Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 18 May 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Is Self-Transcendence Philanthropic? Graded Response Model Approach

\nBandos Ros,
Bandos Ros1,2*Shinji KanekoShinji Kaneko1
  • 1Graduate School for International Development and Cooperation, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
  • 2Department of Environmental Education, Ministry of Environment, Phnom Penh, Cambodia

This study reveals that strong feelings of altruism were found to be statistically significant in explaining prosocial and pro-environmental behaviors. However, this was not the case for the latent trait biosphere in explaining pro-environmental behavior (e.g., past volunteering in clean-up activities). Regardless of whether they are overseas graduates or not, subjects in this study are more altruistic than biospheric by nature. Using the Graded Response Model (GRM) approach, the study found that the biosphere and altruism are obviously independent of each other and merging them into one dimension, in this instance referred to as “self-transcendence,” makes the construct less reliable. That is why this study in consistence with previous studies could not detect the effect of self-transcendence statistically, as it affects both the past volunteering in environmental affairs and the past volunteering in social welfare.

Introduction

Values, the desirable goals and guiding principles in someone's life (see e.g., Brunsø et al., 2004; van der Werff et al., 2013), is a variable that is empirically studied in the field of environmental psychology to find its effects on behaviors. Some studies found that it exerts direct effects on behaviors (Stern et al., 1995; Liobikiene and Juknys, 2016), while others found its effect on behaviors via beliefs, attitudes, and norms (see Gärling et al., 2003; Nilsson et al., 2004; Lee, 2011; Maio, 2011; Steg and de Groot, 2012; van der Werff et al., 2013). Human values can drive a person's actions (Schwartz, 1992 as cited by Dominicis et al., 2017), particularly in inducing pro-environmental behavior (Crompton and Kasser, 2009; Knez, 2016). Though there is a value-action gap, Maio (2011) claimed that, in general, it influences a broad range of behaviors, for instance, waste recycling (Thomas and Sharp, 2013). In the field of environment, values are categorized into four different components (Stern et al., 1998; de Groot and Steg, 2008) (Steg et al., 2014): biospheric values (i.e., valuing the environment), altruistic values (i.e., valuing the welfare and well-being of other human beings), egoistic values (i.e., valuing personal resources/centrality), and hedonic values (i.e., valuing pleasure and comfort).

According to the theory of Values Beliefs Norms (VBN), values, especially biospheric values, determine environmental attitude (Stern et al., 1993, 1995; Hansla et al., 2008; Lee, 2011; Papagiannakis and Lioukas, 2012; van Riper and Kyle, 2014). The theory also confirmed that a person with altruistic values is more likely to act pro-environmentally (Stern, 2000 as cited by Prager, 2012). Altruism, defined as self-sacrifice with no apparent personal reward, was also found to be one of the drivers of volunteering (Unger, 1991; Okabe et al., 2017). Nevertheless, whether altruism is always associated with other types of volunteering or in different cultural contexts is doubtful/open to question. Though Becker (1974) provides evidence for an altruistic motive across many types of voluntary activity, Luria et al. (2017) suggested that people with different values in different cultures are likely to engage in different types of volunteering. Therefore, whether different volunteering fields are affected by various drivers should be explored further, as suggested by Kewes and Munsch (2019).

Altruistic values indicate one's concern for the harmful effects of the action on other human beings. So, altruistic values are more likely to be positively correlated with pro-environmental beliefs and behaviors when such behaviors result in positive outcomes for other people (e.g., Stern et al., 1998; de Groot and Steg, 2007; Perlaviciute and Steg, 2014). According to the Goal Framing Theory, environmental behaviors are governed by normative goals, where people are led to act based on what other people think they should do (Lindenberg and Steg, 2013; Steg et al., 2014). This means that altruistic people are likely convinced to engage in pro-environmental behaviors because they may think that protecting the environment is saving human lives. Therefore, Joireman et al. (2001) found that the more altruistic, the more concerned about the environment people will be, while Dominicis et al. (2017) confirmed that altruistic value was associated with pro-environmental behaviors.

