- 1Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
- 2Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
- 3School of Humanities and Management, Wannan Medical College, Wuhu, China
In the context of the COVID-19, we examined the relationship between college students’ ego depletion and their prosocial behavior. We explored the mediating role of social self-efficacy between ego depletion and prosocial behavior, we also examined the moderating role of personal belief in a just world in this relationship. 1,122 college students completed the ego depletion questionnaire, prosocial behavior questionnaire, social self-efficacy questionnaire, and personal belief in a just world questionnaire. The current findings suggested that: (1) Social self-efficacy mediated the relationship between college students’ ego depletion and their prosocial behaviors. The ego depletion of college students could be used to predict their prosocial behavior through social self-efficacy. (2) Personal belief in a just world moderated the relationship between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior.
Introduction
Ego depletion is a process in which individual self-control resources are consumed in large quantities. After a period of activities that require self-control resources, self-control ability will be exhausted, and this state is ego depletion (Hagger et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2011; Ding et al., 2020a). The individual’s cognition, emotion, and behavioral issues are the aftereffects of ego depletion (Chen et al., 2011), such as the reduction of prosocial behavior (Fennis, 2011; Osgood and Muraven, 2015; Yi et al., 2021), the increase in online flaming, online cheating behavior and aggressive behavior, and future anxiety (Ding et al., 2020a,b; AlHarbi et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2021a).
The core idea behind ego depletion is that the self’s acts of volition draw on some limited resource, and the effects of ego depletion are maladaptive and detrimental to performance (Baumeister et al., 1998, 2007; Muraven et al., 1998). The strength model of self-control suggests that engaging in initial self-control tasks depletes self-control resources, at least partially, leading to fewer resources being available to perform subsequent tasks (Hagger et al., 2010).
Self-control is related to behaviors that meet social expectations and the pursuit of personal achievement. Based on the strength model of self-control, individual’s self-control resources are limited, when the individual is in a state of ego depletion, the depletion effect will occur (Wang et al., 2021b). Especially, according to the strength model, self-control is a finite resource that determines capacity for effortful control over dominant responses. What is worse, once the limited resource is expended, the individual’s performance in self-control tasks will be seriously weakened (Hagger et al., 2010). Prosocial behavior is often a behavior under the superego standard, which requires people to overcome selfishness and the pursuit of self-interest, and do things that are morally encouraged. This behavior requires individuals to use cognitive control to maintain. The ego depletion will lead to individuals not having enough cognitive control ability to control and restrain their behavior, which will lead to the decline of the individual’s preference for prosocial behavior.
Many studies have proved the relation between ego depletion and prosocial behavior. For example, Osgood and Muraven (2015) found that ego depletion can damage individual’s prosocial behavior by reducing their ability or motivation to overcome self-desires. Furthermore, Fei et al. (2016) recruited 58 college students and adopted the dual-task paradigm of ego depletion to investigate the effect of ego depletion on the prosocial behaviors. The results showed that college students with a high degree of ego depletion showed fewer prosocial behaviors than college students with a low degree of ego depletion. Fennis (2011) also found that ego depletion can reduce college students’ prosocial behavior of perspective taking (as a specific form of prosocial behavior). Ren et al. (2014) used the stroop task to generate ego depletion to manipulate the level of self-control of college students and observed whether the prosocial behaviors in the dictator game was affected by ego depletion. The experimental results showed that compared with the subjects in the non-ego depletion group, those in the ego depletion group showed fewer prosocial behaviors.
In addition, the negative emotions caused by the COVID-19 pandemic would further increase the level of people’s ego depletion (Caldas et al., 2021; Robert and Vandenberghe, 2021). Robert and Vandenberghe conducted a study of 650 civil servants in the Quebec government during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic. They examined the effect of anxiety caused by the COVID-19 pandemic on ego depletion, which found that the anxiety caused by the COVID-19 pandemic was positively correlated with ego depletion. It could be seen that individuals are more prone to ego depletion during the COVID-19 pandemic, and more ego depletion may lead to fewer prosocial behaviors. Nevertheless, society needs people to show more prosocial behaviors to jointly resist the COVID-19 during the pandemic. Therefore, it is particularly important to investigate the impact of ego depletion on prosocial behavior during the pandemic of COVID-19.
