Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 December 2022
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities

The gender gap in political psychology

  • Swedish School of Social Science, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

Introduction: I investigated the authorship gender gap in research on political psychology.

Methods: The material comprises 1,166 articles published in the field’s flagship journal Political Psychology between 1997 and 2021. These were rated for author gender, methodology, purpose, and topic.

Results: Women were underrepresented as authors (37.1% women), single authors (33.5% women), and lead authors (35.1% women). There were disproportionately many women lead authors in papers employing interviews or qualitative methodology, and in research with an applied purpose (these were all less cited). In contrast, men were overrepresented as authors of papers employing quantitative methods. Regarding topics, women were overrepresented as authors on Gender, Identity, Culture and Language, and Religion, and men were overrepresented as authors on Neuroscience and Evolutionary Psychology.

Discussion: The (denigrated) methods, purposes, and topics of women doing research on politics correspond to the (denigrated) “feminine style” of women doing politics grounding knowledge in the concrete, lived reality of others; listening and giving voice to marginalized groups’ subjective experiences; and yielding power to get things done for others.

Introduction

Women are not equally represented in science in terms of publications and impact. This is especially true in male-dominated fields such as science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), in which women are severely underrepresented both in terms of raw numbers and in terms of prestige (Larivière et al., 2013; West et al., 2013). Moreover, a large authorship gender gap in publishing can be found not only in fields dominated by men but also in fields that approach or have reached gender parity in participation, such as education or psychology (West et al., 2013).

Psychology is a discipline that is often lauded by researchers who study gender gaps in academia due to the high rate of women participating in the field, from undergraduates to tenure-track professors (e.g., Ceci et al., 2014). An increasing number of women with master’s degrees and doctorates have transformed psychology from a men-dominated field to a women-dominated field. Despite this representation in numbers, women in psychology remain underrepresented as authors and in eminence—women in psychology are underrepresented in first-author publications in top journals (Brown and Goh, 2016), in the citations their work receives (Odic and Wojcik, 2020), in award received by divisions (Eagly and Riger, 2014; Brown and Goh, 2016), and in eminence (Eagly and Miller, 2016). Although political science is strongly dominated by men (Tolleson-Rinehart and Carroll, 2006; Breuning and Sanders, 2007; Teele and Thelen, 2017; Thelwall et al., 2019), this may be less true in the subfield of political psychology.

This research aimed to assess whether there is an authorship gender gap in articles published in Political Psychology. The journal Political Psychology incorporates contributions with a variety of methodologies and topics, as well as both theoretical research and applied research. Exploring how the authorship gender gap varies between different types of contributions could reveal how women are situated within the field. First off, the size of the authorship gender gap may vary as a function of method—women generally tend to employ more qualitative research methods than men (Eagly and Riger, 2014; Thelwall et al., 2019). For instance, in political science, journals that publish qualitative research tend to have more female authors than journals that publish only quantitative research (Teele and Thelen, 2017). Second, the purpose of the paper may be associated with the size of the authorship gender gap. Large-scale bibliometric analyses across all fields of research show that theoretical papers are more typical of men, whereas women are overrepresented as authors in papers intended to have a social impact (Thelwall et al., 2019). Third, authorship gender gaps may vary across topics. For instance, gender studies are one of the few social sciences fields dominated by women (Kretschmer et al., 2012), suggesting that women might not be underrepresented as authors of research on this topic.

The purpose of the present research was to investigate the possible gender gap within the field of political psychology or, more specifically, the field’s flagship journal Political Psychology. I expected women to be generally underrepresented as authors, but the size of the authorship gender gap varies according to the methods, purposes, and topics of the paper. Moreover, I also investigated whether the authorship gender gap has narrowed in the last decades (as suggested by Brown and Goh, 2016) and whether papers authored by women are cited less (as suggested by Odic and Wojcik, 2020). Pertinent to the last point, I also explored whether the number of citations varies with method, purpose, or topic. Some previous studies, run on articles published in the journal Leadership Quarterly, suggest that quantitative, review, methods, and theory articles may be cited more than qualitative articles (Antonakis et al., 2014). Women could thus be expected to be cited less, and this could at least in part be explained by different citation rates for different types of papers.