Self-transcendence values, which make individuals focus on the interests of others and the environment (see e.g., Schwartz, 1992, 2012; Castelo et al., 2021; Wong et al., 2021), are led more by the normative goal (Steg et al., 2014a,b) and have been found to be positively related to pro-environmental beliefs and behaviors (Stern et al., 1998; Schultz and Zelezny, 1999; Boer and Fischer, 2013; Cheung et al., 2014; Steg et al., 2014b), such as energy saving, recycling, protection, and acceptance of environmental policies (Stern, 2000; Nordlund and Garvill, 2002, 2003; Milfont and Gouveia, 2006; de Groot and Steg, 2007, 2008, 2009; Hansla et al., 2008; Nilsson et al., 2016). Though it is well-known to predict volunteering behavior, self-transcendence was unexpectedly found to be statistically insignificant in explaining the variation in any form of volunteering (Onuki and Xiao, 2020). So, whether the self-transcendence value is reliable or not should be further investigated, as suggested by Stern et al. (1998). This study recalls Schwartz's value theory in which altruism and biosphere are combined into one value cluster, that is, the self-transcendence dimension (Dietz et al., 2005; Steg and de Groot, 2012). Though Stern et al. (1998) found the reliability of the construct, various empirical studies later found and supported the distinction between altruistic and biospheric value orientations (Schultz, 2000, 2001; de Groot and Steg, 2008; Steg et al., 2014b). In these contradictory implications, it is necessary to compare the effect of the one-factor construct, the self-transcendence, against the two-factor construct, altruism and biosphere, on different fields of volunteering.

Materials and Methods

The study uses primary data collected from an online survey in November 2020. In cooperation with the Cambodian Association in Japan (CSAJ) and the Hiroshima Alumni Network and using social media (i.e., Facebook), a total of 224 respondents agreed and participated in the survey. The respondents are those scholars experiencing education locally (i.e., in Cambodia only) (N = 59), experiencing overseas education, but residing in their home country (i.e., Cambodia) (N = 65), and doing their education overseas (N = 100) (i.e., 85% in Japan).

Two indicators that are tested include past volunteering in blood or monetary donations and past volunteering in clean-up activities. These two variables are a binary indicator (1 = participated in clean-up activities, 0 = otherwise) and (1 = donated blood or money, 0 = otherwise). Participants were also asked to report their gender, age, educational level, and the fixed effects (i.e., location where they are residing: 1 = overseas education and living in their home country upon their graduation and 0 = otherwise and 1 = overseas education and still living in host countries and 0 = otherwise). These five background variables are included in our subsequent analyses as control variables.

For concerns on social and environmental issues, subjects were asked to rate the seven-point Likert scale on a nine-item measure (0 = extremely disagree to 6 = extremely agree). These items are adapted from a study measuring the validity and reliability of human values, such as altruism, biosphere, and egoism (Stern et al., 1998; Perlaviciute and Steg, 2014; Bouman et al., 2018). A row mean score or summative score of the three constructs is computed after the reliability and validity of the constructs are measured. Higher scores indicate people are more likely to exhibit self-transcendent, altruistic, and biospheric concerns (see Supplementary Table 1 for Descriptive Statistics). I computed the factor score of the three constructs as well, since their loadings were not similar. For instance, the loading of the one-factor model ranges from 0.58 to 0.90 (see Supplementary Table 3). The item that has a loading of 0.90 on a factor is more salient than that with a loading of 0.58. In this case, the factor score will be better than summative score to use because factor score weight items by their salience (Acock, 2013).

I used orthogonal factor analysis with varimax rotation to identify the internal structure of the items. In this analysis, nine items were loaded together. Based on its meaning and the Cronbach's alpha value (∞), item 6 was dropped, otherwise the internal reliability would decrease from ∞ = 0.89 to ∞ = 0.88. The item 7 value of Factor 1 and Factor 2 were close to each other, and it was then presumed to be an item of Factor 2 based on its meaning. I tested the Cronbach's alpha of Factor 2 with and without item 7, and the result showed exactly the importance of including item 7 in Factor 2, which makes the alpha value to increase from ∞ = 0.71 to ∞ = 0.79 (see Supplementary Table 2).

