- 1Department of Psychology, University of Campania ‘Luigi Vanvitelli', Caserta, Italy
- 2CIMeC—Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy
- 3Behavioral Neurology Unit, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, MD, United States
- 4Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, United States
Editorial on the Research Topic
Uncovering the neural correlates of prism adaptation: Evidence from the brain network approach
Since early applications of Prism Adaptation (PA) in the rehabilitation of unilateral spatial neglect (Rossetti et al., 1998; Frassinetti et al., 2002) a plethora of studies on brain-damaged and healthy individuals revealed that PA can exert supramodal effects on high-order cognition, such as spatial attention, time perception, auditory perception, and reward-based learning (e.g., Schintu et al., 2018; Anelli and Frassinetti, 2019; Michel et al., 2019). These results prompted further research aimed at investigating the neural correlates of PA-induced sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects (Panico et al., 2020, 2021; Schintu et al., 2020, 2022), and understanding their temporal dynamics. The role of different variables, such as the state of the system at baseline, the hand used during adaptation and the direction of the prismatic displacement (Schintu et al., 2017; McIntosh et al., 2019) has been largely investigated, but there are still open questions about the processes of generalization, transfer, expansion, and consolidation taking place during and after PA (Prablanc et al., 2020).
PA modulates cognition according to the direction of the visual displacement, which surprisingly interacts with the system (intact vs. damaged) that is being adapted. It is well-known that right-deviating PA can ameliorate neglect symptoms, whereas left PA is ineffective. However, left PA produces relevant cognitive changes in healthy individuals, whereas right PA does not. The review paper by Clarke et al. tackled this long-standing conundrum and proposed a model accounting for such asymmetry in PA-induced effects. The model posits that PA induces instability in the synaptic organization in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) contralateral to the optical deviation, consequently affecting the ventral attentional network (VAN). According to the model, right PA may produce its beneficial effects in right hemisphere stroke patients with neglect by reshuffling the left PPC representations, thus compensating for the functions of the damaged right VAN. Differently, left PA would exert no effect in these patients since the reshuffling would occur in the lesioned right PPC. In healthy individuals, right PA would not produce any effect as healthy individuals may continue to rely on their right (intact) VAN, not taking advantage of the reshuffled left PPC. In contrast, left PA would determine a reshuffling in the right PPC, thus accounting for neglect-like behavior.
Another long-standing question in this field is how sensorimotor adaptation to laterally displaced vision can prompt changes in cognition. In their fMRI study, Schintu et al. used structural equation modeling (SEM), a form of effective connectivity analysis, to uncover connectivity directionality and causality among the network underpinning PA (Schintu et al., 2020). These findings provided the first direct evidence that PA is underpinned by two functionally distinct subnetworks, a parietal–temporal network responsible for sensorimotor aftereffects and a fronto-cerebellar network responsible for cognitive aftereffects, consistent with the model recently put forward by Panico et al. (2020) positing a distinction between networks responsible for PA sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects.
While the network underlying PA is still object of discussion, there is general agreement on the core role of the cerebellum during the different stages of the sensorimotor adaptation: from error correction to aftereffect development. Fleury et al. using anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation assessed the role of the cerebellum in the transfer of prism aftereffects from a throwing task to a pointing task as quantified by behavioral and kinematic measures (Fleury et al., 2021). Although the overall magnitude of transfer was not modified by the stimulation, the results showed that facilitatory stimulation affected the evolution of pointing errors during transfer assessment. Moreover, the inspection of kinematics parameters during transfer assessment suggested a lasting trace of adaptation related to transfer.
Concerning the variables affecting PA-induced aftereffect, in a fMRI study on healthy participants Farron et al. assessed whether the hand (right vs. left) used during right PA affected the process of visuomotor adaptation and, therefore, the reshaping of the attentional system. The authors compared brain activation patterns to visual targets in left, central, and right visual field before and after PA. They observed that left and central targets enhanced activation in the left PPC following right PA, and that this effect was modulated by the hand used during adaptation. This study clearly showed that the hand used during adaptation can affect the degree to which right PA modulates the functioning of the VAN. By using a behavioral approach, Bonnet et al. instead explored the effects induced by vertical PA at both sensorimotor and cognitive levels. Bonnet et al.'s experiments showed that, unlike lateral PA, the aftereffects of vertical PA also occurred in the auditory domain, but not in vertical visuospatial representations. These findings suggested that both vertical and lateral PA share a common substrate in the auditory modality, and that vertical PA does not act on the neural substrate of vertical visuospatial representations.