Materials and methods

Two doctoral students, both of whom identified as women, were employed as research assistants, and they rated all articles that appeared in the journal Political Psychology from the start of 1997 to the end of 2021. No power calculations were made—we sought to include all articles that were available online at the time the data were collected. Between the beginning of 1997 and the end of 2021, the journal published 1,166 research articles, which constituted the material for the present study.

All articles were rated by both raters for author gender, method, purpose, and topic. Each article was rated dichotomously on all employed variables, i.e., either employing a certain method vs. not doing so, having vs. not having applied relevance, or dealing vs. not dealing with a certain topic. The rating scheme and rating criteria were discussed, developed, and revised together in the initial stages of rating, and the final rating scheme that was employed for all articles was arrived at through joint discussion after both research assistants had rated around a hundred articles. After these initial discussions, the research assistants did not discuss their ratings with each other.

The coding of methods was rather straightforward, either a given method was employed or it was not. For the article to be coded as having applied relevance, a real-world problem had to motivate a research question, or the applied relevance had to be mentioned in the abstract. Regarding the topic, the focus was on the framework within which the research was conducted. For instance, speculating upon an “identity” or “evolutionary psychology” explanation of the results did not suffice for the paper to be coded as being within these fields. In addition to discussions with the two research assistants, I relied on political psychology handbooks and research on the submissions to the journal Political Psychology (Mintz and Mograbi, 2015) in developing the rating scheme.

Results

There were generally more men (n = 1681) than women (n = 991) as authors (37.1% women). Looking at the first author or single author, there were again more men (n = 757) than women (n = 409; 35.1% women), and the same was true when looking only at single authors (250 men, 126 women; 33.5% women). Women were thus similarly underrepresented both as co-authors and as lead authors.

The gender gap in lead authorship over time is plotted in Figure 1. There is no indication that the gender gap in authorship would have decreased over time [the linear correlation between the year of publication and the percentage of female lead authors was r(25) = 0.02]. However, the slight dip in the percentage of female lead authors suggested investigating gender representation among the editors of the journal. Between 1997 and 2021, there were two to four (co-)editors each year. The editors were all male from the beginning of 2006 to the end of 2011. All other years, there was one woman among the (co-)editors. Between 2006 and 2011, the all-male years, only 29.7% of lead authors were women. In contrast, the average of those years in which one woman was included as (co-)editor, the percentage of women lead authors was 36.6% [this difference in percentages did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance, F(1, 24) = 3.25, p = 0.084].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The percentage of female first authors over time.

To analyze differences between topics, methods, and purposes, one rater’s rating of the gender of the lead author (first author or single author) was used as the independent variable (rater’s agreement on author gender was 99.5%, and kappa was 0.989).

Tables 13 show the number of male and female lead authors by method, purpose, and topic, respectively. Not surprisingly, given that the proportion of female lead authors was only 0.351, there were fewer women than men in almost all categories. However, there were some exceptions. Regarding methods, there were as many women as men employing qualitative methods and interviews (the number of women and men lead authors employing these methods did not differ statistically significantly at p < 0.05). Regarding the topic, there were as many women as men lead authors writing about Gender, Religion, Social Control, and Class (the number of women and men lead authors writing on these topics did not differ statistically significantly at p < 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of papers with male and female lead authors across methods.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Number of papers with male and female lead authors across purpose.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Number of papers with male and female lead authors across theme.

