Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 13 January 2023
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Etiology and Pathogenesis of Affective Disorders View all 6 articles

Emotional stimulation processing characteristics in depression: Meta-analysis of eye tracking findings

\nGenying HuangGenying Huang1Yafang Li,
Yafang Li1,2*Huizhong ZhuHuizhong Zhu3Hong FengHong Feng1Xunbing Shen,Xunbing Shen1,2Zhencai Chen,Zhencai Chen1,2
  • 1College of Humanities, Jiangxi University of Chinese Medicine, Nanchang, China
  • 2Key Laboratory of Psychology of TCM and Brain Science, Jiangxi Administration of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Jiangxi University of Chinese Medicine, Nanchang, China
  • 3College of Acupuncture and Massage, Jiangxi University of Chinese Medicine, Nanchang, China

Objective: To systematically evaluate the attentional bias in patients with depression toward emotional stimuli and to explore eye movement indicators and potential regulatory variables that can distinguish such patients from healthy individuals.

Methods: Case–control studies regarding eye-tracking in major depressive disorder published in PubMed, Web of Science, ScienceDirect, The Cochrane Library, EBSCOhost, Embase, China National Knowledge Infrastructure, Wanfang, and VIP databases from database initiation until March 12, 2022 were included in the present meta-analysis. Two researchers independently screened the literature and performed data extraction. The quality of the literature was assessed using the Newcastle–Ottawa quality assessment scale.RevMan 5.4 software was used for Meta-analysis.

Results: Overall, 14 studies were included, including 1,167 participants (Ndepression = 474; Nhealthy = 693). We found that (1) fixation duration was significantly lower for positive emotional stimuli in the depression group than that in the healthy group; however, for negative stimuli, the fixation duration was significantly more in the depression group than in the healthy group. No significant difference was observed in terms of neutral emotional stimuli between groups. (2) Patients with depression exhibited a significantly lower fixation count for positive emotional stimuli than healthy individuals, whereas the fixation count for negative emotional stimuli was significantly higher in the depression group than in the healthy group. No significant difference was found for neutral emotional stimuli between groups. (3) No significant difference was detected in terms of the first fixation duration of the positive, negative, and neutral emotional stimuli between groups. (4) subgroup analysis indicated that age effected fixation duration for positive emotional stimuli. In addition, age and the type of negative emotional picture (sad, dysphoric, threat, anger) effected fixation duration for negative emotional stimuli.

Conclusion: Our research supports that patients with depression exhibit a negative attention bias toward emotional stimuli, and the fixation duration and fixation counts may be used as auxiliary objective indicators for depression screening.

1. Introduction

Depression is a common mental disorder with high recurrence and disability rates. The recurrence rate is 40–60% after the first episode, increasing to 90% after three episodes (van Rijsbergen et al., 2015). Depression affects >300 million people globally, constituting a major contributor to the global burden of disease (Beurel et al., 2020). Presently, medical practitioners utilize subjective diagnostic criteria to diagnose depression and evaluate patient symptoms during a clinical interview. Hence, the reliable diagnosis of major depressive disorder (MDD) using objective diagnostic criteria is challenging (Mitchell et al., 2009; Ding et al., 2019).

Attentional biases involved in the cognitive processing of emotional information may not only be a key factor in the etiology, maintenance, and recurrence of depression but also be vital for its diagnosis and treatment (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010; Disner et al., 2017). Eye tracking has proved to be an important technology for studying attentional bias and improving diagnostic techniques. A major advantage of eye tracking is that it directly and continuously records the allocation of attention over an extended period while multiple stimuli compete for attention (Figueiredo et al., 2019).

However, ambiguities remain in the present understanding regarding attentional bias in patients with depression. According to Beck's cognitive model of depression, biased acquisition and processing of negative information plays a primary role in the development and maintenance of depression (Beck, 2008; Disner et al., 2011). The model of emotion context insensitivity of depression proposes that patients with depression process emotional stimuli more slowly than healthy individuals and are consequently insensitive to both positive and negative emotional stimuli but more sensitive to neutral emotional stimuli (Rottenberg et al., 2005; Isaacowitz et al., 2006; Bylsma et al., 2008). As attentional bias in patients with depression is inconsistent, Suslow et al. performed a systematic narrative and meta-analytic review of eye tracking studies involving patients with clinical depression and examined specific aspects of attention or performance in specific tasks (Suslow et al., 2020). However, a limitation of this review was that only studies that used free viewing of images (n = 4) were included. Researchers utilizing eye tracking technology have typically used free-viewing tasks wherein participants are presented with multiple emotional stimuli in the form of emotional scenes, faces, and words or a combination of stimuli with the provision of limited instructions (van Rijsbergen et al., 2015). This provides data regarding the natural direct and continuous allocation of attention of the individuals in the free-viewing tasks (Hermans et al., 1999). Herein, we sought to aggregate and evaluate the available eye tracking research in the literature that used free-viewing studies based on images to better characterize clinical depression with regard to attentional biases for emotional stimuli and the potential moderators of their effects.