When individuals have cognitive loss, they are more likely to question their psychological capital, such as self-efficacy, and will subjectively think that they do not have enough efficacy to act. There are some studies promoting the association (Chow et al., 2015), while others find no significant association (Hagger et al., 2010). Chow et al. (2015) used three experiments to suggest that ego depletion undermines self-control by reducing the motivation (an important protective factor of self-control) to mobilize cognitive resources. One potential cognitive mechanism is the reduction of self-efficacy. Specifically, ego depletion could demotivate self-control by making people believe that they are inefficacious in exerting self-control in subsequent tasks (Chow et al., 2015). In other words, ego depletion can reduce the individual’s self-efficacy. However, Hagger et al. (2010) did not find a relationship between self-efficacy and ego depletion. This may be because self-efficacy was the independent variable and ego depletion was the dependent variable in their study. In Chow et al. (2015)‘s study, ego depletion was the independent variable and self-efficacy was the dependent variable. It can be speculated that ego depletion will lead to the decline of self-efficacy, but the level of self-efficacy cannot affect ego depletion. Social self-efficacy is a manifestation of self-efficacy in the social field, which is a special internal resource. Social self-efficacy affects the establishment and maintenance of individual interpersonal relationships in social situations, as well as the application and exertion of their interpersonal abilities (Gu et al., 2014; Zheng et al., 2019). Based on this, we hypothesized that ego depletion would be linked with social self-efficacy.
The theory of social cognition proposes that the human self-system can play a role in controlling and regulating behavior and can affect people’s choice of behavioral activities and social environment, as well as cognitive and behavioral methods (Gong et al., 2021). Individuals with high self-efficacy will recognize themselves more, compare their existing knowledge and experience with the current situation, and believe that they have enough ability to solve the problems in a positive way. Underestimate the damage or believe that the benefits outweigh the disadvantages, making individuals more inclined to perform prosocial behaviors (Gong et al., 2021). The positive connection between self-efficacy and prosocial behavior has been confirmed and verified in numerous studies. Deng et al. (2018) have conducted a questionnaire survey on 768 junior high school students from Grade one to Grade three in Shandong province and Chongqing province, their results indicated that self-efficacy was the most predictive of prosocial behavior. Gong et al. (2021) have selected 320 college students for investigation and research and found that the higher the self-efficacy of college students, the easier it is for them to implement prosocial behaviors. Patrick et al. (2018) concluded that social self-efficacy could be associated with certain types of prosocial behavior in a survey of a sample of 338 adolescents, which is considered to provide confidence for adolescents to participate in prosocial behavior. Furthermore, as mentioned above, studies have found that self-efficacy plays a mediating role in the impact of ego depletion on self-control (Chow et al., 2015). Based on above analysis, we speculated that social self-efficacy was positively related to individual’s prosocial behavior. In summary, social self-efficacy may play a mediating role between ego depletion and prosocial behavior.
Individuals need to believe in a just world (BJW) in which everybody gets what they deserve, because this enables them to deal with their physical and social environment, as if it were stable and orderly (Lerner, 1980; Dalbert and Stoeber, 2006). Individuals with high level of BJW are better able to cope with the anger-evoking situations, and BJW can be seen as a personal resource to protect not only mental but also physical health (Dalbert, 2002). Personal belief in a just world is an important dimension of belief in a just world, which means that individuals believe that the world is fair to them and that they can be treated fairly (Lipkus et al., 1996; Zhou and Guo, 2013; Liu et al., 2020a). General belief in a just world refers to people’s belief that the world is just in a general sense (Dalbert, 1999; Tian et al., 2019). It has been shown that individuals tend to endorse personal BJW more strongly than general BJW (Dalbert, 1999; Dalbert and Stoeber, 2006). Personal BJW is positively correlated with willingness to help others. Individuals who hold a high personal BJW have more confidence in the future and are more inclined to use fair means to achieve their goals (Ji et al., 2014). Whereas, general BJW is positively correlated with severe social attitudes and social discrimination, and negatively correlated with helping behavior (Bègue et al., 2008; Ji et al., 2014). Therefore, current study focus on the role of personal BJW in prosocial behavior.
If an individual has strong personal BJW, he will firmly believe that his current contributions will not be rewarded immediately, but he will be rewarded in the future. So when someone asks for help, he is more likely to act prosocial (Cook and Rice, 1987). From the perspective of fairness, only by helping others can you get help from others when you need it. This could suggest that the stronger personal BJW, the stronger his willingness to help others. In other words, individuals who hold strong personal beliefs in a just world are more likely to perform prosocial behaviors. Quan (2020) analyzed a sample of 960 adults who completed the prosocial behavior questionnaire and the personal just world belief questionnaire and found that personal BJW is more related to prosocial behavior. The higher personal BJW, the greater tendency of individuals to implement prosocial behaviors. Moreover, social self-efficacy can enhance the confidence of individuals to implement prosocial behaviors (Patrick et al., 2018). That is to say, personal BJW could enhance the positive connection between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior (Figure 1).
