Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 July 2022
Sec. Forensic and Legal Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Social Psychological Process And Effects On The Law View all 14 articles

A Positive Versus Negative Interaction Memory Affects Parole Officers’ Implicit Associations Between the Self-Concept and the Group Parolees

  • 1Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Hackensack, NJ, United States
  • 2School of Criminal Justice, Rutgers University-Newark, Newark, NJ, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, Rutgers University-Newark, Newark, NJ, United States

Background: Parole officers are one of many actors in the legal system charged with interpreting and enforcing the law. Officers not only assure that parolees under their supervision comply with the terms of their release, but also monitor and control parolees’ criminal behavior. They conduct their jobs through their understanding of their official mandate and make considered and deliberate choices while executing that mandate. However, their experiences as legal actors may impact their implicit cognitions about parolees. This experiment is the first of its kind to examine implicit (i.e., automatic) associations between the self and parolees among actors of the legal system.

Objective: The present study examines the implicit cognitive consequences of the quality of the parole officer-parolee relationship from the perspective of the parole officer; specifically, whether parole officers who are reminded of positive experiences with parolees implicitly associate more with the group parolees than those reminded of a negative experience. In addition, we explore the moderating effects of parole officers’ subjective professional orientation and identification.

Method: Eighty-four New Jersey parole officers participated in the study. First, an experimental manipulation of either a past positive or negative experience was administered via a writing task. Participants then completed an Implicit Association Test (IAT) to measure associations between the self-concept of parole officers with parolees who are part of the group criminal, followed by measures of professional orientation and identification.

Results: Participants who were reminded of a positive experience with a parolee exhibited stronger associations between self and the group parolee when compared to those who were reminded of a negative experience. Neither professional orientation nor parole officer group identification were related to implicit associations and did not moderate the effect of the past experience reminder on implicit associations.

Conclusion and Implications: Implicit cognitions of parole officers may influence their behaviors and interactions with those whom they supervise. Positive reminders affect implicit self-associations with parolees presumably via empathy, which is known to affect the quality of therapeutic and supervision relationships; thus, theoretically, leading to improved outcomes for both officers and parolees.

Introduction

We say, “justice is blind,” yet we know that extra-legal factors (e.g., race, gender) are often associated with various law-related decisions, including arrests (e.g., Fielding-Miller et al., 2020), verdicts and sentencing (e.g., Cohen and Yang, 2019), and parole release decisions (e.g., Huebner and Bynum, 2008). Much of the extant research assumes that decisions are arrived at in a thoughtful considered way, and, despite this, that bias often influences these decisions. Bias, of course, can be a conscious cognitive process, and, importantly, it also can operate automatically outside of conscious awareness (for reviews, see Gawronski and Payne, 2011). Automatic or implicit cognitions, such as bias, also may affect interactions between actors in the justice system (e.g., between citizens and police officers, parolees and parole officers). Only recently has implicit social cognition been studied in samples of justice-involved people (Rivera and Veysey, 2015, 2018; Veysey and Rivera, 2017). The present study is the first to our knowledge to examine implicit self-cognitions among actors in the justice system. Using a sample of parole officers, we investigate the implicit association of the self with the group parolee following a positive (versus negative) interaction with a parolee. We suggest that this effect occurs via self-expansion, a mechanism that supports positive downstream outcomes for both parole officers and parolees.

Parole in the United States

At year-end 2019, there were 878,900 individuals under parole supervision (Oudekerk and Kaeble, 2021). Historically, and to a large degree today, the role of the parole officer is to assist in the successful reentry of individuals who have been released from prison while monitoring their behavior and the terms of their release (e.g., maintaining employment and stable housing, abstaining from alcohol and drugs); in sum, responding to their criminogenic and non-criminogenic needs to ensure community safety (Seiter, 2002; National Research Council, 2008).

In the parole officer-parolee relationship, parole officers may function as agents of change who encourage parolees to comply with the conditions of parole, engage in specialized programs, and promote pro-social behaviors and identities (Gibbons and Rosecrance, 2005; Abadinsky, 2009) or alternatively, parole officers may foster an adversarial relationships with their parolees (Ireland and Berg, 2007; Morash et al., 2014; Chamberlain et al., 2017). The effectiveness of the parole officer-parolee relationship is dependent on the parole officer’s ability to create and maintain positive relationships with parolees on their caseloads (Dowden and Andrews, 2004; Landenberger and Lipsey, 2005; Jolliffe and Farrington, 2007; Morash et al., 2014). Evidence-based practices in community corrections highlight the importance of practitioner-client relationships that are characterized by warmth, empathy, respect, and support (Dowden and Andrews, 2004; Ireland and Berg, 2007; Andrews, 2011). In these relationships, parole officers connect and collaborate with offenders, model pro-social behavior, communicate effectively, and apply motivational techniques (Walters et al., 2007). Positive parole officer-parolee relationships foster bonding, closeness, and trust between both parole officers and parolees (Ireland and Berg, 2007; Ross et al., 2008). These factors provide the necessary context in which change can happen. For example, interviews with parolees consistently demonstrate that positive relationships with their parole officers facilitate pro-social cognitive changes (e.g., identity shifts; Giordano et al., 2007; Bui and Morash, 2010; Morash et al., 2014; Stone et al., 2016) that can lead to stable, long-term success.

In contrast, negative parole officer-parolee relationships are characterized as authoritarian, unsupportive, inflexible, and controlling (Stone et al., 2016). Negative parole officer-parolee relationships have been found to be related to confrontation and noncompliance with the terms of supervision (Ireland and Berg, 2007; Morash et al., 2014; Chamberlain et al., 2017). Importantly, negative parole officer-parolee relationships are not conducive to pro-social cognitive changes (Morash et al., 2014; Stone et al., 2016). Taken together, this research suggests that the quality of the parole officer-parolee relationship plays a role in parolees’ cognitions and self-perception with implications for parolees’ overall success.

The extant research also begs the question, if experiences in the parole officer-parolee relationship has an impact on parolees’ cognitions and self-perception, can these experiences also impact the parole officers’ cognitions; especially their self-perception? Specifically, how do negative or positive parole officer-parolee experiences impact the way parole officers think about themselves in relation to those whom they supervise? The present study is the first to our knowledge to address this question by targeting implicit cognitions about the self in relation to parolees.

