Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 31 January 2022
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Emotion Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders View all 9 articles

Perceived Driving Difficulty, Negative Affect, and Emotion Dysregulation in Self-Identified Autistic Emerging Drivers

\r\nMegan Fok,*Megan Fok1,2*Justin M. OwensJustin M. Owens3Thomas H. Ollendick,Thomas H. Ollendick1,2Angela Scarpa,\r\nAngela Scarpa1,2
  • 1Virginia Tech Autism Clinic & Center for Autism Research, Blacksburg, VA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, United States
  • 3Division of Vehicle, Driver & Safety Systems, Virginia Tech Transportation Institute, Blacksburg, VA, United States

Driving is central to adult independence and autonomy; yet most autistic young adults do not acquire driver’s licenses. It is important to understand barriers to achieving this milestone for autistic adults. Differences in negative affect and emotion dysregulation associated with autism may interfere with managing difficult driving situations. The current study compared perceived driving difficulty (DD), emotion dysregulation, and negative affect in emerging drivers with and without autistic traits (AT), and investigated how emotion dysregulation and negative affect relate to perceived DD. We expected (1) greater perceived DD, emotion dysregulation, and negative affect in participants with AT and (2) a positive correlation of perceived DD with both emotion dysregulation and negative affect in the whole sample. Thirty-seven adolescents and young adults (15 AT) self-reported perceived DD in 15 scenarios and completed the Difficulty in Emotion Dysregulation Scale (DERS) and the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS). Autistic participants scored significantly higher on mean perceived DD, DERS Impulse subscale, DASS total and DASS Stress subscale scores. Perceived DD positively correlated with the DERS and DASS total scores, all DASS subscales, and DERS Nonacceptance, Goals, and Impulse subscales across the whole sample. The findings highlight the roles of emotion dysregulation and negative affect in perceived DD in emerging drivers with AT. In particular, emotional stress and impulsivity may map onto mechanisms of over-reactivity to negative affect and explain why autistic people perceive particular situations as difficult when driving. Implications and directions for future research are discussed.

Introduction

The Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee (2017) outlined strategic research targets for the transition to adult services and continued lifespan support. Driving can be considered an appropriate transition target, as it is a central skill that enables successful, independent adult living by providing opportunities to increase economic, educational, and social well-being. Driving, for example, has been shown to increase employment, educational, and social opportunities for autistic adults (Feeley, 2010; Daly et al., 2014; Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee, 2017; Curry et al., 2018; Myers et al., 2019). Autistic adolescents are more likely to be enrolled in post-secondary education or employed if they have their driver’s license and are more likely to obtain licensure if their individualized education plans include driving-related goals (Huang et al., 2012). Longitudinal research on outcomes of autistic adults found that increased community and employment opportunities contributed to greater physical and mental health and reduced maladaptive behaviors (Taylor and Mailick, 2014). Altogether, vocational activities are supported by obtaining a driver’s license and can help autistic individuals achieve better overall outcomes.

The primary characteristics of autism1 are social communication/interaction differences and repetitive, restricted behaviors (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). These characteristics may not directly hinder skills necessary for safe vehicle control, yet autistic individuals obtain driver’s licenses less frequently, experience greater difficulty learning to drive, and exhibit more maladaptive driving behaviors compared to non-autistic (NA) people (Feeley, 2010; Sheppard et al., 2010; Cox et al., 2012; Reimer et al., 2013; Daly et al., 2014; Curry et al., 2018; Myers et al., 2019). Only 34% of autistic adolescents have their driver’s licenses compared to 83.5% of NA peers, and autistic adolescents acquire their licenses on average nine months later than their peers (Curry et al., 2018). Klin et al. (2003) posited that autistic individuals have poor generalization abilities, preventing the translation of knowledge learned in a structured environment (e.g., explicit rules) to performance in a naturalistic environment, such as driving. Indeed, previous research has used virtual reality to simulate more realistic application of driving skills to autistic youth in a controllable setting with unique, adaptive responsive technology (Bian et al., 2015, 2016; Wade et al., 2016). As such, autistic individuals may benefit from driving instruction that considers their unique learning styles.