Materials and Methods
Procedure and Participants
An online survey was conducted among 1,189 college students. We deleted data for participants who completed the questionnaires within 3 min and who did not complete the questionnaires. And, 1,122 college students (including 493 full-time college students and 629 part-time college students) completed these questionnaires in Central China during the COVID-19 pandemic period (April–May 2021). Participants were informed that they could terminate their participation in the online questionnaire at any time. Moreover, participants were also informed that the online survey followed the principles of anonymity, confidentiality, and independence, and the data will only be used for academic survey.
There were 293 male college students and 829 female college students; 407 first-year university students, 216 s-year university students, 206 third-year university students, and 293 fourth-year university students. The average age of the participants was 24.11 ± 5.96 years old.
Measurement
Ego Depletion Scale
We used the ego depletion scale compiled by Nes et al. (2013) and revised by Wang et al. (2015) to measure the ego depletion level. This scale includes 16 items with rating on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated greater levels of ego depletion. A sample item is: “I find it difficult to exercise as much as I should.” This ego depletion scale was widely used among college students in China (Chen and Xiao, 2020; Ho et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2021b). In current study, the Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.79.
Prosocial Behavior Scale
We used the prosocial behavior scale compiled by Carlo and Randall (2002) and revised by Kou et al. (2007) to measure the prosocial behavior tendency of college students. This scale includes 26 items with rating on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (totally inconsistent) to 5 (totally consistent). Higher score indicated stronger levels of prosocial behavior. A sample item is: “I think that helping others without them knowing is the best type of situation.” This prosocial behavior scale was often used among college students in China (Liu et al., 2020b; Zhang et al., 2020b; Fang and Chang, 2021; Gao et al., 2021; Lv and Zhou, 2021). In current study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.93.
Perceived Social Self-Efficacy Scale
The social self-efficacy scale was first compiled by Smith and Betz (2000), which revised by Meng et al. (2007) in China. We used the revised version of social self-efficacy to measure participants’ social self-efficacy. This scale includes 18 items with rating on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (no confidence at all) to 5 (very confidence). Higher scores indicated greater levels of social self-efficacy. A sample item is: “Ask a group of people who you do not know and who are engaging in a social activity if you can join them.” This perceived social self-efficacy scale was widely used among college students in China (Meng et al., 2007, 2012; Liu et al., 2020c; Zhang et al., 2020a). In current study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.96.
Personal Belief in a Just World Scale
Participants’ personal beliefs in a just world were measured by using the personal BJW scale compiled by Dalbert and Stoeber (2006)and revised by Su et al. (2012). This scale includes 7 items with rating on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 (totally disagree) to 6 (totally agree). Higher scores indicated stronger levels of personal BJW. A sample item is: “I am usually treated fairly.” This personal beliefs in a just world scale was widely used among college students in China (Tian et al., 2017; Li et al., 2019; Chen, 2021; Wang et al., 2021c). In current study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.86.
Results
Control and Inspection of Common Method Deviation
The data collected in this survey mainly came from the self-reports of participants, which may cause common method deviations. We adopted procedural control methods to minimize the impact of common method deviations, such as anonymous questionnaire surveys and balanced item order. In addition, in order to further examine whether common method bias exists, we used the Harman single factor method to carry out the common method deviation test. There were a total of 11 factors whose characteristic roots were greater than 1, and the explanatory rate of the first factor was 16.47% (<40%), which indicated that the common method bias was not serious (Podsakoff et al., 2012).
Preliminary Analyses
We have reported the mean, standard deviation, and correlation coefficients of ego depletion, social self-efficacy, personal BJW, and prosocial behavior in Table 1. From the data summarized in the table, it could be seen that the participants’ ego depletion was significantly negatively correlated with their social self-efficacy, personal BJW, and prosocial behavior. Participants’ social self-efficacy was significantly positively correlated with personal BJW and prosocial behavior. Moreover, the participants’ personal BJW was significantly positively correlated with prosocial behavior. Post-hoc Power analysis found that statistical test power was greater than 0.99.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis of the variables involved in this measurement (N = 1,122).
The Relationship Between College Students’ Ego Depletion and Their Prosocial Behavior: A Test of Mediation Effect
We used the PROCESS 3.4 (Hayes, 2013) to analyze the mediation model. Ego depletion was used as the independent variable, social self-efficacy was used as the mediating variable, and prosocial behavior was used as the dependent variable. We used Model 4 and Bootstrap method (sample size is 5,000, 95% confidence interval) to test the significance of the mediation effect (see Table 2).