Self-Expansion Theory and Implicit Social Cognition

Self-expansion theory posits that frequent and positive experiences with close others (e.g., spouses, close friends) can lead individuals to assume attributes, cognitions, and behaviors of those close others (Aron and Aron, 1986; Aron et al., 1991). Put differently, when the conditions of self-expansion are met, that is, frequent and positive experiences with a close other, an individual is more likely to associate their self-concept (i.e., identity and self-perception) with aspects of another person’s identity.

Self-expansion is based on individuals’ desire to enhance personal growth, progress, and self-efficacy (Aron and Aron, 1996). In close relationships, both individuals mutually include some or all aspects of the other into their self-concept. As a result, a mental overlap occurs between the self and the close other which allows both individuals to: (1) vicariously take on the resources (i.e., physical and social capital), (2) cultivate new perspectives, and (3) acquire new characteristics or identities related to the other and incorporate them into their self-concept (Aron and Aron, 1986, 1996, 1997; Aron et al., 1991, 1992). For instance, when a close other is perceived as a part of the self, the allocation of resources is shared (Clark and Mills, 1979), perspective differences are decreased (Brenner, 1973), and characteristics of others are perceived as one’s own (Tesser et al., 1988).

Self-expansion has been applied to understand the effects of intergroup relationships, such as cross-group friendships (e.g., Latino and White friendships; Page-Gould et al., 2010). This line of research suggests that people are motivated not only to expand their self-concept between close others as individuals but also the groups to which a close other belongs. Self-expansion in intergroup relationships requires that an individual has a relationship with a member of a different social group and that they engage in close, frequent, and positive experiences with each other, that then results in the motivation to associate with the group as a whole and its related traits (Smith and Henry, 1996; Coats et al., 2000; Aron and McLaughlin-Volpe, 2001).

Studies have demonstrated the self-expansion phenomenon using “explicit” or direct self-report measures (Aron et al., 1991, 1992; Page-Gould et al., 2010) and “implicit” or indirect (i.e., reaction time) measures (Aron et al., 1991; Aron and McLaughlin-Volpe, 2001; Page-Gould et al., 2010). For example, married couples exhibit explicit associations between the self and their spouse using the Inclusion of the Other in the Self (IOS) scale, a self-report measure of self-expansion on which participants indicate how close they perceive another person by selecting one of seven pairs of circles that vary in their between-circle distances to represent different degrees of cognitive overlap between the self and the other (Aron et al., 1992, 1995). Married individuals also demonstrate self-expansion on “implicit” measures (Aron et al., 1991; Aron and McLaughlin-Volpe, 2001). On “me/not-me” tasks in which reaction time is used to measure similarity, married participants are quicker (i.e., press a button labeled “me” versus one labeled “not me”) to categorize traits related to their spouses as self-descriptive than traits unrelated to their spouses. Similarly, on a “yes/no” task, participants in a romantic relationship respond faster (i.e., press a button labeled “yes” versus one labeled “no”) to traits relevant to both the participants and the partner than to traits that were different between themselves and their partner (Smith et al., 1999). Taken together, this past research provides evidence for explicit and implicit self-expansion.

Professional Self-Expansion

Most of the self-expansion research has focused on self-expansion in personal relationships. To our knowledge, one study has examined self-expansion within the context of people’s occupations (McIntyre et al., 2014). McIntyre et al. (2014) employed a 14-item self-expansion questionnaire to measure the extent to which people exhibited self-expansion with their occupation as a whole. The study demonstrated that people can indeed self-expand with their occupation. Although McIntyre et al. (2014) focused on self-expansion with people’s occupations as a whole, they suggest that people can self-expand with others with whom they interact in the workplace if the interactions meet the pre-requisites of self-expansion. We extend their work and examine the extent to which parole officers self-expand with the group parolee, a group with which parole officers frequently interact in the workplace by measuring implicit criminal-self associations; one potential consequence of self-expansion with parolees.

Implicit Self-Expansion in Parole Officers

The present study adopts an implicit social cognition approach utilizing an Implicit Association Test (IAT), to measure associations between the self-concept of parole officers with the social identity group criminal. Like other social identity groups, “criminals,” including parolees, self-categorize (Krueger, 2001) as criminal, and, as such, perceive themselves as sharing attributes, cognitions and experiences with the group (Boduszek et al., 2013). Further, past criminal experiences are sufficient for individuals to identify the self both explicitly (Asencio, 2011; Boduszek et al., 2013) and implicitly (Rivera and Veysey, 2015, 2018; Veysey and Rivera, 2017) with the group criminal. Indeed, repeated studies have established the IAT as a reliable and valid measure of implicit self-criminal associations (Rivera and Veysey, 2015, 2018; Veysey and Rivera, 2017). The present research, measuring the extent to which parole officers implicitly associate themselves with their parolees, extends past work on direct personal experience as a criminal to indirect personal experience with a member of the group criminal (i.e., parolee).

Our a priori hypothesis is that parole officers who have positive (versus negative) experiences with parolees are likely to exhibit relatively strong implicit associations with the group criminal. The logic underlying this hypothesis follows previous examinations of self-expansion. Parole officers may experience self-expansion through frequent and positive experiences with parolees. Indeed, the occupational role of parole officers requires them to frequently and directly meet with multiple parolees on a daily basis (at roughly 76 h per month; DeMichele, 2007). As such, these meetings provide opportunities for positive interactions that may foster a sense of closeness between parole officers and their parolees. Moreover, parole officers may view the success or shortcomings of their parolees as their own, reflecting a sense of interconnectedness. Finally, parole officers have the potential to engage in positive experiences with their parolees as they support parolees’ reintegration into society. For parole officers, therefore, it is not an experience within the criminal justice system that contributes to self-expansion and its effects on their mental associations with the group criminal, but rather their occupational experiences with others only who have had criminal justice experiences.

Parole Officer’s Subjective Professional Identity and Orientation

The implicit cognitive consequences of the parole officer-parolee relationship to some degree may be dependent on parole officers’ professional characteristics, particularly the importance of, and their basic orientation toward, their role (Seiter and West, 2003; Walters et al., 2007). Consistent with role identity theory, roles that individuals take on, such as occupation, theoretically should affect officers’ self-concept and cognitions (Stryker and Burke, 2000), especially on the job where the role is most likely to be highly salient. Two aspects of parole officers’ role are professional orientation and subjective professional identification.