To test how the models will fit the data, I conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), using the software package Stata 15.1. The CFA results show the one-factor model is a better fit with the data than the two-factor model. The model indices of the one-factor model are listed as follows: chi-square to the degree of freedom [X(13)2 = 20.232 with p > 0.05], Comparative Fit Index (CFI) is 0.991, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) is 0.05, and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) is 0.030. The model indices of the two-factor model are listed as follows: chi-square to the degree of freedom [X(16)2 = 28.837 with p < 0.05], Comparative Fit Index (CFI) is 0.986, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) is 0.061, and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) is 0.032 (see Supplementary Figure 1).

As already computed, the Cronbach's alphas of the one-factor model (∞ = 0.890) exceeded the criteria value of 0.7, indicating a high degree of internal consistency. Consistently, the internal reliability (rho) (p = 0.909) indicates that the variation in the scale is 90.9% (p = 0.909), which is explained by the construct (Acock, 2013). Similarly, the Cronbach's alphas of the two-factor model (i.e., biosphere and altruism construct) were ∞ = 0.893 and ∞ = 0.79 (>0.70), respectively, while the internal reliability (rho) was 87.7% (p = 0.877) and 94.5% (p = 0.945), respectively (see Supplementary Tables 35). From these results, it is presumed that the one-factor and two-factor constructs are reliable and valid (see e.g., Wong et al., 2021).

Results and Discussion

The association between the two volunteering behaviors was first tested because altruism was found to be statistically correlated with the biosphere, as confirmed by Dominicis et al. (2017) and Joireman et al. (2001). Overall, 67.43% of respondents engaged in clean-up activities, of whom 72.25% engaged in blood or money donation. The odds of engaging in clean-up activities is 147/71 = 2.07. This means that there are 2.07 people engaging in clean-up for each person who has never engaged in such activity. The odds of engaging in clean-up activity, if subjects also engaged in blood or money donation, is 125/48 = 2.60. This means among those subjects donating blood or money, there are 2.60 people engaging in clean-up activity for each person who has never engaged in the clean-up. However, the odds of engaging in clean-up activity if subjects have never engaged in blood or money donation is 22/23 = 0.96. This means among those subjects who have never donated blood or money, there are just 0.96 people engaging in clean-up for each person who never participated in the clean-up activity. The odds ratio of engaging in clean-up is 2.60/0.96 = 2.71. So, the odds of engaging in clean-up if subjects donate blood or money are 2.71 times greater than the odds of engaging in clean-up if subjects never donate blood or money. How much greater? I found the difference to be 171% [100 × (2.71–1)] greater.

So, people engaged in social welfare are more likely to be engaged in clean-ups. To measure if the association is statistically significant, I employed chi-squared test, and the result was [X(1,N=218)2 = 8.877 and p < 0.01] with effect size φ (phi) = 0.202. It can be, therefore, concluded that there is a statistically significant association with moderate effect size between the two volunteering activities (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Association between volunteering in clean-up and social welfare.

To determine the predictors of these two past volunteer activities, a binomial logit model was applied. As hypothesized earlier, human values (i.e., altruism, biosphere, and self-transcendence) were used separately as the main predictor variables (or independent variables). The outcome variables (or dependent variables) are past volunteering in social welfare (i.e., blood or money donation) and past volunteering in clean-up activities. Each variable can be answered in the positive or negative, representing whether the respondents performed that activity. Multicollinearity between predictors was also tested using variance inflation factors (VIFs). The result showed that the mean VIFs for all the predictor variables is 1.39, which is acceptable.

Table 2 shows that self-transcendence values, which are hypothetically associated with both volunteering in social welfare and environment-oriented activities, were found to be statistically insignificant. A minor relationship was detected between the value and volunteering in the environment (regressions 15 and 16). However, no statistically significant relationship was found between the value and past volunteering in social welfare (as exhibited by blood or monetary donation). So, it could be concluded that self-transcendence value, which is expectedly associated with volunteering in both social and environmental services, is not supported by this study. This result appears to be inconsistent with previous ones (Stern et al., 1998; Boer and Fischer, 2013). However, it is consistent with a recent study, which reported that self-transcendence had no association with volunteering in any domain (Onuki and Xiao, 2020).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effects of human values on different fields of volunteering.