Ego depletion is significantly negatively associated with social self-efficacy (β = −0.25, t = −8.58, p < 0.001), and social self-efficacy is significantly positively linked to prosocial behavior (β = 0.32, t = 11.00, p < 0.001). Furthermore, ego depletion is still significantly negatively connected with the participants’ prosocial behavior (β = −0.12, t = −4.16, p < 0.001). Post-hoc Power analysis found that statistical test power was greater than 0.99. This result suggested that social self-efficacy played a mediating role between ego depletion and prosocial behavior of the participants. For further verification, we also draw 5,000 samples to estimate the 95% confidence interval of the mediation effect. The indirect effect of ego depletion on prosocial behavior, that is, the Bootstrap 95% confidence interval of ego depletion on social self-efficacy was [−0.31, −0.18] (alpha = 0.05), the Bootstrap 95% confidence interval of social self-efficacy for prosocial behavior was [0.25, 0.38] (alpha = 0.05). In summary, the interval did not contain a value of 0, indicating that social self-efficacy had a significant mediating effect in ego depletion and prosocial behavior among participants. Moreover, the Bootstrap 95% confidence interval of the direct effect of ego depletion on prosocial behavior is [−0.24, −0.13] (alpha = 0.05), and the interval did not contain a value of 0. This shows that participants’ social self-efficacy could play a mediating role between ego depletion and prosocial behavior. And, total effect of ego depletion on prosocial behavior is −0.20 (t = −6.77, p < 0.001), direct effect of ego depletion on prosocial behavior is −0.12 (t = −4.16, p < 0.001).
Test of the Moderating Effect of Personal BJW Between Social Self-Efficacy and Prosocial Behavior
We use PROCESS 3.4 (Hayes, 2013) to further analyze the moderating effect of personal BJW. Ego depletion was used as the independent variable, social self-efficacy was used as the mediator, prosocial behavior was used as the dependent variable, and personal BJW was used as the moderator. And we running Model 14 and Bootstrap method (sample size is 5,000, 95% confidence interval) to test the moderating effect of personal BJW. The interaction of personal BJW and social self-efficacy was significantly related to college students’ prosocial behavior (β = 0.08, t = 3.13, p < 0.01, bootstrap 95% confidence interval was [0.02, 0.13], alpha = 0.05, see Table 3), indicating that personal BJW moderated the relationship between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior. R2 = 0.23, F = 84.51, p < 0.001. Post-hoc Power analysis found that statistical test power was greater than 0.99. So, personal BJW had a significant moderating effect between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior. And direct effect of ego depletion on prosocial behavior is −0.03 (t = − 1.13, p = 0.26), index of moderated mediation is −0.02([−0.03, −0.00]; 95% confidence interval, alpha = 0.05).
Participants were divided into a lower group (Z = −1) and a higher group (Z = 1) based on the standard scores of personal BJW. A simple slope test was used to examine the impact of social self-efficacy on prosocial behaviors of participants who hold different levels of personal BJW. Participants who hold a lower level of personal BJW, social self-efficacy could predict prosocial behavior significantly (βsimple = 0.15, t = 4.00, p < 0.001, Bootstrap 95% confidence interval was [0.08, 0.23], alpha = 0.05); Participants who hold a higher level of personal BJW, their social self-efficacy would be connected with prosocial behavior more significant (βsimple = 0.30, t = 8.68, p < 0.001, Bootstrap 95% confidence interval was [0.24, 0.37], alpha = 0.05). On the whole, the mediating role of social self-efficacy in ego depletion and prosocial behavior was moderated by the level of participants’ BJW (Figure 2).
Discussion
The current study revealed the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behavior of college students during the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as the mediating role of social self-efficacy and the moderating role of personal BJW through a moderated mediation model.
