or Moderator? The Influence of Flow at Work on the Relationship between Resources and Organizational Outcomes. Munich: GRIN Verlag.

Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E., and Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2000). Positive Psychology, An Introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza Costa Correia, S. A., Mendonça Filho, E. J., Tischer, M. I., Mendes de Oliveira, C., and Hofheinz Giacomoni, C. (2020). Long dispositional flow scale (DFS-2) general: adaptation to and validation for Brazilian portuguese. Trends Psychol. 28, 419–436. doi: 10.1007/s43076-020-00028-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stavrou, N. A., and Zervas, Y. (2004). Confirmatory factor analysis of the flow state scale in sports. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2, 161–181. doi: 10.1080/1612197x.2004.9671739

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenenbaum, G., Fogarty, G. J., and Jackson, S. A. (1999). The flow experience: a Rasch analysis of Jackson’s flow state scale. J. Outcome Meas. 3, 278–294.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wang, C. K. J., Liu, W. C., and Khoo, A. (2009). The psychometric properties of dispositional flow scale-2 in internet gaming. Curr. Psychol. 28, 194–201. doi: 10.1007/s12144-009-9058-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitmore, J. G., and Borrie, W. T. (2005). “Exploring the usefulness of the dispositional flow scale for outdoor recreation activities,” in Proceedings of the 2005 Northeastern recreation research symposium, Bolton Landing, New York, USA, April 10–12, 2005, (Bolton Landing, NY), 371–378.

Google Scholar

Keywords: flow, positive psychology, scale adaptation, multidimensional instruments, validation, flow dimensions

Citation: Józefowicz J, Kowalczyk-Grębska N and Brzezicka A (2022) Validation of Polish Version of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 and Flow State Scale-2 Questionnaires. Front. Psychol. 13:818036. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.818036

Received: 19 November 2021; Accepted: 28 February 2022;
Published: 25 April 2022.

Edited by:

Kwok Kit Tong, University of Macau, China

Reviewed by:

Dario Paez, University of the Basque Country, Spain
İsmail Seçer, Atatürk University, Turkey

Copyright © 2022 Józefowicz, Kowalczyk-Grębska and Brzezicka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Justyna Józefowicz, jjozefowicz1@swps.edu.pl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.