Whether PA could be implemented in a virtual reality environment and how sensory parameters (verbal vs. visual) could influence PA-induced aftereffect was investigated by Bourgeois et al. The authors compared the effect of visual vs. auditory-verbal feedback during the adaptation phase on visuospatial judgments implementing a simulated prism exposure in immersed virtual reality. Results confirmed that a virtual PA procedure can induce PA aftereffects, and showed that only the visual, but not the auditory-verbal feedback produces aftereffects following adaptation to virtual prisms, pointing out that the effects of sensorimotor adaptation operate in a visually aligned coordinate system.
The goal of this Research Topic was to provide evidence on the PA neural correlates and on its mechanisms of action from a network approach. According to this approach, the adaptive processes of sensorimotor adaptation to prims and their consequent expansion to cognition arise from the tight interaction between functionally interconnected brain regions. Taken together the studies presented in this Research Topic confirmed from different perspectives that PA is a deceptively simple technique. The PA-induced sensorimotor and cognitive effects are subtended by the dynamic interplay of likely distinct frontal-parietal-cerebellar networks, whose activation can be modulated by several variables related to the adaptation procedures.
Author contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.
Funding
FP was funded by PON Green/Innovation 2014-2020 (DM 1062, 10/08/2021).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Anelli, F., and Frassinetti, F. (2019). Prisms for timing better: a review on application of prism adaptation on temporal domain. Cortex 119, 583–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.017
Fleury, L., Panico, F., Foncelle, A., Revol, P., Delporte, L., Jacquin-Courtois, S., et al. (2021). Non-invasive brain stimulation shows possible cerebellar contribution in transfer of prism adaptation after-effects from pointing to throwing movements. Brain Cogn. 151, 105735. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105735
Frassinetti, F., Angeli, V., Meneghello, F., Avanzi, S., and Làdavas, E. (2002). Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain 125, 608–623. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf056
McIntosh, R. D., Brown, B. M. A., and Young, L. (2019). Meta-analysis of the visuospatial aftereffects of prism adaptation, with two novel experiments. Cortex 111, 256–273. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.013
Michel, C., Bonnet, C., Podor, B., Bard, P., and Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2019). Wearing prisms to hear differently: after-effects of prism adaptation on auditory perception. Cortex 115, 123–132. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.015
Panico, F., Fleury, L., Trojano, L., and Rossetti, Y. (2021). Prism adaptation in M1. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 563–573. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01668
Panico, F., Rossetti, Y., and Trojano, L. (2020). On the mechanisms underlying Prism Adaptation: a review of neuro-imaging and neuro-stimulation studies. Cortex 123, 57–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.003
Prablanc, C., Panico, F., Fleury, L., Pisella, L., Nijboer, T., Kitazawa, S., et al. (2020). Adapting terminology: clarifying prism adaptation vocabulary, concepts, and methods. Neurosci. Res. 153, 8–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2019.03.003
Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Farné, A., Li, L., Boisson, D., et al. (1998). Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature 395, 166–169. doi: 10.1038/25988
Schintu, S., Freedberg, M., Alam, Z. M., Shomstein, S., and Wassermann, E. M. (2018). Left-shifting prism adaptation boosts reward-based learning. Cortex 109, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.09.021
Schintu, S., Freedberg, M., Gotts, S. J., Cunningham, C. A., Alam, Z. M., Shomstein, S., et al. (2020). Prism adaptation modulates connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus with multiple brain networks. Cerebr. Cortex 30, 4747. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa032
Schintu, S., Kravitz, D. J., Silson, E. H., Cunningham, C. A., Wassermann, E. M., and Shomstein, S. (2022). Dynamic changes in spatial representation within the posterior parietal cortex in response to visuomotor adaptation. Cerebr. Cortex 2022, bhac298. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac298
Keywords: prism adaptation, sensorimotor plasticity, spatial attention, cognition, brain networks, dynamic connectivity, neglect
Citation: Panico F, Schintu S and Trojano L (2022) Editorial: Uncovering the neural correlates of prism adaptation: Evidence from the brain network approach. Front. Psychol. 13:1076307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1076307
Received: 21 October 2022; Accepted: 25 October 2022;
Published: 15 November 2022.
Edited and reviewed by: Bernhard Hommel, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Germany
Copyright © 2022 Panico, Schintu and Trojano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Francesco Panico, ZnJhbmNlc2NvLnBhbmljbyYjeDAwMDQwO3VuaWNhbXBhbmlhLml0