Parole officer orientation has been measured along a continuum ranging from surveillance and strict law enforcement to therapeutic support (Sigler and Mcgraw, 1984; Seiter and West, 2003). Research suggests that professional orientation affects the quality of experiences within the parole officer-parolee relationship (Skeem et al., 2003, 2007; Kennealy et al., 2012; Blasko et al., 2015). Surveillance restricts and controls parolee behavior to ensure that individuals fulfill the responsibilities and conditions of parole (Fulton et al., 1997; Seiter, 2002; Skeem et al., 2003). Also, surveillance is related to low levels of trust and cooperation, which may be related to negative experiences within the parole officer-parolee relationship (Fulton et al., 1997; Seiter, 2002; Skeem et al., 2003). Conversely, parole officers who take on a therapeutic role may be more likely to have positive experiences with their parolees as this orientation requires the parole officer to deeply engage with parolees, and to aid them in addressing criminogenic obstacles, such as mental health problems, substance abuse, physical health conditions, inadequate educational and employment skills, and lack of stable housing (Seiter, 2002; Petersilia, 2003; Travis, 2005; National Research Council, 2008; Tatman and Love, 2010; Blasko et al., 2015). Therefore, professional orientation may moderate the effect of parole officers’ relationship experiences with parolees on their implicit associations with parolees. Specifically, among parole officers who take on a therapeutic orientation, a positive experience with a parolee may yield stronger implicit associations with parolees in comparison to those who do not take on a therapeutic orientation.

In addition, parole officers vary in the extent to which they identify with their occupational group. According to social identity theory, people often identify with the social groups to which they belong, including occupational groups (Hogg and Turner, 1987). The extent to which people identify with their occupational group may reflect commitment (Ellemers and Rink, 2005) and is related to positive workplace behavior such as job performance (Meyer et al., 2002; Becker and Kernan, 2003). By extension, the extent to which people identify as a parole officer may influence the types of experiences parole officers have with parolees. For example, those who strongly identify as a parole officer may be more committed to assisting parolees in successful reentry, in turn, impacting their experiences with parolees. Therefore, the extent to which they identify with their occupational group may moderate the effect of parole officers’ relationship experiences with parolees on their implicit associations with parolees. Specifically, among parole officers who strongly identify with their occupational group, a positive experience with a parolee may yield stronger implicit associations with parolees in comparison to those who do not strongly identify with their occupational group.

The Present Study

This experiment is the first of its kind to examine implicit associations between the self-perception of criminal justice practitioners, specifically parole officers, and individuals in the criminal justice system. We experimentally manipulated parole officers’ memory of either a positive or negative experience with a parolee, then utilized an IAT to measure implicit self-criminal associations. We tested the a priori main hypothesis that officers who are reminded of a positive experience with a parolee will exhibit stronger implicit associations with the group criminal in comparison to parole officers who are reminded of a negative experience. Finally, we explored the moderating effect of parole officer orientation and parole officer group identification on the relation between the manipulation (i.e., positive or negative memory task) and implicit self-criminal association.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Design

The population of interest were the 258 parole officers in the New Jersey State Parole Board Division of Parole. At the time of the experiment, the parole officers averaged caseloads of approximately 50 parolees per officer. Officers were expected to conduct three face-to-face interactions per month, including one home visit, per individual. However, it is important to note that these were the minimum standards per parolee, and officers had the discretion to meet with parolees as frequently as deemed fit given the circumstances. Officers had the ability to interact with parolees in a variety of contexts ranging from the parole office to counseling locations to transporting parolees to important appointments.

We invited all New Jersey State parole officers to participate; first via an on-line platform and then through face-to-face invitations to officers on duty at district offices. Eighty-seven (n = 18 online; n = 69 district office) active parole officers completed the experiment. All data were collected anonymously, and participants volunteered to complete the study without any incentive. Three participants’ data were excluded from analyses due to reaction time error rates on the IAT that were greater than 30% overall or over 40% on any given block as recommended by Greenwald et al. (2003).

The final sample consisted of 84 parole officers (28 females, 55 males, 1 other,1 Mage = 37.10, SDage = 7.39, age range: 25–54 years). Table 1 lists the demographics and characteristics of the final sample. Approximately 49% percent of officers identified as White, 26% were Hispanic, 13% were Black, 11% were another ethnicity not listed, and 1% identified as Asian or Pacific Islander. On average, parole officers had been working in their position for nearly nine years (Myearsparole = 8.64, SDyearsparole = 6.634, range: 1–25 years). Approximately 51% of officers were from the sex offender management unit, 35% of parole officers were from a traditional unit, and 14% were from other units. Approximately 18% of parole officers had a criminal history (i.e., arrest, conviction, and/or incarceration). The experiment employed a one-factor two-level (parole officer-parolee experience condition: positive versus negative) between-participants design.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and characteristics of sample participants (N = 84).

Manipulated Variable

Positive Versus Negative Memory Writing Task

Rooted in the basic principles of self-expansion theory, the purpose of the manipulation was to make salient an experience parole officers have had with a parolee. Parole officers were randomly assigned to vividly re-experience either a positive or a negative experience with a parolee. Participants in the positive experience condition were given the following prompt: “Please imagine a positive interaction you have had with a parolee. Please describe the positive experience as well as your thoughts and feelings during this positive interaction. Please provide details and write freely.” Participants in the negative experience condition were given the same prompt, but were asked to focus on a negative interaction. Fifty-two percent of parole officers were randomly assigned to the positive experience condition and 48% to the negative experience condition.

A review of the writing responses confirmed that participants responded to the prompt with positive responses (i.e., used positive words) in the positive experience condition and negative responses (i.e., used negative words) in the negative experience condition, with two exceptions. Two participants (2%) provided a mixed response, one in each condition. Also, we conducted an ANOVA to test if the length of responses (i.e., the number of words) between conditions were different. There was no difference, on average, between the positive (M = 66.65, SD = 64.11) and negative (M = 65.50, SD = 50.44) conditions, F(1, 82) = 0.008, p = 0.928.