Autistic adolescents reportedly struggle with learning the necessary higher-order skills for driving (e.g., multitasking, interpreting other drivers’ non-verbal cues, adapting to unexpected changes) and can benefit from greater parental engagement when learning to drive, individualized driving instruction, and having developed independent daily living skills before beginning to drive (Cox et al., 2012; Myers et al., 2019). While watching videos of other driving scenarios, autistic adults exhibited slower reaction times and identified fewer social hazards, such as pedestrians stepping in front of the vehicle and cyclists crossing the road (Sheppard et al., 2010). Research on the physiological experiences of autistic drivers found increased heart rate and higher eye gaze suggesting greater anxious arousal and less orientation to objects in the driver’s low visual field (e.g., dashboard, directly oncoming vehicles; Reimer et al., 2013). In a simulated driving performance, autistic characteristics, such as insistence on sameness, can affect driving with road detours or unpredictable pedestrian activity and may be correlated with impaired emotional responding (Cox et al., 2020). In addition, autistic drivers experience more difficulties with real driving situations, including driving in traffic, at night, and on highway, and receive more moving violations (Daly et al., 2014; Chee et al., 2017).

Emotion dysregulation can be defined as the inability to modulate affective states and/or to flexibly use strategies to achieve goals and maintain situationally appropriate behaviors (Thompson, 1994). On the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz and Roemer, 2004), in a NA sample, Trógolo et al. (2014) found that the subscales Nonacceptance, Goals, and Awareness predicted poor driving behaviors (e.g., misjudging speed of oncoming car, poor route planning, failing to see pedestrians/other objects), the subscales Nonacceptance and Awareness predicted anxious driving (e.g., feeling nervous or impatient while driving), and the subscales Strategies and Impulse predicted risky driving (e.g., speeding). Although emotion regulation is not diagnostic for autism, emotion dysregulation is argued to underlie other diagnoses that often co-occur with autism, such as depression, anxiety, and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and can lead to functional impairment (Simonoff et al., 2008; Anderson et al., 2011; Mazefsky et al., 2013; Gotham et al., 2015). Thus, it is possible that emotion dysregulation would also be associated with driving difficulty (DD) in autistic drivers. Relatedly, a systematic review of driving factors for autism highlighted that novice autistic drivers experience unique challenges in driving confidence and should develop emotional coping mechanisms (Lindsay, 2017).

In addition to emotion dysregulation, negative affect itself may be related to DD. Previous studies have found that greater negative affect symptoms specific to anxious or depressed mood, for example, are associated with greater risky driving behaviors (Scott-Parker et al., 2011; Trógolo et al., 2014). Moreover, difficulty inhibiting strong negative emotions like anxiety or anger predicted risky driving behaviors (Ulleberg and Rundmo, 2003; Oltedal and Rundmo, 2006; Hayley et al., 2017). These results suggest that negative affect and emotion dysregulation may contribute to DD, although these studies have not been conducted in autistic samples so it is unknown how these factors are impacted in autism. Nonetheless, there is strong context for further study on the association of emotion dysregulation and negative affect with DD in autism, which could have implications for specialized supports when learning to drive.

In this study, we aimed to clarify relationships between emotion regulation, negative affect, and perceived DD in adolescents and young adults with and without autistic traits (AT). Specifically, this study tested the hypotheses that (1) perceived DD, emotion dysregulation, and negative affect scores would be greater in participants with AT and (2) there would be a positive correlation of perceived DD with emotion dysregulation and negative affect in the whole sample. Measures that evaluate these emotional processes may provide guidance and quantify potential challenges in novice drivers with AT.

Method

Participants

The study recruited individuals aged 15 years or older from the Virginia Tech (VT) Autism Clinic & Center for Autism Research registry and the community via e-mails and electronic fliers to participate in an online survey regarding driving attitudes from June 2015 to July 2017. Individuals who self-reported any diagnosis of autism comprised the AT group (n = 15), and those without any self-reported previous autism diagnosis comprised the NA group (n = 22). Participants were required to be able to read and understand the consent/assent forms and survey items. Parental consent and child assent were collected for child participants and informed consent from all adult participants. Informed consent was monitored, and all study procedures were approved by the VT Institutional Review Board. Participants were compensated up to $10.00 for completing online questionnaires. The total sample consisted of 37 individuals (M = 17.5 years, SD = 2.16, range: 15–24). A total of 65% identified as male and 81% as white (Table 1). The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) was further used to characterize the sample, with significantly higher autistic characteristics in the AT versus NA group (see section “The AQ” for more details).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive variables.