There are some studies of depression were not conducted under the assistance of psychiatrists (such as in the context of school counseling). Nevertheless, these studies do have some clinical meanings, and this kind of studies also was often seen in depression screening by using instruments of PHQ-9 and BDI as included criteria for clinical depression. Thus, these studies were included in the current meta-analysis. Furthermore, a subgroup analysis was performed to compare the population of depression based on ICD-10 and DSM-5 diagnostic criteria with the population diagnosed by the scales of PHQ-9 and BDI.

2. Methods

2.1. Search strategy

The key search terms used were “depression,” “depressive disorder,” “MDD,” “eye tracking,” “eye movement,” and “attention bias (AB).” A list of synonyms was created for each key term. The key terms were searched along with their synonyms in each separate database. The databases searched included PubMed, Web of Science, ScienceDirect, EBSCOhost, The Cochrane Library, Embase, China National Knowledge Infrastructure, Wanfang Data, and VIP database. The search window extended from when the databases were created to March 2022.

Two of the authors (Huang and Zhu) conducted independent systematic literature and manual searches of the reference list of the previous meta-analysis, and the retrieved articles were assessed.

2.2. Eligibility criteria

The inclusion criteria of this meta-analysis were as follows: (a) studies that published case–control analysis on eye tracking of individuals with depression, (b) patients aged ≥ 18 years who met the current international diagnostic criteria (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder 5 [DSM-5] or International Classification of Diseases) or were clinically diagnosed with depression as the case group and healthy individuals as the control group, (c) studies that used free-viewing tasks, and (d) studies that contained statistics and sufficient data for analyses.

Exclusion criteria were (a) review articles or meta-analyses, case reports, (b) eye movement studies examining patients with other disorders, and (c) articles that did not meet the diagnostic criteria for patients with depression in the DSM, ICD, or depression Diagnostic Scale, questionnaire (i.e., BDI, HAMD, PHQ-9).

2.3. Study selection

After removing duplicates, the titles and abstracts of the screened studies were independently screened by authors (Huang and Zhu) and any non-pertinent studies were excluded. The final list comprised studies accepted by both the authors. If the inclusion criteria were met, the full-text article was retrieved and screened to evaluate the available data for analysis.

2.4. Data collection process

Data extraction was independently performed by two authors (Huang and Zhu). Disagreements were resolved via discussion with another author (Li) until consensus was reached.

2.5. Data items

Data extraction included the following: (a) basic information regarding the included literature: title, first author, and publication time; (b) basic characteristics of the patients: diagnostic criteria for depression, average age, number of samples in each group, and stimulation materials; (c) eye tracking parameters applied in the free-viewing tasks: fixation duration, dwell time (in ms or %), and number of fixation time (in frequency or %).

2.6. Synthesis of results

Review Manager version 5.4.1. was used for statistical analysis and generation of forest and funnel plots. The effect value was expressed via standardized mean difference (SMD) along with 95% CI; larger the absolute value of SMD, larger the effect size. Chi-square test was used for heterogeneity test, and I2 ≤ 50% and P >0.10 were considered low heterogeneity. According to the research theory and purpose, this study adopted the random effect model (Borenstein et al., 2009).

2.7. Subgroup analysis

Q statistic was used to determine if subgroup analysis was necessary, as significant heterogeneity suggested that there were factors accounting for between-study variance in the effect size. A priori moderator variables included patient characteristics (age) and experimental characteristics (stimulus duration and emotional stimulus type).

2.8. Publication bias

To evaluate for publication bias and the stability of the results, we conducted sensitivity analysis and constructed separate funnel plots to determine fixation duration, number of fixation, and first fixation duration (Jin et al., 2015). We assessed the risk of bias using NOS Literature Quality Evaluation Scale for all the included studies (Stang, 2010).

3. Results

3.1. Study selection and characteristics

The search strategy identified 13,198 articles, which was reduced to 9,214 after excluding duplicates. After screening the titles and abstracts of these articles, 96 were included for full-text screening and 82 were excluded. In total, 14 articles met the inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the excluded and included studies.

These 14 eye tracking studies included 474 patients with depression and 693 healthy controls; participants were arranged in terms of age, and only two studies included older individuals. All the studies involved free-viewing tasks. In these studies, the participants were asked to simply observe emotional stimuli while they were shown faces or images. The emotional stimuli included being happy, positive, neutral, negative, sad, dysphoric, threatened, or angry. The duration of stimulus presentation ranged from 4 to 30 s. The outcome measures included early and later attentional processes (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies included in the meta-analysis.