Altruism, being prosocial or self-less, was found to be statistically significant in predicting past volunteering in social welfare, with consistent results between summative score and factor score (regressions 6 and 8) as expected. From the marginal effect, we can conclude that with one unit increase in altruism (i.e., concerns for others), the probability of donating blood or offering money increases by 7.5%. So, the assumption that altruistic or prosocial behaviors positively relate to past volunteering in social welfare is accepted.

Biosphere was found to be statistically significant at 10% only (regressions 2 and 4). Thus, the assumption that biospheric value (i.e., concern for the environment) will positively correlate with past volunteering in the environment was unexpectedly rejected, in contrast with previous studies. Though a variety of behaviors are not always explained by human values (Maio, 2011), there is doubt and a question as to why altruism was found to be statistically significant, but the biosphere was not. Are subjects prosocial and pro-environment? To answer these questions, the strength of salience or sensitivity of each construct, “altruism” and “biosphere,” should be further explored. The question also applies to the construct, “self-transcendence,” since it was not statistically significant in predicting either of the two past volunteering behaviors.

To realize to what extent the subjects are altruistic or biospheric, or to detect the sensitivity of subject's altruism, biosphere, and self-transcendence, the Latent Trait Model (LTM), which is known as a scale assessment tool, was applied. Since the items to be measured are ordinal, the Graded Response Model (GRM) belonging to the item response theory (IRT) was utilized (Samejima, 1969). Based on the information provided by each item's response categories, an item information function (IIF) was developed for the three latent variables, that is, biosphere, altruism, and self-transcendence. Figure 1 shows the graphs for each item's information function. The graphs were created using the Stata 15.1 package (Acock, 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Strength of salience or sensitivity of human value (human concerns). (A) Item information function (Biosphere). (B) Item information function (Altruism). (C) Item information function (Self-transcendence).

As seen in Figure 1, the peaks in each graph represent the location on which the item of underlying value latent trait (i.e., human concerns) provides information the most. Because the Information is the reciprocal of the standard error of measurement (i.e., the variance of the latent trait level) at a given point on the latent trait, the more information, therefore, means the less error. The amount of information basically indicates an item's ability to measure the value latent trait reliably (Bankert et al., 2017). For reliability of the item's ability, please see Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Reliability for the human values of θ (theta) from the GRM model.

The strength of human concern lies on the x-axis and has, in its transformed scale of theta, a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 with a somewhat arbitrary range that covers the extent of the latent trait (Bankert et al., 2017). For the sake of this research, the theta for human concern strength ranges from −4 to 4, with those closer to −4 displaying lower levels of concern and those closer to 4 displaying higher levels of concern. Thus, an information function peaking closer to 4 provides a considerable amount of information at high levels of concern, whereas an information function that peaks closer to −4 better captures lower levels of concern. Finally, an information function that peaks at the midpoint presents an item with considerable ability to distinguish middle from higher and lower levels of concern. Thus, ideal items should cover a wide range of subject's concern strengths and be able to discriminate effectively among different levels.

From the information provided in the latent trait, self-transcendence, the seven items do not supplement each other, which is at odds with the other two latent traits, the biosphere and the altruism. The items belonging to biosphere (i.e., value 1, value 2, value 3, and value 4) are up above, while the items belonging to altruism (i.e., value 5, value 6, and value 7) are far below (Graph A, Figure 1). In this case, the three items belonging to altruism provide the least information. Combining these two constructs into one self-transcendence dimension more likely provides more information on the biosphere than the altruism side. The primary conclusion is, therefore, that altruism and biosphere are more likely two separate constructs (Schultz, 2000, 2001; de Groot and Steg, 2008; Steg et al., 2014b) which should not be merged into self-transcendence dimension, as reported in previous studies of Stern et al. (1998) and Boer and Fischer (2013).

On the contrary, regarding the information provided in the latent trait, altruism and biosphere, the items of each construct supplement each other to cover a broad range of the latent trait of human concern on social and environmental issues. For the biosphere, value 1: “It is important to love nature more” provides the most information, while value 4: “It is important we shall live with nature” provides the least information. Value 2: “It is important to stop environmental pollution” supplements with value 3: “It is important to protect and preserve environment,” where there is low information provided by value 2 (i.e., θ = −2) and is covered by the information provided by value 3 (Graph A, Figure 1).