Those in the AT group self-reported the following co-occurring conditions: ADHD (n = 7), anxiety disorder (n = 7), mild intellectual disability (n = 1), schizophrenia (n = 1), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (n = 1); those in the NA group only self-reported co-occurring ADHD (n = 3).

Questionnaires

Driving Metrics

Participants provided driving metrics (e.g., enrollment of driver’s education, status and age of licensure), and rated perceived DD on a four-point scale (1 = very easy to 4 = very difficult) of 15 situations they may encounter while driving: during the daytime, the nighttime, and dawn/dusk, in bad weather, heavy traffic, towns, cities/urban areas, construction zones, and familiar and unfamiliar areas, on a highway/interstate, with silent and talking passengers, without any passengers, and near pedestrians/bicyclists.

The perceived DD scale was developed for this study and presented scenarios predicted to potentially elicit higher perceived DD ratings, including driving at night, in bad weather, with passengers, near pedestrians, etc., along with scenarios such as driving in familiar areas or during the daytime that may elicit lower perceived DD ratings. Scenarios were selected based on findings from previous literature that reviewed driving scenarios that autistic people have reportedly struggled in Daly et al. (2014), Chee et al. (2017). Perceived DD items were analyzed individually across groups and averaged for a total score, where higher scores indicate greater perceived difficulty. Internal consistency for perceived DD for the current sample was acceptable for both groups (NA, Cronbach’s α = 0.88; AT, α = 0.76).

The AQ

The AQ is a 50-item, self-report questionnaire that rates autistic characteristics in adults as “definitely agree,” “slightly agree,” “slightly disagree,” or “definitely disagree” (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). The AQ has reasonable construct validity. and was used to characterize autistic characteristics in this self-reported sample of participants (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). In the current sample, the total score was significantly greater in the AT group (M = 31.13, SD = 7.16) than the NA group (M = 19.69, SD = 6.07), t(35) = 5.24, p < 0.001, g = 1.71, and internal consistency was acceptable for both groups (NA, α = 0.72; AT, α = 0.83).

DERS

The DERS is a 36-item, self-report questionnaire that assesses frequency of difficulties in emotion regulation, rated on a five-point Likert Scale (1 = almost never to 5 = almost always) resulting in six dimensions of regulation: nonacceptance of emotional responses (Nonacceptance), difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior (Goals), impulse control difficulties (Impulse), lack of emotional awareness (Awareness), limited access to emotion regulation strategies (Strategies), and lack of emotional clarity (Clarity; Gratz and Roemer, 2004). Some items were reverse coded, and responses were summed for a total score and six subscale scores. Higher scores indicate greater emotion regulation difficulty. The DERS demonstrated good psychometrics in two separate samples of college-aged adults, one autistic and one NA, (Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Conner and White, 2018). There was excellent internal consistency in our sample for both groups (NA, α = 0.90; AT, α = 0.94).

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale

The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS) is a 21-item, self-report questionnaire that measures the severity of negative affect consistent with depression, anxiety, and stress (Antony et al., 1998). Participants rated items pertaining to emotional states in the past week on a four-point scale (0 = did not apply to me at all to 3 = applied to me very much/most of the time). Raw scores were summed and doubled creating a total score and three 7-item subscale scores (Depression, Anxiety, Stress). The Depression subscale measures hopelessness, lack of interest/involvement, and anhedonia. The Anxiety subscale measures autonomic arousal, bodily sensations, and subjective experience of anxious affect. The Stress subscale measures difficulty relaxing, being easily agitated, and irritability. The DASS demonstrated good psychometrics in autistic and NA adults recruited from clinical and community samples (Antony et al., 1998; Clara et al., 2001; Henry and Crawford, 2005; Nah et al., 2018). Our internal consistency was good for both groups (NA, α = 0.88; AT, α = 0.90).