3.2. Publication bias

Publication bias was measured using funnel plots, and potential heterogeneity was eliminated using the one-by-one exclusion method. Publication bias minimally affected the results, and sensitivity analysis established the conclusion as relatively robust (see Figures 25).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Funnel plots of publication bias of first fixation duration (A), fixation duration (B), and fixation count (C).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Sensitivity analysis of first fixation time of positive emotional stimulus (A), neural emotional stimulus (B), and negative emotional stimulus (C).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Sensitivity analysis of fixation duration of positive emotional stimulus (A), neural emotional stimulus (B), and negative emotional stimulus (C).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Sensitivity analysis of fixation count of positive emotional stimulus (A), neural emotional stimulus (B), and negative emotional stimulus (C).

3.3. Quality of individual studies

The quality of the included studies was determined using the 10-point Newcastle–Ottawa scale (NOS; Table 2). Of the analyzed studies, two scored 5 points, six scored 6 points, and six scored 7 points. Thus, the high quality of the literature indicates that publication bias exhibited minimal effect on the results.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality of individual studies.

3.4. Synthesis of results

3.4.1. Attention maintenance on emotional stimuli

The fixation duration for positive emotional stimuli was significantly lower in patients with depression than in healthy individuals (SMD = −0.71, P = 0.004), although for negative emotional stimuli, the fixation duration was significantly higher in patients with depression than in healthy individuals (SMD = 0.59, P = 0.0004) (Table 3). There was no significant difference for neutral emotional stimuli between groups (SMD = 0.01, P = 0.94). With regard to the number of fixations, patients with depression exhibited a significantly lower fixation count for positive emotional stimuli than healthy individuals (SMD = −0.87, P = 0.004), whereas the fixation count for negative emotional stimuli was significantly higher in the depression group than in the healthy controls (SMD = 0.32, P = 0.006). There was no significant difference for neutral emotional stimuli between groups (SMD = −0.13, P = 0.44). For the first fixation duration, no significant difference was observed for positive, negative, and neutral emotional stimuli between groups.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Eye movement characteristics of patients with depression.

According to Cohen's definition of effect size (where 0.2–0.5, 0.5–0.8, and >0.8 represent small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively), the number of fixations for positive emotional stimuli had a large effect size, the fixation time for both positive and negative emotional stimuli had a medium effect size, and the total gaze number for negative emotional stimuli had a small effect size. This shows that the processing of emotional stimuli varies in patients with depression from that in healthy individuals and that the fixation time and number of fixation is important for processing positive and negative stimuli, and thus useful for diagnosing depression.

3.4.2. Analysis of subgroups

According to the Cochrane evaluation system, ≤ 50% of heterogeneity in I2 is acceptable. Herein, the I2 of patients with depression in terms of fixation time for positive and negative emotional stimuli was 89% and 87% (P < 0.00001), respectively, indicating that regulatory variables may influence the effect size and that further analysis of the role of regulatory variables is warranted. Age, stimulus presentation time, emotional valence, and emotional stimulus type (emotional face and scene) potentially influences the fixation time of the participants. Therefore, we analyzed whether these four variables modulated fixation time (Tables 4, 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Moderate effects of fixation duration on negative emotional stimuli.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Moderate effect of fixation duration under positive emotional stimulation.

For fixation time in response to negative stimulus, subgroup analyses showed that (a) The effect size of the young and middle-aged group (SMD = 1.21) was larger than that of the older group (SMD = −0.19), with the effect size of the young and middle-aged group being significant (P = 0.0007). (b) The effect size (SMD) of the sadness and anger stimuli was 0.86 and 0.98, respectively, with that of the sadness stimuli being significant (P = 0.02) and that of anger stimuli being highly significant (P < 0.00001). The effect size of the threatening and dysphoric pictures was not significant. (c) There were no significant moderating effects on the time of stimulus presentation or the type of emotional stimuli.

For fixation time in response to positive stimulus, subgroup analyses showed that the effect size of the young and middle-aged group (SMD = −0.85) was larger than that of the older group (SMD = −0.18), with the effect size of the young and middle-aged group being significant (P = 0.005). The time of stimulus presentation and the type of emotional stimuli exhibited no significant interaction effect.