In Graph B (Figure 1), the four values supplement each other well. Value 5: “It is important to help each other,” value 8: “It is important we shall have equal opportunity,” and value 9: “It is important we should take care of those who are worse off ” display multiple peaks above the midpoint of the latent trait continuum and thus provide good coverage of higher level of concerns for other people. Value 7: “It is important to have equal justice” provides more information below the midpoint of the latent trait though. So, if the subjects of this study are less altruistic, they believe that equality-based justice is important, but if their concerns are high (i.e., θ > 0), they more likely agree that the other three values (i.e., helping people, providing equal opportunity, and taking care of those who are worse off) are more crucial.

The Reliability of the fitted IRT model can be computed with the following equation:

ρi=1-σe2    (1)

The error variance σe2 is defined as 1/I, where I is the information. At some points on the scale, there is very little error variance, while at the other points, there is a lot. The error variance is small when there is a lot of information and larger when there is less information (Thissen, 2000). So, Equation (1) could be written again as:

ρi=1-σe2=1-(1/Ii)    (2)

This formula defines the reliability of the measure at different points, i, along the θ continuum (Acock, 2018).

As mentioned earlier, the items representing value 1 of the latent trait biosphere provide the most information, when compared to the other items (Graph A). The reliability P−3 = 0.925 means only 0.075 (7.5%) of the variance in the measure of the people who have low biospheric levels θ = −3 is not reliable (Table 3). Though multiple peaks were detected among the four items, the reliability of the information provided is high only when the θ value is between −3 and 0, so it is more likely that the subjects of this study are less biospheric (i.e., less concern for environment or the subjects are neither pro-environmentalist nor environmental activists). If the subjects of this study have stronger concern (i.e., θ > 0), the influence of biosphere on past volunteering in clean-up would have likely been statistically detected. It could be presumed that the subjects of this study strongly value nature and environment (M = 5.36, SD = 0.66, α = 0.893), but do not see themselves as people who act pro-environmentally (Lorenzoni et al., 2007; Gössling et al., 2009; see, e.g., van der Werff et al., 2013; Juvan and Dolnicar, 2014). This gap could occur because people do not acknowledge environmental problems or because they do not believe that these problems could or should be mitigated via individual actions and thus deny or displace individual responsibility.

In contrast, the items of latent trait altruism supplement each other (Graph B). The reliability P1 = 0.789 means 78.9% of the variance in the measure of the people who have a high altruistic level (θ = 1) is reliable (Table 3). It is concluded that the subjects of this study are more altruistic (i.e., more concerned for others, prosocial, or selfless).

Why Altruism Does Not Have Association With Past Volunteering in Clean-Up Activities?

As confirmed by GRM, the construct “altruism” was found to be reliable, and it should have, therefore, been found to be statistically significant in predicting the past volunteering in clean-up (Joireman et al., 2001). But, this is not the case (regressions 10 and 12, Table 2). The reason behind this is that whether people volunteer or not is also related to obligation (as payback, social/parental pressures, and need to conform/win approval) (Wiehe and Isenhour, 1977 as cited by Henderson, 1981; Unger, 1991). If this is true, the association between the past volunteering in clean-up and knowledge, attitudes, and behavior toward the environment may be statistically insignificant. Using OLS, I regressed the effect of past volunteering on eight items assessing the knowledge, attitudes, and behavior. Figure 2 clearly shows that there is no statistically significant difference between those subjects who had volunteered or had not volunteered in terms of their environmental understanding, as well as their pro-environment intention and practices. So, this finding is consistent with the past studies, which reported that obligation (i.e., something needs to be done) drives people to volunteer (Wiehe and Isenhour, 1977 as cited by Henderson, 1981; Unger, 1991).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effects of past volunteering in clean-up on pro-environmental knowledge, intention, and practices. ×, Waste-sorting difficulty; •, Time to sort waste; ■, Involvement of neighbors; ▴, Waste collection facilities; ■, Involvement of family members; *, Intention to sort waste; °, Waste-shopping bag usage; □, Burning or burying waste. The plot shows the statistically insignificant effects of volunteering in clean-up activities on environmental knowledge and pro-environmental practices. Full regression results are reported in Supplementary Table 6.