Statistical Plan

We performed analyses using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0, 2019) with a two-tailed alpha = 0.05. We conducted independent-samples t-test and chi-square analyses to compare AT and NA groups in descriptive data, driving metrics, average total perceived DD score, and DERS and DASS total and subscale scores. To protect against Type-1 Error, we conducted Bonferroni corrections by setting the alpha at 0.003/0.008/0.017 for comparisons on the DD scale/DERS/DASS, respectively. For t-tests, we reported effect sizes as Hedge’s g to provide a magnitude of group differences. Effects equal to or greater than 0.2/0.5/0.8 were respectively considered small/medium/large (Cohen, 1988). We conducted Pearson correlations to quantify the relationship of perceived DD with the DERS and DASS in the full sample. Given that those with autism and co-occurring conditions may have more consequences on driving (e.g., Cox et al., 2016), we conducted additional one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc Bonferroni corrections that looked at AT and NA groups with and without anxiety and ADHD.

Sensitivity Power Analyses

We conducted sensitivity power analyses to indicate effect sizes that the current study was sufficiently powered to detect based on the current sample size and analytic approaches employed using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). For independent samples t-tests, the present study had sufficient statistical power (80%) to detect significant differences between the AT and NA groups that exceeded a large effect size (g ≥ 0.85). For chi-square analyses and Pearson correlations, respectively, the present study had a sufficient sample size to detect a significant χ2 difference of at least 3.84 and significant bivariate correlations of at least 0.40.

Results

Demographic Factors

There were no differences between the AT and NA groups on age, sex, or race (Table 1). Both groups reported similar levels of education, χ2(3, N = 37) = 0.87, p = 0.832, and having a job, χ2(1, N = 37) = 0.12, p = 0.729 (Table 1). Of those employed, the NA group worked significantly more hours per week, t(14) = 2.17, p = 0.048, g = 1.09.

Driving Education and Behavior

Among the 30 participants (n = 13 AT; 17 NA) who were eligible for a driver’s license in Virginia (at least 16 years old), significantly fewer participants in the AT group reported having a current driver’s license than in the NA group (χ2 = 4.44, p = 0.035). Also, a significantly greater proportion of individuals in the AT group reported a delay in obtaining licensure than in the NA group (χ2 = 8.17, p = 0.004; Table 2). The most common reason for delay was intrinsically related (66.7%; e.g., fear/stress around driving) for the AT group compared to extrinsically related (50%; e.g., parental permission) for the NA group. The proportions of those with a current or past driver’s license or learner’s permit or without ever having a license or permit did not differ significantly between the AT and NA groups in the whole sample (χ2 = 4.46, p = 0.216; Table 2). Of those without a current driver’s license, AT participants expressed wanting a driver’s license (χ2 = 1.16, p = 0.283), and attended and completed driver’s education at the same rate as NA participants (χ2 = 2.34, p = 0.310; Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Group differences on driving in participants 16 + years old.

The average perceived DD score was not related to sex (t[34] = 0.22, p = 0.825), age (t[37] = 0.12, p = 0.509), or having a driver’s license (t[27] = 1.32, p = 0.199). On average, the AT group reported significantly greater perceived DD than the NA group (t[35] = −4.04, p < 0.001; Table 3). The AT group rated all 15 driving situations as more difficult than the NA group, and significantly more difficult for driving in towns and with talking passengers2 (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Group differences on DD, the DERS, and the DASS.

An exploratory ANOVA compared AT participants with and without a self-reported anxiety disorder to each other, and also to NA participants without an anxiety disorder on perceived DD. AT participants with and without co-occurring anxiety did not significantly differ from each other, but both AT groups rated significantly greater overall DD than the NA without anxiety group (Supplementary Table 1).

Similarly, an exploratory ANOVA compared AT participants with and without a self-reported ADHD to each other, and also to NA participants with and without ADHD on perceived DD. There were no significant differences between AT participants with and without co-occurring ADHD and between the NA participants with and without co-occurring ADHD. However, both AT groups indicated greater overall perceived DD than the NA without ADHD but not when compared to the NA with ADHD group (Supplementary Table 2).