The current study found that social self-efficacy mediates the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behavior, which is consistent with the Strength Model of Self-control. Based on the strength model of self-control, self-control resources are limited. When individual is in a state of self-depletion, individuals would have ego depletion effect (Wang et al., 2021b). Our study also found a negative correlation between self-depletion and self-efficacy. The more self-depletion, the lower the self-efficacy, which is consistent with previous research results (Chow et al., 2015). Moreover, based on the theory of social cognition (human self-system could play a role in controlling and regulating behavior, and can influence its choice of behavioral activities and behavior; Gong et al., 2021), individuals with high self-efficacy would consider themselves competent enough to solve the problems encountered in a positive way, and thus are more inclined to perform prosocial behaviors (Gong et al., 2021). Our investigation of the positive relationship between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic was more consistent with previous research conclusions (Deng et al., 2018; Patrick et al., 2018; Gong et al., 2021). For example, research conducted by Patrick et al. (2018)‘s study suggested that social self-efficacy could be associated with certain types of prosocial behaviors. Thus we can see that social self-efficacy plays an indispensable role in enhancing prosocial behaviors. Therefore, improving the establishment and maintenance of individual interpersonal relationships in social situations and the application and exertion of interpersonal skills can increase the possibility of prosocial behaviors. In short, college students’ ego depletion can be associated with their prosocial behaviors through their social self-efficacy during the COVID-19 pandemic.
The moderating effect of personal BJW between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior was also verified. Current research suggested that when individuals hold strong personal BJW, the positive connection between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior was stronger. At a motivational level, personal BJW is related to social goals that require the suspension of immediate self-interest. Personal BJW shows associations with the human motivational values of benevolence, as well as a desire to learn more about others, talk about feelings, and make others feel better (Bartholomaeus and Strelan, 2019). If someone asks for help, individuals are more likely to be willing to perform prosocial behavior, when they have the confidence to participate in prosocial actions (Patrick et al., 2018). We may also be able to understand the moderating effect of personal BJW between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior from the perspective of the resource conservation model. The model of resource conservation stated that people strive to retain, protect, and build resources, and that what is threatening to them is the potential or actual loss of these valued resources (Hobfoll, 1989). That is to say, individuals already possess some resources and will often strive to acquire, maintain, and protect resources that they consider valuable (Hu and Shen, 2020). Individuals with more resources are less susceptible to resource loss attacks and are more capable of obtaining resources, thus forming a value-added spiral (Hobfoll et al., 2003; Hu and Shen, 2020). Prosocial behavior is often an act of helping others at the expense of one’s own interests. Therefore, from the perspective of personal material gain, prosocial behavior is often accompanied by losses (Schwartz, 2010; Patrick et al., 2018). Personal BJW can be understood as additional resource making this losses more bearable. In conclusion, personal BJW can enhance the positive connection between social self-efficacy and prosocial behavior. This result suggests that we should pay attention to the difference in the strength of personal BJW when intervening to improve individuals’ prosocial behavior tendencies by enhancing individual social self-efficacy.
Our investigation has enriched the application of the strength model of self-control and the theory of social cognition in the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic, which has positive enlightening significance for how to enhance people’ s prosocial behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic.
We can start by reducing the ego depletion of college students. Based on the ego depletion theory, when individuals modify the way they think, feel, or behave to adapt to societal norms and expectations, they draw from a limited pool of regulatory resources (Baumeister et al., 2000; Caldas et al., 2021). If individuals draw from this pool too much, this results in resource depletion that ultimately “renders the self temporarily less able and less willing to function normally or optimally” (Baumeister et al., 2007; Caldas et al., 2021). We need to take concrete action to mitigate the ego depletion among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. For instance, we can encourage college students to actively participate in repeated practice on self-control tasks (e.g., regulating mood and monitoring eating habits), which have been proved to be effective in alleviating ego depletion (Hagger et al., 2010).
Limitations
In addition, this survey could still be improved in the following aspects: Firstly, Since this survey was a cross-sectional study, it could not make causal inferences about the relationship among college students’ ego depletion, prosocial behaviors, social self-efficacy, and personal BJW. Secondly, what we reveal was the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behaviors of college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. This kind of prosocial behavior is holistic, and the relationship between ego depletion and specific dimensions of prosocial behavior (such as anonymity) would be investigated in the future, which will provide us with more detailed information about the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behavior. Thirdly, we have discussed the mediating role of social self-efficacy, a sense of self-efficacy in the social field, between ego depletion and prosocial behavior. The possible role of other special self-efficacy between ego depletion and prosocial behavior still has value to study. Finally, the discussion on the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic was mainly based on college students. The research objects could be further expanded to increase the reliability of the conclusions.