Conclusions

The latent trait self-transcendence (i.e., combining altruism and biosphere) is not found to be reliable and valid until two items (i.e., item 8: It is important we shall have equal opportunity and item 9: It is important we should take care of those who are worse off) are dropped from the model. In this way, its effect on prosocial and pro-environmental volunteering was found to be statistically insignificant, which more likely resulted from the independence of altruism from biosphere (Table 3). Though the high correlation between the two constructs was statistically detected, merging altruistic and biospheric values into self-transcendence dimension appeared to be vague. This result more likely reflects the reasons behind the study of Onuki and Xiao (2020), who found that self-transcendence had no association with volunteering in any domain. At the same time, this result also explains why the latent trait biospheric value (i.e., concerns for environment) did not affect the past volunteering in the environment, while the latent trait altruistic value did affect the past volunteering in social welfare. It is, indeed, because the subjects in this study have a lower level of concern for the environment than others.

Author's Note

I am interested in behavior change in the context of environment. Since my background is more related to environmental education, I look at values-related factors to understand its effect on human behavior change. There are many studies on human values and it seems various findings were found by the same factors such as self-transcendence. With the new method of GRM, we could explore in more detail the underlying reasons behind those findings. In short, my study found the construct 'self-transcendence' which is the combined altruistic and biospheric concern is invalid or less reliable since the two constructs are obviously independent from each other. Pro-environmentalist is more likely different from pro-socialist. However, I suggest to look at more deeply on this issue with bigger population or even in different cultural contexts.

Data Availability Statement

The data used to support the above findings is available from the corresponding author upon a reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The Gratitude should first go to the Cambodian Student Association in Japan (CSAJ) and the Hiroshima Alumni Network for cooperation, otherwise this study would have not been possible. The thankfulness is secondly sent to a friend who voluntarily proofread the manuscript and provided some insightful comments and suggestions. Finally, a special gratefulness is sent to the Samdhana Institute (SI) for sponsoring the publication of this article.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.816793/full#supplementary-material

References

Acock, A. C. (2013). Discovering Structural Equation Modeling Using Stata, Stata Press Books, StataCorp LP, number dsemus, August. In Stata Press Publication StataCorp LP College Station, Texas.

Google Scholar

Acock, A. C. (2018). A Gentle Introduction to Stata. Stata press.