Emotion Regulation and Negative Affect

Independent sample t-tests showed significant differences between AT and NA groups on the DERS and DASS (Table 3). The AT group reported significantly higher scores on the DERS Impulse, DASS Total, and DASS Stress.

Perceived Driving Difficulty and Emotionality

Across the whole sample, increased perceived DD was significantly (r = 0.52--0.53) correlated to increased DASS Total and Stress subscale3 (Table 4). The DERS subscales Awareness, Strategies, and Clarity were not significantly correlated with DD (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlation with perceived driving difficulty.

Discussion

The current study examined the relationship of negative affect and emotion dysregulation with perceived DD in AT and NA participants who are interested in driving. The results of this study contribute to understanding the transition to adulthood for autistic individuals by highlighting the emotional difficulties they may face when achieving driving goals. These findings indicate significant group differences between individuals with and without autistic traits on driving metrics, perceived DD, emotion dysregulation, and negative affect.

Regarding their driving metrics, the AT group was less likely to obtain their driver’s license on time or at all which is consistent with previous studies of autistic individuals (Daly et al., 2014; Curry et al., 2018). Additionally, AT participants reported greater perceived DD and experienced significantly greater stress than NA individuals. A further look into the perceived DD scale showed that driving areas that AT participants rated significantly more difficult included scenarios that may be more demanding due to their unpredictable or distracting nature. These results support previous studies in autistic samples that have suggested that responding to unexpected situations is negatively impacted due to impaired emotionality (Sheppard et al., 2010; Cox et al., 2020). Exploratory analyses found that AT participants with a co-occurring disorder of anxiety or ADHD experienced overall DD when compared to the NA group; however, there were no differences within the AT groups regardless of co-occurring anxiety or ADHD, suggesting that perceived DD is not better explained by additional symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity and anxiety in AT participants, but could be the transdiagnostic features of emotion dysregulation and negative affect in autistic people. However, any endorsement of co-occurring conditions is self-reported which may not reflect the extent to which these symptoms could influence driving perceptions.

Correlations indicated that greater overall perceived DD was related to increased stress on the DASS. Although they were not significant after Bonferroni corrections, some of the DERS (Total and subscales: Nonacceptance, Goals, Impulse) were positively correlated with perceived DD. These DERS subscales may affect perceptions of demanding driving situations because their items map onto negative biases of emotional states (e.g., “When I am upset, I feel weak”—Nonacceptance), poor cognitive control (e.g., “I have difficulty concentrating”—Goals), and poor behavioral control (e.g., “I have difficulty controlling my behaviors”—Impulse), whereas the non-significant subscales target meta-cognitive concepts not directly related to views toward driving (e.g., “I believe that my feelings are important”—Awareness, “I believe there is nothing I can do to feel better”—Strategies, “I do not know how I am feeling”—Clarity). These findings are consistent with findings by Trógolo et al. (2014) that correlated risky driving styles to the Nonacceptance, Goals, and Impulse subscales and explained that broader internalizing symptomology (e.g., anxiety) can predict maladaptive driving styles, and further extends those findings to include autistic participants.

Moreover, of the current participants who reported a delay in obtaining their driver’s license, AT participants attributed their delay to negative internal feelings toward driving compared to the extrinsically related factors reported by those in the NA group. This related to the greater understanding that autistic individuals may experience a more intense reaction to negative affect (Mazefsky et al., 2013), and they converge with the results that showed an increase in negative affect and dysregulation from the DERS and DASS. Overall, these elevations begin to contribute to the relationship of emotion dysregulation and negative affect and perceived DD in new autistic drivers. These findings also align with the idea that autistic individuals may need specific support for socio-emotional differences when learning to drive as, for example, the intervention recently described in Baker-Ericzén et al. (2020). In addition, virtual reality driving simulations can transform generalized skill instruction for autistic learners, highlighted as a unique learning difficulty by Klin et al. (2003), by providing adaptive driving instructions that can respond to their real-time physiological arousal (Bian et al., 2019).