Given the generally large authorship gender gap in favor of men, more informative than analyzing the absolute numbers of men and women as lead authors were to look at the relative number of men and women. Chi-square tests were run to investigate, given the general overrepresentation of men, whether the authorship gender gap varied across methods (Table 1), purposes (Table 2), and topics (Table 3). Regarding methods, there were disproportionately many women lead authors in papers employing qualitative methods or interviews. In contrast, men were overrepresented as authors of papers employing quantitative methods. There were also disproportionately many women lead authors in research with an applied purpose. Regarding the topic, there were disproportionately many women lead authors within the fields of Gender, Identity, Culture and Language, and Religion. There were some indications that this might also be true for Ethnicity and Political Participation, although conventional levels of statistical significance were not reached. On the contrary, there were disproportionately many men as lead authors in papers on Evolutionary Psychology and Neuroscience.

To alleviate fears that the above results were being driven by only some variables, I analyzed the associations between variables: methods, purposes, and topics. The strongest association was between qualitative methods and the topic of Culture and Language, with an effect size of φ = 0.252 [χ2(1, 1066) = 74.784]. Entering these two variables into a binary logistic regression predicting author gender gave very similar coefficient estimates regardless of whether the two variables were entered alone or simultaneously (all coefficient p-values in both types of models were statistically significant at p < 0.01). This suggests that the associations between, on the one hand, methods, purposes, and topic and, on the other hand, gender were generally rather independent of each other.

Regarding the number of citations, there was no gender bias. Male lead authors had an average of 102 (SD = 192) citations, and female first authors had an average of 93 [SD = 166; F(1, 1164) = 0.722, ns]. Of the 50 most-cited articles, 18 (36%) had a female lead author, which is almost exactly what one would expect given that 37% of all lead authors were women. Similarly, when looking at the top 10 most-cited articles, three of them had a female lead author.

Although articles authored by women were, contrary to expectations, not cited less frequently, the types of papers for which women were overrepresented as authors were cited less frequently. As expected, papers employing qualitative methods (Table 4) and having an applied focus (Table 5) were cited less often. However, the topics that women authors were overrepresented on were not cited particularly poorly, with the exception of Culture and Language (Table 6).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean number of citations across employed methods.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mean number of citations across purposes.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Mean number of citations across themes.

Discussion

The first finding was that only 37.1% of the authors who published their articles in Political Psychology in 1997–2021 were women (33.5% single authors, 35.1% first authors). The average for the top 200 psychology journals from 2003 to 2018 was 44.2% (Odic and Wojcik, 2020). The most important finding for a wider audience was that the authorship gender gap was not identical across various methodological approaches, research purposes, and research topics—i.e., women scholars were different in how (methods), why (purpose), and what (theme) they did. This result was especially intriguing given that the journal investigates politics. Politics is of course not gender-neutral—women politicians are expected to be different in how, why, and what they do. One of the questions the results provoke is whether research on politics is gendered similarly to how politics is gendered. That is, gender differences in what political psychologist do may be parallel to gender differences in what politicians do.

Regarding methodology, women were, relative to the general authorship gender gap in favor of men, overrepresented as lead authors in research employing qualitative methods and interviews. Women were also overrepresented in research with an applied purpose. These results are consistent with previous large-scale cross-disciplinary bibliometric results (Thelwall et al., 2019). The similarities between how and why women do research and how and why women do politics are striking. A “feminine style” in women’s political discourse has been argued to consist, among other things, of basing political judgments on concrete, lived experience; valuing inclusivity and the relational nature of being; conceptualizing the power of public office as a capacity to “get things done”; and moving women’s issues to the forefront of the public arena (Blankenship and Robson, 1995). This “feminine style” ascribed to women politicians could just as well describe the results for women scholars. Several approaches within qualitative research, especially when interviews are employed, have been thought of as seeking to capture “lived experience” (Frechette et al., 2020), legitimize the subjectivity of human reality (Morgan and Drury, 2003), or “give voice” to those who are rarely heard (Larkin et al., 2006). Women in politics and women scholars seem to ground their knowledge in the concrete, lived reality of others (their electorate or their research participants), listening and giving voice to marginalized groups’ subjective experiences, and yielding their own power or their voice for the sake of others. This is all very consistent with the broader stereotype of women as highly communal (i.e., kind, warm, empathetic, and caring) and less agentic (i.e., analytical, independent, and competitive) than men (Fiske et al., 2002).