Google Scholar

Bankert, A., Huddy, L., and Rosema, M. (2017). Measuring partisanship as a social identity in multi-party systems. Polit. Behav. 39, 103–132. doi: 10.1007/s11109-016-9349-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, G. S. (1974). A Theory of Social Interactions. J. Political Economy 82:1063–93. doi: 10.3386/w0042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, D., and Fischer, R. (2013). How and when do personal values guide our attitudes and sociality? Explaining cross-cultural variability in attitude-value linkages. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1113–1147. doi: 10.1037/a0031347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouman, T., Steg, L., and Kiers, H. A. L. (2018). Measuring values in environmental research: a test of an environmental Portrait Value Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 9:564. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunsø, K., Scholderer, J., and Grunert, K. G. (2004). Closing the gap between values and behavior - A means-end theory of lifestyle. J. Bus. Res. 57, 665–670. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(02)00310-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelo, N., White, K., and Goode, M. R. (2021). Nature promotes self-transcendence and prosocial behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 76:101639. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, W. Y., Luke, M. A., and Maio, G. R. (2014). On attitudes towards humanity and climate change: the effects of humanity esteem and self-transcendence values on environmental concerns. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 496–506. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crompton, T., and Kasser, T. (2009). Meeting environmental challenges: the role of human identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 535–537. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Groot, J. I. M., and Steg, L. (2007). Value orientations and environmental beliefs in five countries: validity of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic and biospheric value orientations. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 38, 318–332. doi: 10.1177/0022022107300278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Groot, J. I. M., and Steg, L. (2008). Value orientations to explain beliefs related to environmental significant behavior: how to measure egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric value orientations. Environ. Behav. 40, 330–354. doi: 10.1177/0013916506297831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Groot, J. I. M., and Steg, L. (2009). Mean or green: which values can promote stable pro-environmental behavior? Conserv. Lett. 2, 61–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-263X.2009.00048.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, T., Fitzgerald, A., and Shwom, R. (2005). Environmental values. Ann. Rev. Environ. Resour. 30, 335–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.energy.30.050504.144444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominicis, S., de Schultz, P. W., and Bonaiuto, M. (2017). Protecting environment for self interested reasons: altruism Is not the only pathway to sustainability. Front. Psychol. 8:1065. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärling, T., Fujii, S., Gärling, A., and Jakobsson, C. (2003). Moderating effects of social value orientation on determinants of proenvironmental behavior intention. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00081-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gössling, S., Haglund, L., Kallgren, H., Revahl, M., and Hultman, J. (2009). Swedish air travellers and voluntary carbon offsets: towards the co-creation of environmental value? Curr. Issues Tourism 12, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/13683500802220687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansla, A., Gamble, A., Juliusson, A., and Gärling, T. (2008). The relationships between awareness of consequences, environmental concern, and value orientations. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, K. A. (1981). Motivations and perceptions of voluntarism as a leisure activity. Leis. Res. 13, 208–218. doi: 10.1080/00222216.1981.11969484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joireman, J. A., Lasane, T. P., Bennett, J., Richards, D., and Solaimani, S. (2001). Integrating social value orientation and the consideration of future consequences within the extended norm activation model of proenvironmental behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 133–155. doi: 10.1348/014466601164731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juvan, E., and Dolnicar, S. (2014). The attitude-behaviour gap in sustainable tourism. Ann. Tourism Res. 48, 76–95. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2014.05.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kewes, A., and Munsch, C. (2019). Should I Stay or Should I Go? Engaging and disengaging experiences in welfare-sector volunteering. Voluntas 30, 1090–1103. doi: 10.1007/s11266-019-00122-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knez, I. (2016). Is climate change a moral issue? Effects of egoism and altruism on pro-environmental behavior. Curr. Urban Stud. 4, 157–174. doi: 10.4236/cus.2016.42012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K. (2011). The role of media exposure, social exposure and biospheric value orientation in the environmental attitude-intention-behavior model in adolescents. J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 301–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberg, S., and Steg, L. (2013). “Goal-framing theory and norm-guided environmental behavior,” in Encouraging Sustainable Behavior: Psychology and the Environment, ed van Trijp, H. C. M (New York, NY: Encouraging Sustainable Behavior. Psychology Press), 37e54.

Google Scholar

Liobikiene, G., and Juknys, R. (2016). The role of values, environmental risk perception, awareness of consequences, and willingness to assume responsibility for environmentally-friendly behaviour: the Lithuanian case. J. Cleaner Prod. 112, 3413–3422. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2015.10.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzoni, I., Nicholson-Cole, S., and Whitmarsh, L. (2007). Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob. Environ. Change 17, 445–459. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2007.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, G., Cnaan, R. A., and Boehm, A. (2017). Religious attendance and volunteering: testing national culture as a boundary condition. J. Sci. Study Relig. 56, 577–599. doi: 10.1111/jssr.12360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maio, G. R. (2011). Don't Mind the Gap Between Values and Action. Common Cause Briefing Paper.

Google Scholar

Milfont, T. L., and Gouveia, V. V. (2006). Time perspective and values: an exploratory study of their relations to environmental attitudes. J. Environ. Psychol. 26, 72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, A., Hansla, A., Heiling, J. M., Bergstad, C. J., and Martinsson, J. (2016). Public acceptability towards environmental policy measures: value-matching appeals. Environ. Sci. Policy 61, 176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.envsci.2016.04.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, A., von Borgstede, C., and Biel, A. (2004). Willingness to accept climate change strategies: the effect of values and norms. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 267–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordlund, A. M., and Garvill, J. (2002). Value structures behind proenvironmental behavior. Environ. Behav. 34, 740–756. doi: 10.1177/001391602237244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordlund, A. M., and Garvill, J. (2003). Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 339–347. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00037-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okabe, Y., Shiratori, S., and Suda, K. (2017). What Motivates Japan's International Volunteers? Categorizing Japan Overseas Cooperation Volunteers (JOCVs) (No. 158).