4. Discussion

The eye-tracking paradigm has gained more and more importance in depression research in recent years. Eye-tracking devices trace the time course of visual attention by almost continuously recording eye movements, thus allowing for revealing the characteristics of attention processing (Bianchi and Laurent, 2015; Lazarov et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2022). Fixation eye movement indicators are widely used in attention bias research of depression, mainly including first fixation time, fixation duration, fixation number, etc. The first fixation duration generally represents the characteristics of early processing, which can reflect the attention attraction of the emotional stimulus (Rantanen et al., 2021). The fixation duration generally represents the processing difficulty, a longer fixation duration indicates a higher level of attention and a corresponding greater processing difficulty (Isaac et al., 2014). The number of fixations primarily reflects the information extraction efficiency and information processing ability of the cognitive process (Roux et al., 2016; Akram et al., 2020). In this meta-analysis, published evidence regarding the characteristics of clinical depression with respect to attentional biases in response to emotional stimuli and their potential moderator effects was analyzed. The primary outcome of this study was that in patients with depression, the fixation time and fixation count for negative and positive emotional stimuli were considerably higher and lower than those in healthy patients, respectively. Age was an important regulatory variable of fixation time for positive emotional stimuli, and age and type of negative emotional stimuli were regulatory variables for the fixation time of negative emotional stimuli.

4.1. Patients with depression showed attentional maintenance of negative emotional stimuli

Fixation duration has been considered indicative of the depth of individual cognitive processing and maintenance of a stimulus. Our meta-analysis identified that the effect sizes of fixation time for negative and positive emotional stimuli reached 0.59 and −0.71, respectively. This suggests moderate differences between patients with depression and healthy individuals in terms of attention maintenance for negative and positive emotional stimuli. Our results are similar to those reported by Suslow et al. (2020). A study regarding the “antireward center” mechanism of depression reported that the abnormal increase in the activity of the lateral bridle nucleus neurons in patients with depression does not only prevent the processing of positive emotional information but also inhibits the positive processing of reward stimuli. Furthermore, the insensitivity of such patients toward reward stimuli impacts the goal-directed attentional system (Gold and Kadriu, 2019). This confirms that patients with depression exhibit insufficient bias toward positive emotional information and excessive cognitive processing and maintenance of negative emotional information.

The number of fixations primarily reflects the information extraction efficiency and information processing ability of the cognitive process. Patients with depression looked at fewer positive images compared with healthy individuals. The effect sizes of the fixation times for negative and positive emotional stimuli (SMD = 0.32 and −0.87, respectively) suggest moderate differences between patients with depression and healthy individuals with respect to attention maintenance in response to negative and positive emotions. The results of event-related potential studies (Ke et al., 2020) also confirmed that patients with depression exhibited electroencephalogram characteristics of prolonged response time when processing negative emotional information. This indicates that patients with depression extract negative emotional information with low efficiency and poor information processing ability.

The first fixation duration chiefly reflects the early attention given to directional processing characteristics (Grafton et al., 2016). No significant group differences between patients with depression and healthy individuals were observed for initial attention orientation to negative, neutral, or positive emotional stimuli, consistent with previous findings (Grafton et al., 2016). Thus, patients with depression appear to not be characterized by alterations during early orientation toward emotional stimuli (Suslow et al., 2020).

Difficulties in separating negative emotional information may be an important reason underlying the onset and maintenance of depression. In addition, functional brain imaging studies have demonstrated that the intensity of cortical activity, such as in the right prefrontal and right parietal lobe, weakens in depression, and that this cortical function is primarily associated with functions such as attentional selection and transfer (Skinner et al., 2018; Yaroslavsky et al., 2019). However, the electrophysiological indicators involved in the two-stage model framework of emotional attention regulation suggest that patients with depression mostly exhibit an enhanced P3 amplitude and negative bias during early attention allocation (Ao et al., 2020). However, controversy remains concerning the occurrence stage of negative attention bias, and this may be related to the lack of consistency in the emotional stimulus types and presentation time in the various studies. Hence, more evidence regarding the onset stage of negative attention bias is required.

4.2. Age and negative emotional type affect attention processing in patients with depression

The subgroup analysis revealed a significant effect of age. Patients with depression who were young and middle-aged observed more negative emotional stimuli (SMD = 1.21) and less positive emotional stimuli (SMD = −0.85) than healthy individuals, although no significant difference between older healthy individuals and older patients with depression was observed. According to the socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen et al., 1999; Carstensen, 2006), older adults prioritize emotion-regulatory goals. Furthermore, individual attentional sensitivity to negative stimuli gradually decreases with age (Mather and Carstensen, 2003; Lu et al., 2017); structural and functional changes in the brain also follow the same trajectory of behavioral change (Luna and Sweeney, 2004). These findings confirm the presence of emotional attentional biases in patients with MDD that are influenced by age. Attention bias modification (ABM) techniques have been confirmed to effectively improve cognitive bias modification procedures in depression (Vazquez et al., 2016). Consequently, clinicians should select the corresponding intervention based on patient age to improve clinical efficacy of ABM techniques. However, as there are presently relatively few empirical studies regarding eye movements in older patients with depression, more studies are warranted to confirm these preliminary results.