In addition, consistent with these broader stereotypes are the topics, on which women were, given the general authorship gender gap in favor of men, overrepresented: Gender, Identity, Culture and Language, and Religion. Two topics that narrowly missed the cut-off points for statistical significance were Political participation and Ethnicity. It is again rather striking that the topics women scholars work on are the very same type of topics that women politicians are associated with. In electoral politics, even though women’s overall proportions in the governments of developed democracies have increased considerably over the last few decades, women cabinet ministers in charge of the most prestigious (i.e., pivotal, resourceful, and visible) positions remain an exception (Krook and O’Brien, 2012; Kroeber and Hüffelmann, 2021). Women members of government typically preside over low-prestige portfolios, such as Women, Equality, Minority Affairs, Culture, Minority Affairs, and Immigration (Kroeber and Hüffelmann, 2021). These portfolios are very similar in content to the above topics on which women authors were overrepresented.

Besides affinities in the methods, purposes, and topics, of women scholars and politicians, there are similarities in the prestige that they are afforded. Regarding research methods, policymakers are infatuated with the number and quantitative methods, particularly at the intersection of science and bureaucracy (Porter, 1995). Also in the present study, qualitative research was poorly cited. This was consistent with previous research on the citation rates of leadership research (Antonakis et al., 2014), as well as with the present result that papers with a more applied focus were poorly cited. That applied papers, like qualitative papers, may lack prestige can be inferred not only from citation rates but also from a recent Association for Psychological Science presidential column, in which it was noted that “Many of us in academia may be walking around with an implicit or explicit ‘basic is better’ attitude” (Medin, 2012). Thus, although women were not per se less likely to be cited than men, the type of research that women did was less likely to be cited.

Regarding the topics of the research, Gender, Identity, Culture and Language, and Religion, also maybe Ethnicity and Political Identity, are all topics that in eyes of both other psychologists and laypeople are perceived as less rigorous, mainstream, and objective than research in areas of psychology, and the researchers engaged with this type of research on identity or “me-search” (Rios and Roth, 2020) are seen as more subjective and less intelligent (Rios and Roth, 2020; Brown et al., 2022).

Both women politicians and scholars appear to focus on similar issues, and these issues are denigrated. In retrospect, this is perhaps not that surprising. Just as “intelligence” in political leadership is associated with masculinity (for a meta-analysis, see Koenig et al., 2011), so is being a “rational” and an “objective” scientist (Carli et al., 2016). These parallels offer intriguing questions for future research. For instance—given that female politicians can increase their chances of electoral success by focusing on “soft” issues in the election campaign (Herrnson et al., 2003)—one can ask whether papers authored by women are more likely to be accepted if they focus on “soft” topics, are qualitative, or are applied. Another instance, as in politics, in which men can capture “soft issues” (Herrnson et al., 2003), are qualitative papers or papers on “women’s topics” more likely to be published when authored by men? On a very general level, it would be interesting to investigate whether “women’s topics” are considered less scientific because women focus on these topics, or whether women focus on these topics because they are rejected when venturing into areas perceived as requiring more masculine characteristics, such as “rationality” and “objectivity” (Carli et al., 2016).

Those men were overrepresented in research employing quantitative methods and in research rooted in Neuroscience and Evolutionary Psychology also aligns well with the above-discussed gender stereotypes. Although the number of papers on these topics was rather small, the notion that at least Evolutionary Psychology (e.g., Meredith, 2013) is dominated by men aligns well with previous research. In terms of future research, our results regarding the overrepresentation of men do offer some intriguing suggestions. Both Neuroscience and Evolutionary Psychology can be argued to take a rather essentialist approach. For instance, in a much-cited paper, DeLamater and Hyde (1998) argued that modern essentialism, within which they count both Evolutionary Psychology and Brain Research, consists of a belief that certain phenomena are natural, inevitable, and biologically determined. They contrast this belief with that of social constructionism, the belief that reality is socially constructed, which emphasizes language as an important means by which we interpret experience. Essentialist claims about inevitable differences with regard to sex and gender have been argued to be incompatible with feminism (e.g., Kelly, 2014), and the controversies surrounding this topic have come to reach larger audiences (e.g., Hufendiek, 2022). This could in part explain why women do not venture into working on these themes. Interesting questions could be whether men are disproportionately overrepresented as authors in other psychological or social sciences research that takes an essentialist approach to sex or more broadly an essentialist approach to individual differences.