Google Scholar

Onuki, M., and Xiao, Y. (2020). Personal Determinants of Volunteering for Former International Volunteers: A Case of Japan Overseas Cooperation Volunteers (No. 201).

Google Scholar

Papagiannakis, G., and Lioukas, S. (2012). Values, attitudes and perceptions of managers as predictors of corporate environmental responsiveness. J. Environ. Manag. 100, 41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2012.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlaviciute, G., and Steg, L. (2014). Contextual and psychological factors shaping evaluations and acceptability of energy alternatives: Integrated review and research agenda. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 35, 361–381. doi: 10.1016/j.rser.2014.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prager, K. (2012). Understanding behaviour change: how to apply theories of behaviour change to SEWeb and related public engagement activitiesKatrin Prager, James Hutton Institute. Scotland's Environ. 4, 1–24.

Google Scholar

Samejima, F. (1969). Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika Monogr. Suppl. 34(4, Pt. 2):100. doi: 10.1007/BF03372160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective talking on concern for environmental issues. J. Soc. Issues 56, 391–406. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2001). The Structure of Environmental Concern: Concern for Self, Other People, and the Biophere.

Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W., and Zelezny, L. (1999). Values as Predictors of Environmental Attitudes: Evidence for Consistency Across 14 Countries.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–65. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings Psychol. Cult. 1:2. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., Bolderdijk, J. W., Keizer, K., and Perlaviciute, G. (2014a). An integrated framework for encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: the role of values, situational factors and goals. J. Environ. Psychol. 38, 104–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and de Groot, J. I. M. (2012). Environmental Values. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199733026.013.0005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., Perlaviciute, G., van der Werff, E., and Lurvink, J. (2014b). The significance of hedonic values for environmentally relevant attitudes, preferences, and actions. Environ. Behav. 46, 163–192. doi: 10.1177/0013916512454730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theory: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues. 56, 407–424. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., and Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environ. Behav. 25, 322–348. doi: 10.1177/0013916593255002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Kalof, L., and Guagnano, G. A. (1995). Values, beliefs, and proenvironmental action: attitude formation toward emergent attitude objects. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1611–1636. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02636.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C, Dietz, T., and Guagnano, G. A. (1998). A brief inventory of values. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 58, 984–1001. doi: 10.1177/0013164498058006008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thissen, D. (2000). “Reliability and measurement precision,” in Computerized Adaptive Testing: A Primer, ed H. Wainer (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 159–184. Available online at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2000-03637-004

Google Scholar

Thomas, C., and Sharp, V. (2013). Understanding the normalisation of recycling behaviour and its implications for other pro-environmental behaviours: a review of social norms and recycling. Resour. Conserv. Recycling 79, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2013.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unger, L. S. (1991). Altruism as a motivation to volunteer. J. Econ. Psychol. 12, 71–100. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(91)90044-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Werff, E., Steg, L., and Keizer, K. (2013). The value of environmental self-identity: The relationship between biospheric values, environmental self-identity and environmental preferences, intentions and behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Riper, C. J., and Kyle, G. T. (2014). Understanding the internal processes of behavioral engagement in a national park: a latent variable path analysis of the value-belief-norm theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 38, 288–297. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiehe, V. R., and Isenhour, L. (1977). Motivation of volunteers. Soc. Welf. 4, 73–79.

Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P., Arslan, G., Bowers, V. L., Peacock, E. J., Kjell, O. N. E., Ivtzan, I., et al. (2021). Self-Transcendence as a buffer against COVID-19 suffering: the development and validation of the Self-Transcendence Measure-B. Front. Psychol. 12:648549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: altruism, biosphere, volunteering, self-transcendence, Graded Response Model (GRM)

Citation: Ros B and Kaneko S (2022) Is Self-Transcendence Philanthropic? Graded Response Model Approach. Front. Psychol. 13:816793. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.816793

Received: 17 November 2021; Accepted: 31 March 2022;
Published: 18 May 2022.

Edited by:

Paul T. P. Wong, Trent University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Victoria L. Bowers, Saybrook University, United States
Elena Shmeleva, Russian State Social University, Russia

Copyright © 2022 Ros and Kaneko. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bandos Ros, cm9zLmJhbmRvcyYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.