Conclusion
College students’ ego depletion could reduce their prosocial behaviors, which has been confirmed by many studies during regular periods (Fennis, 2011; Ren et al., 2014; Osgood and Muraven, 2015; Fei et al., 2016). The current study further enriches this result with an online questionnaire survey on the relationship between ego depletion and prosocial behaviors of college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. The current study found that the ego depletion of college students could be linked to their prosocial behaviors through social self-efficacy. Moreover, the stronger the personal BJW held by college students, the more significant the connection between their social self-efficacy and prosocial behaviors.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Academic Committee of Wannan Medical College. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
LH and LL designed the survey. HL, CY and GW collected data. LL wrote the manuscript. LH and CY revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by the Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project of Anhui Provincial (Grants No. AHSKQ2019D059).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
AlHarbi, B., Ibrahim, K., Al-Rababaah, J., and Al-mehsin, S. (2021). The ego depletion and its relationship with the future anxiety among the university female students. Int. J. High. Educ. 10, 128–139. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v10n2p128
Bartholomaeus, J., and Strelan, P. (2019). The adaptive, approach-oriented correlates of belief in a just world for the self: a review of the research. Personality and Individual Differences 151:109485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.06.028
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1252–1265. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion: a resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social Cognition 18, 130–150. doi: 10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.130
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
Bègue, L., Charmoillaux, M., Cochet, J., Cury, C., and de Suremain, F. (2008). Altruistic behavior and the bidimensional just world belief. Am. J. Psychol. 121, 47–56. doi: 10.2307/20445443
Caldas, M., Ostermeier, K., and Cooper, D. (2021). When helping hurts: COVID-19 critical incident involvement and resource depletion in health care workers. J. Appl. Psychol. 106, 29–47. doi: 10.1037/apl0000850
Carlo, G., and Randall, B. A. (2002). The development of a measure of prosocial behaviors for late adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 31, 31–44. doi: 10.1023/A:1014033032440
Chen, Y. M. (2021). The relationship between social trauma and undergraduates’ subjective well-being: The mediating role of personal belief in justice and life meaning. Univ. China Educ. J. 3, 169–180.
Chen, X., and Xiao, Z. J. (2020). The relationship between self-regulatory fatigue and mobile phone addiction of college students: The mediating effect of negative emotion and fun seeking. J. Neijiang Normal Univ. 35, 7–11. doi: 10.13603/j.cnki.51-1621/z.2020.04.002
Chen, Y. Y., Xu, Y., Yang, H. K., and Guo, L. (2011). Aftereffects of ego depletion: evidence from self-control strength model. J. Normal Univ. 6, 14–20. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0209.2011.06.002
Chow, J. T., Hui, C. M., and Lau, S. (2015). A depleted mind feels inefficacious: ego-depletion reduces self-efficacy to exert further self-control. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 45, 754–768. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2120
Cook, K. S., and Rice, E. (1987). “Social exchange theory,” in Handbook of Social Psychology. eds. J. DeLamater and A. Ward (Dordrecht: Springer), 53–76.
Dalbert, C. (1999). The world is more just for me than generally: About the personal belief in a just world scale’s validity. Soc. Justice Res 12, 79–98. doi: 10.1023/A:1022091609047
Dalbert, C. (2002). Beliefs in a just world as a buffer against anger. Soc. Justice Res 15, 123–145. doi: 10.1023/A:1019919822628
Dalbert, C., and Stoeber, J. (2006). The personal belief in a just world and domain-specific beliefs about justice at school and in the family: A longitudinal study with adolescents. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 30, 200–207. doi: 10.1177/0165025406063638
Deng, L. Y., Li, B. L., Wu, Y. X., Xu, R., and Jin, P. P. (2018). The effect of family environment for helping behavior in middle school students: mediation of self-efficacy and empathy. J. Normal Univ. 5, 83–91.
Ding, Q., Liu, Y. L., Zhang, Y. X., and Zhou, Z. K. (2020a). Psychological entitlement and online cheating behavior in college students: A moderated mediation model. Stu. Psychol. Behav. 18, 537–543.
Ding, Q., Zhang, Y. X., and Zhou, Z. K. (2020b). Relative deprivation and college students’ online flaming: mediating effect of ego depletion and gender difference. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 36, 200–207. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.09
Fang, J. D., and Chang, B. R. (2021). The disadvantage of material desire: The influence of materialism on prosocial behaviors and its mechanism. J. Southwest Univ. 43, 157–165. doi: 10.13718/j.cnki.xdzk.2021.12.018
Fei, D. Z., Qian, D. H., and Huang, X. C. (2016). The self-control process model of altruistic behavior: The positive effect of moral emotions under the ego depletion. Acta Psychol. Sin. 48, 1175–1183. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01175
Fennis, B. M. (2011). Can’t get over me: ego depletion attenuates prosocial effects of perspective taking. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 580–585. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.828
Gao, L., Hua, S. Q., Li, P. P., Yang, J. P., and Wang, X. C. (2021). The effect of sibling relationship on adolescents’ prosocial behavior: The roles of moral disengagement and empathy. Stu. Psychol. Behav. 19, 216–222.