Lastly, the NA group had similar employment rates to the AT group; however, those in the NA group worked 12 more weekly hours on average than those in the AT group. Although our main objective was not to characterize employment, the current findings support the association between employment and independent driving considering that autistic drivers are generally provided more employment opportunities than their non-driving autistic peers (Feeley, 2010; Huang et al., 2012; Daly et al., 2014; Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee, 2017; Curry et al., 2018; Myers et al., 2019).

Limitations

Due to the nature of this online study, we were unable to administer a gold-standard assessment battery [e.g., Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord et al., 2012)] to confirm participants’ autism diagnostic status. Therefore, the primary limitation of this study was its use of a self-referred sample of autistic participants, and thus the sample may not be representative of the larger autism population. Although the average AQ total score for the AT group was right below the recommended cutoff (32) for autism, they scored significantly higher than the NA group which demonstrated group differences in autistic features between the two groups. Another limitation is the perceived DD scale that was developed for the purpose of this study, and therefore does not have established psychometrics. The current findings may overestimate driving behavior and attitudes due to selection bias such that those who are interested in or currently driving may have been more likely to participate. While the small sample size may present as a limitation, sensitivity power analyses demonstrated that the significant effects were large enough to be detected. Nonetheless, smaller effects may be overlooked and warrant further investigation with a larger sample.

Future directions include collecting more detailed data for a more complete profile of sensitive transition periods for autistic individuals. Future work would also benefit from conducting in-person visits with caregiver persepectives to allow for a comprehensive evaluation of cognitive, psychological, and objective driving abilities and confirm clinical group status. Conducting repeated visits would provide a prospective examination of how emotion regulation and negative affect relate to driving in autism. Finally, a larger sample would allow for more fine-grained analyses to examine the potential moderating role of associated mood and anxiety disorders in both AT and NA individuals as they prepare for driving. This study was limited by its cross-sectional descriptive design, and, therefore, a more comprehensive understanding of these relationships using longitudinal designs may contribute to developing interventions that address negative affect and emotion dysregulation related to driving.

Conclusion

The current study is the first to examine the relationships of emotion regulation, negative affect, and perceived difficulties with driving in a sample of AT and NA participants and address its implications for adult functioning. AT participants reported greater overall perceived DD in different situations, and greater impairment in emotion dysregulation and negative affect compared to NA participants. These initial findings lend support to understanding emotion dysregulation and negative affect as important parts to learning to drive in general and may be especially relevant considering emotion processing differences in emerging autistic drivers. Independent driving is an age-appropriate skill for daily living for autistic adolescents and young adults and provides greater employment opportunities. Previous research has supported that daily living skills may be more predictive of later adult employment than other childhood factors (academic success, symptom severity, and IQ; Taylor and Mailick, 2014). Preparing autistic adolescents and adults for driving by addressing the impact of emotion regulation skills may be useful to consider promoting successful independent functioning and later adult outcomes.

Data Availability Statement

The de-identified aggregate dataset may become available upon request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to MF, bWZva0B2dC5lZHU=.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by VT Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the adult participants and the parents of child participants, and assent was provided by the child participants.

Author Contributions

JO and AS conceptualized the study and oversaw data collection for analysis. MF contributed to the study conceptualization, data analysis, and interpretation of results. JO and TO contributed to the development and conceptualization of the perceived DD scale in the original study. All authors critically revised the article for intellectual content and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by a VT Center for Autism Research faculty seed grant.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the individuals who participated in this study and the staff at VT Autism Clinic & Center for Autism Research and VT Transportation Institute who contributed to data collection. We also thank Drs. Charlie Klauer and Denis Gracanin for initial contributions to the larger study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.754776/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ Autistic self-advocates recommend the use of identify-first (i.e., “autistic adults”) instead of person-first (i.e., “Adults with ASD”) language. Recent publications have recommended inclusive and non-ableist language such as using “autism” instead of “autism spectrum disorder” and “co-occurring” instead of “comorbid” (Botha et al., 2021; Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021; Bradshaw et al., 2021).
  2. ^ Before Bonferroni corrections, the AT group rated driving during the daytime, nighttime, heavy traffic, in cities/urban areas, construction zones, and unfamiliar areas, and without passengers as significantly higher.
  3. ^ When not corrected for multiple-comparison, all six DASS subscales, DERS Total, and DERS subscales for Nonacceptance, Goals, and Impulse were significantly positively correlated to perceived DD (ps = 0.014–0.049; Table 4).