The subgroup analysis revealed a significant effect of negative emotional type. For instance, the effect size of fixation time on the emotional stimuli such as sadness and anger was 0.86 and 0.98, respectively, indicating a more significant attentional bias toward these stimuli. Daily negative emotions persist in depression, especially constant sadness (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In addition, patients with MDD exhibited inflexibility in sadness and avoidance, which were associated with weaker FC between the right sgACC and pregenual/dorsal anterior cingulate (pg/dACC) (Schwartz et al., 2019). Furthermore, the emotions sadness and anger have been highly associated with depression (Koh et al., 2002; Ou and Hall, 2018). According to clinical surveys, the irritability rate of patients with depression can be as high as 30–85% (Bodner et al., 2018), and the DSM-5 views anger as a marker of depression in children and adolescents (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Anger and irritability are prevalent in adults with depression and hinder the correction of depression cognitive bias more strongly than other negative emotions (Perlis et al., 2009). Our results are similar to those of previous studies, thus verifying that sadness and anger play an important etiological role in developing depression, which further helps simulate the treatment of emotion regulation and attentional bias correction in depression.

The interaction effect of the stimulus presentation time on emotional stimuli was not significant. Our results are similar to those of a meta-analysis conducted by Peckham who found no evidence regarding the significant moderation of biased attention toward negative information by stimulus presentation time in emotional Stroop or dot probe tasks (Peckham et al., 2013). Total gain duration and early stage of automatic attention allocation are both indicators of attention bias. Total gain duration usually used as attention maintenance index to evaluate negative attention bias (Duque and Vázquez, 2015; Lazarov et al., 2018), and it is reported that there was a significant main effect of stimulus presentation time on total gain duration (Wells et al., 2014). Previous ERP studies have demonstrated that automatic attention attraction by emotionally stimuli could reliably enhance the P1 amplitudes in the dot-probe task, which has been associated with rapid and unconscious bottom-up attention allocation (Luck and Kappenman, 2012), representing a facilitated perception in response to the emotional stimulus (Brosch et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2015). Eye tracking study in patients with depression have found a bias in the initial orientation to depression-related images (Sears et al., 2011). There is a lack of consensus on whether negative attentional bias occurs at early attention stages. It is demonstrated that patients with depression show a attentional bias toward congruent sadness at 100 ms, indicating that negative attention bias occurs in the early stage of attention (Trapp et al., 2018; Ao et al., 2020). While Cao found that the negative attention bias of patients with depression appeared after 6 s, suggesting that attention bias does not occur until the later stage of cognitive processing (Shuang et al., 2019). Therefore, we suggest that stimulus presentation time may be an influencing factor of emotional stimulus processing in patients with depression. But our study results are inconsistent with these results, probably owing to the stimulus material or material presentation time grouping. Therefore, these factors should be controlled and further discussed in the future.

In addition, attention bias mechanism has been suggested to mostly occur when processing social information stimuli. Faces showing emotions reflect the emotional state of others and thus contain more social information than emotional scene pictures and are more likely to attract the attention of patients with depression (Gilboa-Schechtman et al., 2004). However, there was no significant difference in the attentional bias toward emotional faces or scenes among the patients with depression included in this study. According to Leyman's research on attentional bias using forward-placed and inverted emotional faces, it is the emotional titer expressed by stimulus materials that causes individual attentional bias (Leyman et al., 2011) and when we looked back at the emotional scenes that were included in the study, we found that some of the emotional scene materials contained both characters and scenes, which is the same emotional valence as the emotional faces. Our results may support the idea that it is the emotional valence expressed by the stimulus material that causes attention bias in patients with depression. We suggest that future studies can compare the attention bias of depressed patients to emotional faces and emotional scenes to verify this view.

Eye movements are sensorimotor functions of the brain, and the effect of gender on attention patterns has been shown in autism spectrum disorders (Harrop et al., 2018). Classification of emotions based on EEG and eye movement signals also agree that emotion classification differs between male and female, and there are larger differences of brain activities between male and female for sad, which is closely associated with depression (Bao et al., 2019). Studies on attention bias in healthy adults also show that attention bias is influenced by gender and mainly manifests in the regulation function of negative stimulus avoidance (Pintzinger et al., 2016). Previous studies on attentional bias in depression have shown that sex differences in the subjects may lead to different initial attentional bias, and female patients with depression are more likely to have an initial attentional bias toward negative information (Sears et al., 2011; Li et al., 2016) and compared with men, women also have longer depressive episodes and are also presented higher levels of inflammatory, serotonergic markers, and neurotrophic (Oquendo et al., 2013; Labaka et al., 2018). Thus, it is important to consider gender as a biological variable in clinical depression studies, which may facilitate the development of more effective diagnostic methods and pharmacotherapies for depression.