Measured Variables

Implicit Criminal-Self Associations

The current study used a Single-Category Implicit Association Test (SC-IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998; Karpinski and Steinman, 2006), which uses reaction times to operationalize the strength of implicit mental associations between the self and criminal (Rivera and Veysey, 2015, 2018; Veysey and Rivera, 2017). Parolees are members of the group criminal. Prior to becoming a parolee, a person must first be convicted of a crime and incarcerated, and, therefore, is formally labeled a criminal. Also, during a first introduction, parole officers are made aware of a parolee’s criminal history, thus affirming the parolee’s identity as a criminal.

The SC-IAT was administered on a computer and participants were asked to complete four blocks of reaction time trials that were preceded by a set of instructions. Semantic stimuli that represent self, other, and criminal randomly appeared one after the other in the center of the screen. The self-related words were me, my, mine, I, and myself. The other-related words were they, them, their, theirs, and other (“self” and “other” words were used in prior studies, e.g., Rivera and Veysey, 2015; Veysey and Rivera, 2017). The criminal-related words were criminal, felon, lawbreaker, offender, convict, delinquent, and prisoner. The criminal words were pre-tested with a separate adult sample (for a full description, see Rivera and Veysey, 2018).

As each word appeared on the screen, category labels were positioned on the top left and top right of the screen. For half of the task, participants used the “A” key to classify the words that belong to either the “self” or “criminal” category (labels on the top left) and the “K” key to classify the words that belong to the “other” category (label on the top right). The second half of the task was reversed; participants used the “A” key to classify the words that belong to the “self” category (label on the top left) and the “K” key to classify the words that belong to the “other” or “criminal” category (labels on the top right). These tasks were counterbalanced between participants. For each task, participants first read the instructions then completed 17 practice trials, followed by 51 critical trials. For each trial, the target word remained on the screen until participants classified it to one of the three categories on the monitor (“self,” “other,” or “criminal”). If the participant responded correctly, a new target word appeared. If the participant responded incorrectly, the message “ERROR” appeared on the screen in place of the target word and remained until the participant pressed the correct key.

The SC-IAT was scored in accordance with the procedures outlined by Greenwald et al. (2003) and Karpinski and Steinman (2006). The score is the difference between the reaction times between the self and criminal trials and the other and criminal trials. Relatively higher SC-IAT scores indicate faster reaction times when self-related words and criminal-related words are paired together than when other-related words are paired with criminal words. Thus, a higher IAT score indicates stronger associations between self and criminal.

Subjective Identification With Parole Officers

Participants indicated the extent to which they identified with their professional group. Parole officers were asked to think about their identification with other parole officers and respond to two items (“Being a parole officer is an important part of who I am” and “Being a parole officer is important to my sense of self”) on a 7-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree (6). The two items were highly correlated (r = 0.75, p < 0.001) and therefore combined into a single measure.

Professional Orientation

Guided by the Parole Officer Professional Orientation measure (Fulton et al., 1997), which was designed to assess how parole officers perform their job functions and goals, participants indicated the extent to which they adopted a professional orientation (i.e., a surveillance vs. therapeutic orientation). Parole officers were asked to rate how they perform their job and respond to two items: (a) one regarding their subjective job role—“The most important aspect of your job is …” on a 5-point scale ranging from exclusively social work (1) to exclusively law enforcement (5); and (b) a second regarding their occupational strategy: “The most effective way to change behavior is through…” on a 5-point scale ranging from exclusively positive reinforcement (1) to exclusively punitive sanctions (5). Higher scores on both questions indicate a stronger focus on strategies related to law enforcement. While both questions sought to measure the construct of professional orientation, their low correlation, r(84) = 0.39, p < 0.001, suggests that they are tapping into different elements of professional orientation. Therefore, the items were not combined and were explored independently.

Demographics

Participants completed a demographics and background questionnaire that included variables such as gender, age, race/ethnicity, length of time as a parole officer, type of caseload, and personal criminal history.

Procedure

Participants were informed that the study’s purpose was to examine the relation between parole officers’ professional experiences and attitudes. First, participants were randomly assigned to and completed the positive or negative experience writing task and then completed all remaining measures in the order listed above. Finally, participants were fully debriefed about the purpose of the study.

Results

See Table 2 (entire sample) and Table 3 (by experience condition) for descriptives and zero-order correlations.2

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Zero-order correlations and descriptives for all participants (N = 84).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Zero-order correlations and descriptives by experience condition.

To test our main hypothesis of the effect of making parole officer experiences salient on implicit associations, SC-IAT scores (criminal-self association strength) were subjected to a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). As predicted, participants who were reminded of a positive experience with their parolees (M = −0.05, SD = 0.14) exhibited stronger associations between criminal and self when compared to those who were reminded of a negative experience with their parolees (M = −0.13, SD = 0.15), F(1, 82) = 5.20, p = 0.025, d = 0.50 (medium effect size).3 These findings support our hypothesis that parole officers who are reminded of positive experiences exhibit a stronger implicit association (i.e., a reduced cognitive distance) between criminal and self in comparison to parole officers who are reminded of negative experiences.

We next explored if individual differences in subjective group identification or the two professional orientations (i.e., subjective job role and occupational strategy) moderated the effect of past experiences on criminal-self associations. To this end, we conducted three hierarchical multiple regression analyses in which scores on the three individual differences measures (mean-centered) and parole officer-parolee experience condition (coded 0 = negative experience, 1 = positive experience) were entered in the first step and their interaction term in the second step. Consistent with our main hypothesis results above, the main effect of the Experience Condition was significant across the three models, ΔFs(2, 81) > 2.81, ps < 0.036, R2s > 0.07, βs > 0.23, ps < 0.025. However, there was no significant main effect of Subjective Parole Officer Identification, ΔF(2, 81) = 3.29, p = 0.247, R2 = 0.08, β = −0.13, p = 0.247, and no significant Subjective Parole Officer Identification X Experience Condition interaction, ΔF(3, 80) = 0.66, p = 0.417, R2 = 0.08, β = 0.12, p = 0.417; no significant main effect of Subjective Job Role, ΔF(2, 81) = 2.82, p = 0.493, R2 = 0.07, β = −0.08, p = 0.693, and no significant Subjective Job Role X Experience Condition interaction, ΔF(3, 80) = 0.01, p = 0.929, R2 = 0.07, β = −0.01, p = 0.929; and, finally, no significant main effect of Occupational Strategy, ΔF(2, 81) = 3.72, p = 0.146, R2 = 0.08, β = −0.16, p = 0.063, and no significant Occupational Strategy X Experience Condition interaction, ΔF(3, 80) = 1.40, p = 0.241, R2 = 0.10, β = 0.17, p = 0.241.