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Anderson, D. K., Maye, M. P., and Lord, C. (2011). Changes in maladaptive behaviors from mid-childhood to young adulthood in autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 116, 381–397. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-116.5.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., and Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 10:176. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker-Ericzén, M. J., Smith, L., Tran, A., and Scarvie, K. (2020). A cognitive behavioral intervention for driving for autistic teens and adults: a pilot study. Autism Adulthood 3, 168–178. doi: 10.1089/aut.2020.0009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger Syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, D., Wade, J., Swanson, A., Warren, Z., and Sarkar, N. (2015). Physiology-based affect recognition during driving in virtual environment for autism intervention. Int. Conf. Physiol. Comput. Syst. 2, 137–145. doi: 10.5220/0005331301370145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, D., Wade, J., Swanson, A., Weitlauf, A., Warren, Z., and Sarkar, N. (2019). Design of a physiology-based adaptive virtual reality driving platform for individuals with ASD. ACM Trans. Assess. Comput. 12, 1–24. doi: 10.1145/3301498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, D., Wade, J., Warren, Z., and Sarkar, N. (2016). “Online engagement detection and task adaptation in a virtual reality based driving simulator for autism intervention,” in Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction. Users and Context Diversity. UAHCI 2016. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 9739, eds M. Antona and C. Stephanidis (Berlin: Springer International), 538–547.

Google Scholar

Botha, M., Hanlon, J., and Williams, G. L. (2021). Does language matter? Identity-first versus person-first language use in autism research: a response to Vivanti. J. Autism Dev. Disord. [Online ahead of print] 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04858-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottema-Beutel, K., Kapp, S. K., Lester, J. N., Sasson, N. J., and Hand, B. N. (2021). Avoiding ableist language: suggestions for autism researchers. Autism Adulthood 3, 18–29. doi: 10.1089/aut.2020.0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, P., Pickett, C., Brooker, K., Urbanowicz, A., and van Driel, M. (2021). “Autistic” or “with autism?” Why the way general practitioners view and talk about autism matters. Aust. J. Gen. Pract. 50, 104–108. doi: 10.31128/AJGP-11-20-5721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chee, D. Y., Lee, H. C., Patomella, A.-H., and Falkmer, T. (2017). Driving behaviour profile of drivers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 2658–2670. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3178-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clara, I. P., Cox, B. J., and Enns, M. W. (2001). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Depression–Anxiety–Stress Scales in depressed and anxious patients. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 23, 61–67. doi: 10.1023/A:1011095624717