Furthermore, the results of subgroup analysis of diagnostic methods showed that there was no significant difference in the negative attention bias of depressive patients diagnosed based on clinical diagnostic criteria and questionnaire, and we preliminarily believed that the diagnostic methods did not have much influence on the results. However, in order to ensure the reliability and extensibility of the research results, the inclusion criteria should be more strictly established in future studies.

4.3. Limitations

Several limitations must be considered before translating these results into clinical settings. First, we only analyzed the attention-processing characteristics of emotional stimuli in patients during the onset of depression. Thus, our study did not summarize these characteristics in the different stages of depression, such as first recurrence and disease duration, and did not comprehensively evaluate the value of this parameter for diagnosing depression. Second, only a limited number of studies was included in the meta-analysis for older patients with depression, thereby potentially reducing the reliability of its conclusions. To stimulate the clinical application of attention bias correction technology, future studies should observe and compare the attention-processing characteristics of patients with depression on emotional stimuli at different ages and stages and degrees of the disease, explore more indicators, such as saccade, and further evaluate the application value of eye movement indicators in diagnosing depression.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://drive.google.com/file/d/1d1_w1sqk78UIRZLj8xBVDikt0w6_AtgS/view?usp=share_link.

Author contributions

GH and YL convinced and designed this study. GH, YL, and HZ search and screen the literature. ZC and XS processed methodological analysis. GH and ZC processed data and drafted the manuscript. ZC and YL reviewed and modified the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was partially supported by the grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 82104737, 31960180, 31460251, and 32000736), the project of “1050 Young top-notch talent” of Jiangxi University of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Jiangxi University of Chinese Medicine Science and Technology Innovation Team Development Program (CXTD22016 and CXTD22014), Science and Technology Plan Project of Jiangxi Health Commission (202211428), PhD research startup foundation of Jiangxi University of Traditional Chinese Medicine (2020BSZR013 and 2018BSRW001), and Teaching Reform Project of Jiangxi University of Chinese Medicine (2021jzyb-32).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Akram, U., Ellis, J. G., Cau, G., Hershaw, F., and Drabble, J. (2020). Eye tracking and attentional bias for depressive internet memes in depression. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 575–581. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-06001-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akram, U., Ellis, J. G., Cau, G., Hershaw, F., Rajenthran, A., Lowe, M., et al. (2021). Eye tracking and attentional bias for depressive internet memes in depression. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 575–581.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Ao, X., Mo, L., Wei, Z., Yu, W., Zhou, F., Zhang, D., et al. (2020). Negative bias during early attentional engagement in major depressive disorder as examined using a two-stage model: high sensitivity to sad but bluntness to happy cues. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14, 593010. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.593010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, L. Q., Qiu, J. L., Tang, H., Zheng, W. L., and Lu, B. L. (2019). Investigating sex differences in classification of five emotions from EEG and eye movement signals. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 6746–6749. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2019.8857476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beurel, E., Toups, M., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2020). The bidirectional relationship of depression and inflammation: double trouble. Neuron 107, 234–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Laurent, E. (2015). Emotional information processing in depression and burnout: an eye-tracking study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 265, 27–34. doi: 10.1007/s00406-014-0549-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodner, N., Kuppens, P., Allen, N. B., Sheeber, L. B., and Ceulemans, E. (2018). Affective family interactions and their associations with adolescent depression: a dynamic network approach. Dev. Psychopathol. 30, 1459–1473. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley and Sons. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., and Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychol. Sci. 19, 362–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., and Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 676–691. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 312, 1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol. 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, X., Yue, X., Zheng, R., Bi, C., Li, D., Yao, G., et al. (2019). Classifying major depression patients and healthy controls using EEG, eye tracking and galvanic skin response data. J. Affect. Disord. 251, 156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A., and Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 467–477. doi: 10.1038/nrn3027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Disner, S. G., Shumake, J. D., and Beevers, C. G. (2017). Self-referential schemas and attentional bias predict severity and naturalistic course of depression symptoms. Cogn. Emot. 31, 632–644. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1146123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duque, A., and Vázquez, C. (2015). Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: evidence from an eye-tracking study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry.35, 517–528. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eizenman, M., Yu, L. H., Grupp, L., Eizenman, E., Ellenbogen, M., Gemar, M., et al. (2003). A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatr. Res. 118, 117–128.