Gong, Y., Mao, F. Q., Xia, Y. W., Zhang, T., Wang, G., and Wang, X. (2021). Mediating role of psychological security between college students’ self-efficacy and prosocial tendency. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 29, 146–151. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2021.01.027
Gu, J. N., Meng, H., and Fan, J. Y. (2014). Social self-efficacy: dimension, measurement and its mechanisms. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1791–1800. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01791
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression Based Approach. United States: The Guilford Press.
Ho, M. Y., Van Tongeren, D. R., and You, J. (2020). The role of self-regulation in forgiveness: A regulatory model of forgiveness. Front. Psychol. 11:1084. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01084
Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 44, 513–524. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513
Hobfoll, S. E., Johnson, R. J., Ennis, N., and Jackson, A. P. (2003). Resource loss, resource gain, and emotional outcomes among inner city women. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 632–643. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.632
Hu, S. M., and Shen, C. G. (2020). Effect of perceived employability on active job-seeking behavior and subjective well-being of university graduates: A moderated mediation model. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 28, 126–131.
Ji, W. H., Zhang, L. G., and Kou, Y. (2014). How the belief in a just world influence college student’s intention to help people in need: The role of attribution of responsibility and the cost of helping. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 30, 496–503. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2014.05.019
Kou, Y., Hong, H. F., Tan, C., and Li, L. (2007). Revisioning prosocial tendencies measure for adolescent. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 23, 112–117. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2007.01.020
Li, X., Hou, M. L., Wu, X. L., and Qin, H. X. (2019). The influence of college students’ personal justice world belief on their emergency helping tendency: The mediating effect of empathy. J. Sci. Technol. Normal Univ. 1, 104–110. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-3558.2019.01.016
Lipkus, I. M., Dalbert, C., and Siegler, I. C. (1996). The importance of distinguishing the belief in a just world for self versus for others: implication for psychological well-being. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 666–677. doi: 10.1177/0146167296227002
Liu, X. Y., Jin, Y. C., and An, J. X. (2020c). Social support and loneliness in young adults: A moderated mediation model. J. Psychol. Sci. 43, 586–592. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.02.03
Liu, G. Z., Zhang, D. J., Zhu, Z. G., Li, J. J., and Chen, X. (2020a). The effect of family socioeconomic status on adolescents’ problem behaviors: The chain mediating role of parental emotional warmth and belief in a just world. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 36, 240–248. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.13
Liu, Q., Zhao, F., and Zhang, S. Y. (2020b). Considering the reciprocal relationship between meaning in life and prosocial behavior: A cross-lagged analysis. J. Psychol. Sci. 43, 1438–1445. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20200623
Lv, G. Q., and Zhou, Y. (2021). The mediating effect of prosocial behaviors and basic psychological needs on the relationship between eudaimonia orientation and happiness. Psychol. Tech. App. 9, 95–101.
Meng, H., Fan, J. Y., and Liu, J. (2007). Goal orientation and adjustment: The mediating role of social self-efficacy. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 23, 54–58.
Meng, H., Yang, Z., Xu, L., and Tang, C. (2012). Undergraduates’ social self-efficacy and academic achievements: The moderating role of adjustment. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 20, 552–555. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2012.04.014
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., and Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 774–789. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774
Nes, L. S., Ehlers, S. L., Whipple, M. O., and Vincent, A. (2013). Self-regulatory fatigue in chronic multisymptom illnesses: scale development, fatigue, and self-control. J. Pain Res. 6, 181–188. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S40014
Osgood, J., and Muraven, M. (2015). Self-control depletion does not diminish attitudes about being prosocial but does diminish prosocial behaviors. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 68–80. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2014.996225
Patrick, R., Bodine, A., Gibbs, J., and Basinger, K. (2018). What accounts for prosocial behavior? Roles of moral identity, moral judgment, and self-efficacy beliefs. J. Genet. Psychol. 179, 231–245. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2018.1491472
Podsakoff, P. M., Mac Kenzie, S. B., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 539–569. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100452
Quan, S. (2020). Socioeconomic status and prosocial behaviors among Chinese emerging adults: sequential mediators of parental warmth and personal belief in a just world. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 120:105680. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105680
Ren, J., Li, R. X., Zhan, J., Liu, D., Lin, M., and Peng, N. Q. (2014). Can good people commit evil acts? Evidence of ego-depletion on individuals’ altruistic behavior. Acta Psychol. Sin. 46, 841–851. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00841
Robert, V., and Vandenberghe, C. (2021). Anxiety related to the COVID-19: An examination of its effects in workplaces. Psychol. Travail Org. 27, 3–16. doi: 10.1016/j.pto.2021.01.005
Schwartz, S. H. (2010). “Basic values: how they motivate and inhibit prosocial behavior,” in Prosocial Motives, Emotions, and Behavior: The Better Angels of our Nature. eds. M. Mikulincer and P. R. Shaver (United States: American Psychological Association), 221–241.