General Discussion

This research extends earlier findings regarding implicit self-expansion in general, contributes to the sparse literature on employment-based self-expansion, and provides evidence of a self-expansion mechanism to a highly stigmatized group. The study examined the extent to which parole officers implicitly associate self with the group criminal as a function of a memory-based manipulation. The primary finding that officers who are reminded of a positive experience with a parolee demonstrate stronger implicit self-criminal association on the IAT than those who were reminded of a negative experience with parolees supports other group-based implicit self-expansion studies. These former studies, however, target one or more cross-group friends and self-expansion to their groups (e.g., having one or more Latinx friends and the group Latinxs; Page-Gould et al., 2010). The present study, like McIntyre et al.’ (2014), is based within a professional work environment. While McIntyre et al. (2014) used “job” as a proxy group identity, the present research tested self-expansion to the client group of parolees after manipulating a memory in which officers imagined an interaction with an individual parolee on their caseload.

For self-expansion at the group level to occur, interactions with one or more members of a group must be: (1) close, (2) frequent, and (3) positive. We suggest that these self-expansion requirements occur in parole officers’ relationships with a parolee in the workplace. Closeness is promoted through their relationship interactions with individual parolees who represent the group and its characteristics. The role of the parole officer requires that they interact with parolees for numerous hours a week. During this time parole officers engage with parolees and support them to lead pro-social lives (e.g., assistance with treatment, job placement, and housing). High frequency is assumed due to the time spent and average number of parole officer-parolee interactions on any given day. And, finally, positive (vs. negative) interactions were experimentally manipulated in the present research. Under these conditions, the results demonstrated that a single recalled positive versus negative memory had a significant effect on implicit associations between parole officers’ self and the group criminals.

While our main hypothesis was supported, the nature of the effect needs further understanding. Self-expansion is about taking on aspects of the close other or of the close other’s group. In our research, we infer evidence for self-expansion from parole officers’ stronger implicit self-criminal associations on the IAT following the reminder of a past positive relative to a negative experience with a parolee. The mean IAT score in the positive experience condition, however, was near the midpoint of the scale, whereas the mean IAT score in the negative experience condition significantly more. Since the IAT in general is a measure of relative associations, presently self-criminal associations relative to other-criminal associations, its scoring is a function of the difference between reaction times to categories simultaneously paired on the computer screen. Thus, another yet complementary suggestion from our data is that parole officers in the negative experience condition increased their cognitive distance between their self-concept and the group parolee, whereas those in positive experience condition demonstrated a decrease in this cognitive distance.

The potential positive impact of these findings is related to the malleability of this effect. If a single positive reminder affects implicit associations between the officer and the parolee, the quality of the parole officer-parolee relationship may be similarly affected. In general, self-expansion results in the acquisition of perspectives, causing the self to be concerned with the needs of the other (Wegner, 1980; Deutsch and Mackesy, 1985; Aron and Aron, 1986). Thus, even in relationships with people belonging to stigmatized groups, self-expansion results in the reduction of cognitive distance. Within this definition, self-expansion may be a cognitive driver of empathy, which requires one to take the perspective of the other (Selman, 1980; Wegner, 1980; Aron et al., 2004; Batson, 2009).

Empathy is an essential component of the therapeutic relationship, and it can facilitate collaboration, trust, and understanding between the practitioner and client (Murphy and Baxter, 1997) and is related to positive client outcomes (Taxman, 2002; Ross et al., 2008; Taxman and Ainsworth, 2009; McCambridge et al., 2011). Empathy is also related to bonding, feelings of closeness, attitudes towards stigmatized groups (Finlay and Stephan, 2000), and changes in behavior, such as helping the other (Batson et al., 2002). Therefore, it is plausible that empathy follows from self-expansion and, as such, positive reminders and messaging may be used to improve the quality of the interaction and, thus, improve parolee success (Gunnison and Helfgott, 2011).

Although the present study does not directly measure empathy, our data suggest that one way to potentially bolster empathy between parole officers and parolees is to provide environments that foster positive interactions. Practices such as Motivational Interviewing have been increasingly adopted by corrections agencies due to their positive effects on parolees (Dowden and Andrews, 2004; Landenberger and Lipsey, 2005), and have an effect on parole officers as well (Iarussi and Powers, 2018). Because Motivational Interviewing promotes rapport and requires the parole officer to take the perspective of the parolee, it is therefore possible that Motivational Interviewing also provides parole officers with opportunities to acquire the perspective of their parolees and promote helping behaviors that support parolees’ successful reentry and long-term desistance.

In the absence of these experiences, reminding parole officers of positive or successful experiences with parolees may serve to strengthen implicit self-criminal associations, which can have occupational behavioral effects. Positive experiences can be promoted through the use of bulletin boards that showcase events in which parole officers and parolees work together or celebrate the accomplishments of parolees (e.g., educational or employment successes). This practice may serve as a reminder that successful and positive experiences between parole officers and parolees do happen and can result in positive outcomes. Future research should examine the relation between implicit self-criminal associations and empathy and its downstream consequences for parole officer behaviors and other outcomes.

Additionally, we explored the moderating role of parole officers’ subjective professional identity (i.e., professional orientation and parole officer identification) on the relation between past experiences and implicit self-criminal associations. None of the measures of subjective professional identity were related to implicit associations, nor did they moderate the effect of past experiences on implicit associations. This suggests that making a positive memory salient is powerful enough to override (i.e., regardless of) individual-level professional characteristics.

While self-expansion may impact parole officer performance and parolee outcomes, it is important to note that the present study cannot answer the fundamental question about whether implicit cognitions in this setting affect parole officer behavior. To be clear, this study is not longitudinal nor does it include measures of actual parole officer behavior or parolee success. However, research suggests that self-expansion can influence not only cognition but also behavior (Aron et al., 1991; Aron and Aron, 1996; Davis et al., 1996; Cialdini et al., 1997). Moreover, implicit social cognitions predict behavioral actions, often having greater explanatory power in behavioral outcomes than explicit social cognitions (Greenwald et al., 2009; Charlesworth and Banaji, in press). Further, research demonstrates that occupations which promote self-expansion are related to increased job satisfaction and commitment (McIntyre et al., 2014). Based on prior work, we speculate that self-expansion may have implications for the well-being of parole officers as well.