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Figueiredo, G. R., Ripka, W. L., Romaneli, E. F. R., and Ulbricht, L. (2019). Attentional bias for emotional faces in depressed and non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Ann. Int. Conf. 2019, 5419–5422. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2019.8857878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Ben-Artzi, E., Jeczemien, P., Marom, S., and Hermesh, H. (2004). Depression impairs the ability to ignore the emotional aspects of facial expressions: evidence from the Garner task. Cogn. Emot. 18, 209–231. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000176a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, P. W., and Kadriu, B. (2019). A major role for the lateral habenula in depressive illness: physiologic and molecular mechanisms. Front. Psychiatry 10, 320. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., and Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafton, B., Southworth, F., Watkins, E., and MacLeod, C. (2016). Stuck in a sad place: biased attentional disengagement in rumination. Emotion 16, 63–72. doi: 10.1037/emo0000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrop, C., Jones, D., Zheng, S., Nowell, S. W., Boyd, B. A., and Sasson, N. (2018). Sex differences in social attention in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 11, 1264–1275. doi: 10.1002/aur.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., and Eelen, P. (1999). Eye movement registration as a continuous index of attention deployment: data from a group of spider anxious students. Cogn. Emot. 13, 419–434. doi: 10.1080/026999399379249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaac, L., Vrijsen, J. N., Rinck, M., Speckens, A., and Becker, E. S. (2014). Shorter gaze duration for happy faces in current but not remitted depression: evidence from eye movements. Psychiatry Res. 218, 79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., and Wilson, H. R. (2006). Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? an eye-tracking study. Psychol. Aging 21, 40–48. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Z. C., Zhou, X. H., and He, J. (2015). Statistical methods for dealing with publication bias in meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 34, 343–360. doi: 10.1002/sim.6342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ke, T., Wu, J., Willner, C. J., Brown, Z., Banz, B., Van Noordt, S., et al. (2020). The glass is half empty: negative self-appraisal bias and attenuated neural response to positive self-judgment in adolescence. Soc. Neurosci. 15, 140–157. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1697744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., and Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: an eye tracking study. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1238–1243.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Klawohn, J., Bruchnak, A., Burani, K., Meyer, A., Lazarov, A., Bar-Haim, Y., et al. (2020). Aberrant attentional bias to sad faces in depression and the role of stressful life events: Evidence from an eye-tracking paradigm. Behav. Res. Ther. 135, 103762.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Koh, K. B., Kim, C. H., and Park, J. K. (2002). Predominance of anger in depressive disorders compared with anxiety disorders and somatoform disorders. J. Clin. Psychiatry 63, 486–492. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v63n0604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labaka, A., Goñi-Balentziaga, O., Lebeña, A., and Pérez-Tejada, J. (2018). Biological sex differences in depression: a systematic review. Biol. Res. Nurs. 20, 383–392. doi: 10.1177/1099800418776082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanza, C., Muller, C., and Riepe, M. W. (2018). Positive mood on negative self-statements: paradoxical intervention in geriatric patients with major depressive disorder. Aging Ment. Health. 22, 748–754.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lazarov, A., Ben-Zion, Z., Shamai, D., Pine, D. S., and Bar-Haim, Y. (2018). Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: a potential target for attention bias modification. J. Affect. Disord. 238, 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyman, L., de Raedt, R., Vaeyens, R., and Philippaerts, R. M. (2011). Attention for emotional facial expressions in dysphoria: an eye-movement registration study. Cogn. Emot. 25, 111–120. doi: 10.1080/02699931003593827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Lu, S., Wang, G., Feng, L., Fu, B., and Zhong, N. (2016). Alleviated negative rather than positive attentional bias in patients with depression in remission: an eye-tracking study. J. Int. Med. Res. 44, 1072–1086. doi: 10.1177/0300060516662134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Zhang, D., and Luo, Y. (2015). How disgust facilitates avoidance: an ERP study on attention modulation by threats. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 598–604. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, S., Xu, J., Li, M., Xue, J., Lu, X., Feng, L., et al. (2017). Attentional bias scores in patients with depression and effects of age: a controlled, eye-tracking study. J. Int. Med. Res. 45, 1518–1527. doi: 10.1177/0300060517708920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Kappenman, E. S. (2012). ERP components and selective attention. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components 295–327. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195374148.013.0144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, B., and Sweeney, J. A. (2004). The emergence of collaborative brain function - fMRI studies of the development of response inhibition. Adolescent Brain Development: Vulnerabilities And Opportunities 1021, 296–309. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol. Sci. 14, 409–415. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. J., Vaze, A., and Rao, S. (2009). Clinical diagnosis of depression in primary care: a meta-analysis. The Lancet 374, 609–619. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60879-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, K., Quigley, L., Fernandez, A., Dobson, K., and Sears, C. (2019). Concurrent and prospective relations between attentional biases for emotional images and relapse to depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 43, 893–909.