Smith, H. M., and Betz, N. E. (2000). Development and validation of a scale of perceived social self-efficacy. J. Career Assess. 8, 283–301. doi: 10.1177/106907270000800306
Su, Z. Q., Zhang, D. J., Wang, X. Q., and Wang, J. L. (2012). Research on the relationship between belief in a just world and prosocial tendencies of college students. Chin. J. Behav. Med. Brain Sci. 21, 433–435. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1674-6554.2012.05.015
Tian, Z. P., Liu, A. S., and Li, J. (2017). Mediating effect of belief in a just world and self-esteem on relationship between childhood abuse and subjective well-being in college students. Chin. Ment. Health J. 31, 312–318. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2017.04.011
Tian, Y., Song, Y. Z., Zhou, Z. K., Huang, C., and Zhao, Y. Q. (2019). A multilevel analysis of the developmental trajectory of belief in a just world in adolescence and its predictors. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 35, 146–156. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.02.03
Wang, C. Y., Lei, L., and Qiao, X. (2021b). The mediating effects of pursuing pleasure between self-regulatory fatigue and smartphone addiction among undergraduate students: The moderating effects of connectedness to nature. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 37, 601–608. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.04.17
Wang, C. Y., Yu, Q. T., and Ji, S. H. (2020). Will fatigue lead to a tendency of money worship? The influence of self-regulatory fatigue on materialism. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 28, 591–595. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.03.032
Wang, L. G., Zhang, J. Y., Wang, J., Tao, T., Fan, C. L., and Gao, W. B. (2015). Validity and reliability of the Chinese version of the self-regulatory fatigue scale in young adults. Chin. Ment. Health J. 29, 290–294.
Wang, X. X., Zhong, Y., and Zhang, Y. (2021a). The effect of self-depletion on aggressive behavior: The role of restorative environments. Stu. Psychol. Behav. 19, 137–144.
Wang, H., Zhou, L., and Lei, L. (2021c). Using shattered assumption theory to understand how cyberbullying victimization is linked with perceived control among Chinese college students. J. Interpers. Violence, 8862605211042572. doi: 10.1177/08862605211042572 [Epub Ahead of Print]
Yi, S. X., Hao, F., and Liu, C. J. (2021). The impact of activation of money concept and preference for monetary function on altruism follow Chening ego depletion. J. Psychol. Sci. 44, 954–959. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20210426
Zhang, X., Chen, S., and Bzi, B. Y. (2020a). The effect of nostalgia on meaning in life: mediating role of social self-efficacy. Psychol. Tech. App. 8, 552–558. doi: 10.16842/j.cnki.issn2095-5588.2020.09.006
Zhang, Y., Yang, X., Liu, D., and Wang, Z. (2020b). Chinese college students’ parental attachment, peer attachment, and prosocial behaviors: The moderating role of respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Biol. Psychol. 150:107844. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107844
Zheng, X. X., Chen, J., Luo, Y., and Meng, H. (2019). The relationship between social self-efficacy and job burnout: An adjusted mediation model-From the perspective of social exchange. J. Psychol. Sci. 42, 350–357. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20190214
Keywords: ego depletion, prosocial behavior, social self-efficacy, personal belief in a just world, college students, the COVID-19 pandemic
Citation: Li L, Liu H, Wang G, Chen Y and Huang L (2022) The Relationship Between Ego Depletion and Prosocial Behavior of College Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Role of Social Self-Efficacy and Personal Belief in a Just World. Front. Psychol. 13:801006. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.801006
Edited by:
Daniela Rabellino, Western University, CanadaReviewed by:
Elisabetta Sagone, University of Catania, ItalyNina Reinhardt, University of Kassel, Germany
Copyright © 2022 Li, Liu, Wang, Chen and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yun Chen, Nzg1MTAxNTkyQHFxLmNvbQ==; Long Huang, bG9uZ2h1YW5nMTlAMTI2LmNvbQ==