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Conner, C. M., and White, S. W. (2018). Brief report: feasibility and preliminary efficacy of individual mindfulness therapy for adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 290–300. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3312-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, D. J., Owens, J. M., Barnes, L., Moncrief, M., Boukhechba, M., Buckman, S., et al. (2020). A pilot study comparing newly licensed drivers with and without autism and experienced drivers in simulated and on-road driving. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 1258–1268. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04341-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, N. B., Reeve, R. E., Cox, S. M., and Cox, D. J. (2012). Brief report: driving and young adults with ASD: parents’ experiences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2257–2262. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1470-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, S. M., Cox, D. J., Kofler, M. J., Moncrief, M. A., Johnson, R. J., Lambert, A. E., et al. (2016). Driving simulator performance in novice drivers with autism spectrum disorder: the role of executive functions and basic motor skills. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 1379–1391. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2677-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curry, A. E., Yerys, B. E., Huang, P., and Metzger, K. B. (2018). Longitudinal study of driver licensing rates among adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Int. J. Res. Pract. 22, 479–488. doi: 10.1177/1362361317699586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daly, B. P., Nicholls, E. G., Patrick, K. E., Brinckman, D. D., and Schultheis, M. T. (2014). Driving behaviors in adults with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 3119–3128. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2166-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeley, C. (2010). Evaluating the transportation needs and accessibility issues for adults on the autism spectrum in new jersey. Paper Presented the 89th Annual Meeting of the Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Gotham, K., Brunwasser, S. M., and Lord, C. (2015). Depressive and anxiety symptom trajectories from school age through young adulthood in samples with autism spectrum disorder and developmental delay. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 369.e3–376.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratz, K. L., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayley, A. C., Ridder, B., Stough, C., Ford, T. C., and Downey, L. A. (2017). Emotional intelligence and risky driving behaviour in adults. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 49, 124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2017.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., and Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 227–239. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, P., Kao, T., Curry, A. E., and Durbin, D. R. (2012). Factors associated with driving in teens with autism spectrum disorders. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 33, 70–74. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31823a43b7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0 (2019). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee (2017). 2016-2017 Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee Strategic Plan for Autism Spectrum Disorder. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee. Available online at: https://iacc.hhs.gov/publications/strategic-plan/2017/ (accessed June 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., and Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 358, 345–360. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindsay, S. (2017). Systematic review of factors affecting driving and motor vehicle transportation among people with autism spectrum disorder. Disabil. Rehabil. 39, 837–846. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2016.1161849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., and Bishop, S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: ADOS-2. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Mazefsky, C. A., Herrington, J., Siegel, M., Scarpa, A., Maddox, B. B., Scahill, L., et al. (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 679–688. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, R. K., Bonsu, J. M., Carey, M. E., Yerys, B. E., Mollen, C. J., and Curry, A. E. (2019). Teaching autistic adolescents and young adults to drive: perspectives of specialized driving instructors. Autism Adulthood 1, 202–209. doi: 10.1089/aut.2018.0054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nah, Y.-H., Brewer, N., Young, R. L., and Flower, R. (2018). Brief report: screening adults with autism spectrum disorder for anxiety and depression. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 1841–1846. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3427-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oltedal, S., and Rundmo, T. (2006). The effects of personality and gender on risky driving behaviour and accident involvement. Saf. Sci. 44, 621–628. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2005.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimer, B., Fried, R., Mehler, B., Joshi, G., Bolfek, A., Godfrey, K. M., et al. (2013). Brief report: examining driving behavior in young adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorders: a pilot study using a driving simulation paradigm. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2211–2217. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1764-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott-Parker, B., Watson, B., King, M. J., and Hyde, M. K. (2011). The psychological distress of the young driver: a brief report. Inj. Prev. 17, 275–277. doi: 10.1136/ip.2010.031328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheppard, E., Ropar, D., Underwood, G., and van Loon, E. (2010). Brief report: driving hazard perception in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 504–508. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0890-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., and Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 921–929. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., and Mailick, M. R. (2014). A longitudinal examination of 10-year change in vocational and educational activities for adults with autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Psychol. 50, 699–708. doi: 10.1037/a0034297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59, 25–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trógolo, M. A., Melchior, F., and Medrano, L. A. (2014). The role of difficulties in emotion regulation on driving behavior. J. Behav. Health Soc. Issues 6, 107–117. doi: 10.5460/jbhsi.v6.1.47607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulleberg, P., and Rundmo, T. (2003). Personality, attitudes and risk perception as predictors of risky driving behaviour among young drivers. Saf. Sci. 41, 427–443. doi: 10.1016/S0925-7535(01)00077-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, J., Zhang, L., Bian, D., Fan, J., Swanson, A., Weitlauf, A., et al. (2016). A gaze-contingent adaptive virtual reality driving environment for intervention in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. ACM Trans. Interact. Intell. Syst. (TIIS) 6, 1–23.

Google Scholar

Keywords: adults, driving, emotion regulation, negative affect, transition planning

Citation: Fok M, Owens JM, Ollendick TH and Scarpa A (2022) Perceived Driving Difficulty, Negative Affect, and Emotion Dysregulation in Self-Identified Autistic Emerging Drivers. Front. Psychol. 13:754776. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.754776

Received: 07 August 2021; Accepted: 03 January 2022;
Published: 31 January 2022.

Edited by:

Federica Biassoni, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Mirjana Djordjevic, University of Belgrade, Serbia
Tania Moretta, University of Padova, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Fok, Owens, Ollendick and Scarpa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Megan Fok, bWZva0B2dC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.