Google Scholar

Noiret, N., Carvalho, N., Laurent, E., Vulliez, L., Bennabi, D., Chopard, G., et al. (2015). Visual scanning behavior during processing of emotional faces in older adults with major depression. Aging Ment Health. 19, 264–273.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Oquendo, M. A., Turret, J., and Grunebaum, M. F. (2013). Sex differences in clinical predictors of depression:a prospective study. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 1179–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ou, C. H., and Hall, W. A. (2018). Anger in the context of postnatal depression: an integrative review. Birth 45, 336–346. doi: 10.1111/birt.12356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peckham, A. D., McHugh, R. K., and Otto, M. W. (2013). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depression and Anxiety 30, 407. doi: 10.1002/da.22092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlis, R. H., Fava, M., Trivedi, M. H., Alpert, J., Luther, J. F., Wisniewski, S. R., et al. (2009). Irritability is associated with anxiety and greater severity, but not bipolar spectrum features, in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 119, 282–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintzinger, N. M., Pfabigan, D. M., Tran, U. S., Kryspin-Exner, I., and Lamm, C. (2016). Attentional biases in healthy adults: exploring the impact of temperament and gender. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 52, 29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantanen, M., Hautala, J., Loberg, O., Nuorva, J., and Astikainen, P. (2021). Attentional bias towards interpersonal aggression in depression - an eye movement study. Scand. J. Psychol. 62, 639–647. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., and Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 627–639. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, P., Brunet-Gouet, E., Passerieux, C., and Ramus, F. (2016). Eye-tracking reveals a slowdown of social context processing during intention attribution in patients with schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41, E13–E21. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, J., Ordaz, S. J., Kircanski, K., Ho, T. C., Davis, E. G., Camacho, M. C., et al. (2019). Resting-state functional connectivity and inflexibility of daily emotions in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 249, 26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sears, C. R., Newman, K. R., Ference, J. D., and Thomas, C. L. (2011). Attention to emotional images in previously depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study. Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 517–528. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9396-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuang, C., Di, W., Chen, X., Yuqin, C., Xiaohui, W., and Danmin, M. (2019). Time course study of face recognition attentional bias in depression. Chin. J. Behav. Med. Brain Sci. 28, 1075–1095.

Google Scholar

Skinner, I. W., Hubscher, M., Moseley, G. L., Lee, H., Wand, B. M., Traeger, A. C., et al. (2018). The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 1778–1792. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0946-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stang, A. (2010). Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 25, 603–605. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suslow, T., Husslack, A., Kersting, A., and Bodenschatz, C. M. (2020). Attentional biases to emotional information in clinical depression: a systematic and meta-analytic review of eye tracking findings. J. Affect. Disord. 274, 632–642. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trapp, W., Kalzendorf, C., Baum, C., and Hajak, G. (2018). Lautenbacher S. Attentional biases in patients suffering from unipolar depression: results of a dot probe task investigation. Psychiatry Res. 261, 325–331. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rijsbergen, G. D., Hollon, S. D., Elgersma, H. J., Kok, G. D., Dekker, J., Schene, A. H., et al. (2015). Understanding emotion and emotional scarring in recurrent depression. Compr. Psychiatry 59, 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazquez, C., Blanco, I., Sanchez, A., and McNally, R. J. (2016). Attentional bias modification in depression through gaze contingencies and regulatory control using a new eye-tracking intervention paradigm: study protocol for a placebo-controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 16, 439. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1150-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Lyu, H. L., Tian, X. H., Lang, B., Wang, X. Y., St. Clair, D., et al. (2022). The similar eye movement dysfunction between major depressive disorder, bipolar depression and bipolar mania. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 23, 689–702. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2022.2025616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, T. T., Clerkin, E. M., Ellis, A. J., and Beevers, C. G. (2014). Effect of antidepressant medication use on emotional information processing in major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 195–200. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12091243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaroslavsky, I., Allard, E. S., and Sanchez-Lopez, A. (2019). Can't look away: attention control deficits predict rumination, depression symptoms and depressive affect in daily life. J. Affect. Disord. 245, 1061–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Yu, F., Hu, Q., Qiao, Y., Xuan, R., Ji, G., et al. (2020). Effects of SSRI antidepressants on attentional bias toward emotional scenes in first-episode depressive patients: Evidence from an eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Investig. 17, 871–879.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, eye tracking, emotional stimuli, meta-analysis, attention bias

Citation: Huang G, Li Y, Zhu H, Feng H, Shen X and Chen Z (2023) Emotional stimulation processing characteristics in depression: Meta-analysis of eye tracking findings. Front. Psychol. 13:1089654. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1089654

Received: 07 November 2022; Accepted: 28 December 2022;
Published: 13 January 2023.

Edited by:

Jianjun Wang, Shenzhen Traditional Chinese Medicine Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Xuejun Bai, Tianjin Normal University, China
Maorong Hu, Nanchang University, China

Copyright © 2023 Huang, Li, Zhu, Feng, Shen and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yafang Li, yes 1159674822@qq.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.