This study utilizes data from parole officers, a unique and often inaccessible sample. However, obtaining data from this sample is not without its limitations. New Jersey employs nearly 600 parole officers across 16 parole offices throughout the state (New Jersey State Parole Board, 2020). First, the sample size was relatively small due to the time consuming and costly nature of in-person data collection from on-duty officers during department-wide meetings. The researcher responsible for data collection visited several parole offices numerous times over the course of two years. The intention was to achieve a high response rate. However, some officers were not willing to participate in this research for various reasons ranging from lack of interest to job demands. Ideally, future research with a larger sample could expand on the current research. Second, because the study was limited to ten minutes per participant, the amount of data that could be obtained was limited. For example, measures of explicit association were not collected. While implicit social cognition research demonstrates that implicit and explicit associations with stigmatized groups are often poorly correlated or uncorrelated (Greenwald et al., 2009), self-expansion research demonstrates that implicit and explicit associations are often correlated (Coats et al., 2000; Page-Gould et al., 2010). Future research should examine the effect of experiences on both implicit and explicit associations to provide a comprehensive understanding of self-expansion with this particular group.

Finally, while this study examines the effect of reminders of past experiences with a single parolee, this study does not answer the question of repeat experiences. Do parole officers who have a positive experience with a parolee tend to repeatedly engage in positive experiences with their parolees? If so, parole officers who frequently engage in positive experiences may exhibit self-expansion even in the absence of contexts which facilitate positive experiences, and, therefore, translate to continuous positive experiences with parolees. This is squarely in line with research that demonstrates that individuals who had high quality relationships with cross-group friends did not exhibit stress or anxiety following conflict (Page-Gould, 2012). For parole officers, negative experiences with parolees may occur, but parole officers who self-expand may be better equipped to overcome such experiences, thereby potentially improving their relationships with parolees. Despite these limitations, the present data suggest a promising step in understanding the conditions which strengthen implicit associations with parolees.

Conclusion

The present study is the first experiment at the intersection of psychology and criminology to apply the self-expansion model and implicit social cognition measurements within a criminal justice setting to understand the cognitive effects of a parole officer’s relationship with parolees. Results demonstrate that parole officers who are reminded of positive experiences with a parolee exhibit stronger associations between themselves and the group parolee in comparison to those reminded of a negative experience. Positive experiences can increase parole officers’ positive perceptions of parolees, as well as bolster their overall working relationship with parolees. This may lead to benefits such as increased job satisfaction and may have downstream consequences for parolees; namely, desistance and successful reentry – a primary goal of parole and of the criminal justice system.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Rutgers University IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was partially supported by the Rutgers School of Criminal Justice and the Rutgers Implicit Social Cognition (RISC) Lab.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the members of RISC Lab for their thoughtful comments on this manuscript, and Ethan Motschmann for his support with computer programming.

Footnotes

1. ^One participant did not indicate their gender.

2. ^Zero-order correlations between the demographic variables (age, length of time as a parole officer), subjective parole officer identification, the professional orientation measures (subjective role, occupational strategy), and implicit criminal-self association scores were conducted for the entire sample and by each condition. The relation between the categorical demographic variables (gender, race/ethnicity, type of caseload, criminal history, district office location) and implicit criminal-self association scores across the entire sample and by condition were analyzed using a series of ANOVAs. For gender, participants who identified as “other” were nominal (n = 1), so gender was coded was coded (1 = male; 0 = female, other) to facilitate the interpretation of the results; race/ethnicity was coded (1 = African American/Black and Hispanic; 0 = all others), type of caseload was coded (1 = sex offender unit; 0 = all other units), criminal history was coded (1 = arrested, convicted, and/or incarcerated; 0 = was not arrested, convicted, and/or incarcerated), and district office location was coded (1 = online participant/unknown district; 2 = Northern New Jersey, 3 = Central/Southern New Jersey). The ANOVAs and zero-order correlation analyses revealed that no demographic and measured variables were statistically significantly related to implicit criminal-self association scores.

3. ^We conducted the same analyses using the subsample of participants who did not have a criminal history (n = 69). Results were similar to those with the full sample (positive experience: M = −0.05, SD = 0.15, negative experience: M = −0.12, SD = 0.16; F(1, 67) = 4.11, p = 0.047, d = 0.45, medium effect size).

References

Abadinsky, H. (2009). Probation and Parole: Theory and Practice. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Andrews, D. A. (2011). “The principles of effective correctional programs,” in Correctional Contexts: Contemporary and Classical Readings. eds. E. J. Latessa and A. M. Holsinger (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 228–237.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., and Aron, E. N. (1986). Love and the Expansion of Self: Understanding Attraction and Satisfaction. Washington: Hemisphere Publishing Corp/Harper & Row Publishers.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., and Aron, E. N. (1996). “Self and self expansion in relationships,” in Knowledge Structures in Close Relationships: A Social Psychological Approach. eds. G. J. O. Fletcher and J. Fitness (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 325–344.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., and Aron, E. N. (1997). “Self-expansion motivation and including other in the self.” in Handbook of Personal Relationships. 2nd Edn., Vol. 1. W. Ickes (Section Ed.) and S. Duck (Ed.), (Chichester, New York: Wiley), 251–270.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Norman, C. (2004). “Self-expansion model of motivation and cognition in close relationships and beyond,” in Self and Social Identity. eds. M. B. Brewer and M. Hewstone (Blackwell Publishing), 99–123.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., and Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 241–253. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2001). “Including others in the self: extensions to own and partner’s group memberships,” in Individual Self, Relational Self, and Collective Self: Partners, Opponents, or Strangers? eds. C. Sedikides and M. B. Brewer (Philadelphia, Hove England: Psychology Press), 89–109.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., Paris, M., and Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: prospective studies of self-concept change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 1102–1112. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asencio, E. K. (2011). Familiarity, legitimation, and frequency: The influence of others on the criminal self-view. Sociol. Inq. 81, 34–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-682X.2010.00352.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2009). “These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena,” in The Social Neuro-Science of Empathy. eds. J. Decety and W. Ickes (Cambridge: MIT Press), 3–15.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., and Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy and attitudes: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 28, 1656–1666. doi: 10.1177/014616702237647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, T. E., and Kernan, M. C. (2003). Matching commitment to supervisors and organizations to in-role and extra-role performance. Hum. Perform. 16, 327–348. doi: 10.1207/S15327043HUP1604_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasko, B. L., Friedmann, P. D., Rhodes, A. G., and Taxman, F. S. (2015). The parolee–parole officer relationship as a mediator of criminal justice outcomes. Crim. Justice Behav. 42, 722–740. doi: 10.1177/0093854814562642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boduszek, D., Adamson, G., Shevlin, M., Mallett, J., and Hyland, P. (2013). Criminal social identity of recidivistic prisoners: the role of self-esteem, family and criminal friends. J. Police Crim. Psychol. 28, 15–25. doi: 10.1007/s11896-012-9105-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenner, M. (1973). The next-in-line effect. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 12, 320–323. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80076-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bui, H. N., and Morash, M. (2010). The impact of network relationships, prison experiences, and internal transformation on women's success after prison release. J. Offender Rehabil. 49, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/10509670903435381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, A. W., Gricius, M., Wallace, D. M., Borjas, D., and Ware, V. M. (2017). Parolee–parole officer rapport: does it impact recidivism? Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 62, 3581–3602. doi: 10.1177/0306624X17741593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlesworth, T. E. S., and Banaji, M. R. (in press). “The relationship of implicit social cognition and discriminatory behavior,” in Handbook on Economics of Discrimination and Affirmative Action. ed. A. Deshpande (New York: Springer).

Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., and Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: when one into one equals oneness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 481–494. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, M. S., and Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 12–24. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coats, S., Smith, E. R., Claypool, H. M., and Banner, M. J. (2000). Overlapping mental representations of self and in-group: reaction time evidence and its relationship with explicit measures of group identification. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 36, 304–315. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1999.1416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A., and Yang, C. S. (2019). Judicial politics and sentencing decisions. Am. Econ. J. Econ. Pol. 11, 160–191. doi: 10.1257/pol.20170329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., and Luce, C. (1996). Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: a merging of self and other. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 713–726. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeMichele, M. T. (2007). Probation and Parole’s Growing Caseloads and Workload Allocation: Strategies for Managerial Decision Making. Lombardi, IL: The American Probation & Parole Association.

Google Scholar

Deutsch, F. M., and Mackesy, M. E. (1985). Friendship and the development of self-schemas: The effects of talking about others. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 11, 399–408. doi: 10.1177/0146167285114006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowden, C., and Andrews, D. A. (2004). The importance of staff practice in delivering effective correctional treatment: a meta-analytic review of core correctional practice. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 48, 203–214. doi: 10.1177/0306624X03257765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemers, N., and Rink, F. (2005). Identity in work groups: the beneficial and detrimental consequences of multiple identities and group norms for collaboration and group performance. Soc. Identif. Groups 22, 1–41. doi: 10.1016/S0882-6145(05)22001-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fielding-Miller, R., Cooper, H. L. F., Caslin, S., and Raj, A. (2020). The interaction of race and gender as a significant driver of racial arrest disparities for African American men. J. Urban Health 97, 112–122. doi: 10.1007/s11524-018-00325-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finlay, K. A., and Stephan, W. G. (2000). Reducing prejudice: the effects of empathy on intergroup attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 1720–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02464.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fulton, B., Stichman, A., Travis, L., and Latessa, E. (1997). Moderating probation and parole officer attitudes to achieve desired outcomes. Prison J. 77, 295–312. doi: 10.1177/0032855597077003005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawronski, B., and Payne, B. K. (Eds.). (2011). Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Gibbons, S. G., and Rosecrance, J. D. (2005). Probation, Parole, and Community Corrections in the United States. Boston: Pearson Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Giordano, P. C., Schroeder, R. D., and Cernkovich, S. A. (2007). Emotions and crime over the life course: a neo-Meadian perspective on criminal continuity and change. Am. J. Sociol. 112, 1603–1661. doi: 10.1086/512710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., and Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnison, E., and Helfgott, J. B. (2011). Factors that hinder offender reentry success: a view from community corrections officers. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 55, 287–304. doi: 10.1177/0306624X09360661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogg, M. A., and Turner, J. C. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self-stereotyping and the salience of social categories. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 26, 325–340. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1987.tb00795.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, B. M., and Bynum, T. S. (2008). The role of race and ethnicity in parole decisions. Criminology 46, 907–938. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2008.00130.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iarussi, M. M., and Powers, D. F. (2018). Outcomes of motivational interviewing training with probation and parole officers: findings and lessons learned. Fed. Probat. 82, 28–35.

Google Scholar

Ireland, C., and Berg, B. (2007). Women in parole: gendered adaptations of female parole agents in California. Women Crim. Just. 18, 131–150. doi: 10.1300/J012v18n01_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2007). A rapid evidence assessment of the impact of mentoring on re-offending: A summary. Available at: https://www.ojp.gov/ncjrs/virtual-library/abstracts/rapid-evidence-assessment-impact-mentoring-re-offending-summary (Accessed June 3, 2022).

Google Scholar

Karpinski, A., and Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test (SC-IAT) as a measure of implicit social cognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 16–32. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennealy, P. J., Skeem, J. L., Manchak, S. M., and Louden, J. E. (2012). Firm, fair, and caring officer-offender relationships protect against supervision failure. Law Hum. Behav. 36, 496–505. doi: 10.1037/h0093935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. (2001). “Social categorization, the psychology of,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. eds. N. J. Smelser and P. B. Baltes (Amsterdam: Elsevier Ltd), 14219–14223.

Google Scholar

Landenberger, N. A., and Lipsey, M. W. (2005). The positive effects of cognitive–behavioral programs for offenders: a meta-analysis of factors associated with effective treatment. J. Exp. Criminol. 1, 451–476. doi: 10.1007/s11292-005-3541-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCambridge, J., Day, M., Thomas, B. A., and Strang, J. (2011). Fidelity to motivational interviewing and subsequent cannabis cessation among adolescents. Addict. Behav. 36, 749–754. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntyre, K. P., Mattingly, B. A., Lewandowski, G. W. Jr., and Simpson, A. (2014). Workplace self-expansion: implications for job satisfaction, commitment, self-concept clarity, and self-esteem among the employed and unemployed. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 59–69. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2013.856788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, J. P., Stanley, D. J., Herscovitch, L., and Topolnytsky, L. (2002). Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: a meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. J. Vocat. Behav. 61, 20–52. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.2001.1842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morash, M., Kashy, D. A., Smith, S. W., and Cobbina, J. E. (2014). The effects of probation or parole agent relationship style and women offenders’ criminogenic needs on offenders’ responses to supervision interactions. Crim. Justice Behav. 42, 412–434. doi: 10.1177/0093854814551602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, C. M., and Baxter, V. A. (1997). Motivating batterers to change in the treatment context. J. Interpers. Violence 12, 607–619. doi: 10.1177/088626097012004009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council (2008). Parole, desistance from crime, and community integration. Available at: https://www.nap.edu/catalog/11988/parole-desistance-from-crime-and-community-integration (Accessed June 3, 2022).

Google Scholar

New Jersey State Parole Board (2020). Annual Report. Available at: https://www.state.nj.us/parole/docs/reports/AnnualReport2019.pdf (Accessed June 3, 2022).

Google Scholar

Oudekerk, B., and Kaeble, D. (2021). Probation and parole in the United States, 2019. Bulletin (NCJ 256092). Bureau of Justice Statistics.

Google Scholar

Page-Gould, E. (2012). To whom can I turn? Maintenance of positive intergroup relations in the face of intergroup conflict. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 3, 462–470. doi: 10.1177/1948550611426937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page-Gould, E., Mendoza-Denton, R., Alegre, J. M., and Siy, J. O. (2010). Understanding the impact of cross-group friendship on interactions with novel outgroup members. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 775–793. doi: 10.1037/a0017880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersilia, J. (2003). When Prisoners Come Home: Parole and Prisoner Reentry. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Rivera, L. M., and Veysey, B. M. (2015). Criminal justice system involvement and gender stereotypes: consequences and implications for women’s implicit and explicit criminal identities. Albany Law Rev. 78, 1109–1128.

Google Scholar

Rivera, L. M., and Veysey, B. M. (2018). Implicit self-criminal cognition and its relation to criminal behavior. Law Hum. Behav. 42, 507–519. doi: 10.1037/lhb0000300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, E. C., Polaschek, D. L., and Ward, T. (2008). The therapeutic alliance: a theoretical revision for offender rehabilitation. Aggress. Violent Behav. 13, 462–480. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2008.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seiter, R. P. (2002). Prisoner reentry and the role of parole officers. Fed. Probat. 66, 50–54.

Google Scholar

Seiter, R. P., and West, A. D. (2003). Supervision styles in probation and parole: an analysis of activities. J. Offender Rehabil. 38, 57–75. doi: 10.1300/J076v38n02_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selman, R. L. (1980). The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding. New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Sigler, R. T., and Mcgraw, B. (1984). Adult probation and parole officers: influence of their weapons, role perceptions and role conflict. Crim. Justice Rev. 9, 28–32. doi: 10.1177/073401688400900105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skeem, J. L., Encandela, J., and Louden, J. E. (2003). Perspectives on probation and mandated mental health treatment in specialized and traditional probation departments. Behav. Sci. Law 21, 429–458. doi: 10.1002/bsl.547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skeem, J. L., Louden, J. E., Polaschek, D., and Camp, J. (2007). Assessing relationship quality in mandated community treatment: blending care with control. Psychol. Assess. 19, 397–410. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. R., Coats, S., and Walling, D. (1999). Overlapping mental representations of self, in-group, and partner: further response time evidence and a connectionist model. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 873–882. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025007009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. R., and Henry, S. (1996). An in-group becomes part of the self: response time evidence. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 635–642. doi: 10.1177/0146167296226008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, R., Morash, M., Goodson, M., Smith, S., and Cobbina, J. (2016). Women on parole, identity processes, and primary desistance. Fem. Criminol. 13, 382–403. doi: 10.1177/1557085116670004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryker, S., and Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present and future of an identity theory. Soc. Psychol. Q. 63, 284–297. doi: 10.2307/2695840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatman, A. W., and Love, K. M. (2010). An offender version of the working alliance inventory-short revised. J. Offender Rehabil. 49, 165–179. doi: 10.1080/10509671003666560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taxman, F. S. (2002). Supervision-exploring the dimensions of effectiveness. Fed. Probat. 66, 14–27.

Google Scholar

Taxman, F. S., and Ainsworth, S. (2009). Correctional milieu: the key to quality outcomes. Vict. Offenders 4, 334–340. doi: 10.1080/15564880903227347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tesser, A., Millar, M., and Moore, J. (1988). Some affective consequences of social comparison and reflection processes: the pain and pleasure of being close. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 49–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, J. (2005). But They All Come Back: Facing the Challenges of Prisoner Reentry. Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.

Google Scholar

Veysey, B. M., and Rivera, L. M. (2017). Implicit criminal identity and age: implications for criminal persistence and desistance. Crim. Justice Behav. 44, 1249–1261. doi: 10.1177/0093854817722173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walters, S. T., Clark, M. D., Gingerich, R., and Meltzer, M. L. (2007). Motivating Offenders to Change: A Guide for Probation and Parole. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice, National Institute of Corrections.

Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (1980). “The self in prosocial action,” in The Self in social Psychology. eds. D. M. Wegner and R. R. Vallacher (New York: Oxford University Press), 131–157.

Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, parole, self-expansion, implicit social cognition, criminal identity

Citation: Saad MK, Rivera LM and Veysey BM (2022) A Positive Versus Negative Interaction Memory Affects Parole Officers’ Implicit Associations Between the Self-Concept and the Group Parolees. Front. Psychol. 13:787583. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.787583

Received: 30 September 2021; Accepted: 23 May 2022;
Published: 28 July 2022.

Edited by:

Yael Granot, Smith College, United States

Reviewed by:

Kyle Ward, University of Northern Colorado, United States
Lacey Schaefer, Griffith University, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Saad, Rivera and Veysey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marina K. Saad, bS5zYWFkQGZkdS5lZHU=; Luis M. Rivera, bHVpc0Bwc3ljaG9sb2d5LnJ1dGdlcnMuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.