An important takeaway for future research is that merely looking at the numerical representation of women is not enough. Probing deeper by looking at authorship gender gaps as a function of the topics, methods, and purposes of the research can shed light on the processes underlying these gaps. To what extent can research also within other disciplines be described as women researching (possibly derided) women’s issues and, in doing so, employing certain (possibly derided) methods?

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Antonakis, J., Bastardoz, N., Liu, Y., and Schriesheim, C. A. (2014). What makes articles highly cited? Leadersh. Q. 25, 152–179. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blankenship, J., and Robson, D. C. (1995). A “feminine style”; in women’s political discourse: An exploratory essay. Commun. Q. 43, 353–366. doi: 10.1080/01463379509369982

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breuning, M., and Sanders, K. (2007). Gender and journal authorship in eight prestigious political science journals. PS Polit. Sci. Polit. 40, 347–351. doi: 10.1017/S1049096507070564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. J., and Goh, J. X. (2016). Some evidence for a gender gap in personality and social psychology. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 7, 437–443. doi: 10.1177/1948550616644297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, E. R., Smith, J. L., and Rossmann, D. (2022). “Broad” impact: Perceptions of sex/gender-related psychology journals. Front. Psychol. 13:796069. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.796069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carli, L. L., Alawa, L., Lee, Y., Zhao, B., and Kim, E. (2016). Stereotypes about gender and science: Women≠ scientists. Psychol. Women Q. 40, 244–260. doi: 10.1177/0361684315622645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceci, S. J., Ginther, D. K., Kahn, S., and Williams, W. M. (2014). Women in academic science: A changing landscape. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 15, 75–141. doi: 10.1177/1529100614541236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLamater, J. D., and Hyde, J. S. (1998). Essentialism vs. social constructionism in the study of human sexuality. J. Sex Res. 35, 10–18. doi: 10.1080/00224499809551913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Miller, D. I. (2016). Scientific eminence: Where are the women? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 899–904. doi: 10.1177/1745691616663918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Riger, S. (2014). Feminism and psychology: Critiques of methods and epistemology. Am. Psychol. 69, 685–702. doi: 10.1037/a0037372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902.

Google Scholar

Frechette, J., Bitzas, V., Aubry, M., Kilpatrick, K., and Lavoie-Tremblay, M. (2020). Capturing lived experiences: Methodological considerations for interpretive phenomenological inquiry. Int. J. Qual. Methods 19:160940692090725. doi: 10.1177/1609406920907254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrnson, P. S., Lay, J. C., and Stokes, A. K. (2003). Women running “as women”: Candidate gender, campaign issues, and voter-targeting strategies. J. Polit. 65, 244–255. doi: 10.1111/1468-2508.t01-1-00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hufendiek, R. (2022). Beyond essentialist fallacies: Fine-tuning ideology critique of appeals to biological sex differences. J. Soc. Philos. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1111/josp.12389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, S. (2014). Tofu feminism: Can feminist theory absorb evolutionary psychology? Dialect. Anthropol. 38, 287–304. doi: 10.1007/s10624-014-9353-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., and Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychol. Bull. 137, 616–642. doi: 10.1037/a0023557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kretschmer, H., Kundra, R., Beaver, D., and Kretschmer, T. (2012). Gender bias in journals of gender studies. Scientometrics 93, 135–150. doi: 10.1007/s11192-012-0661-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroeber, C., and Hüffelmann, J. (2021). It’s a long way to the top: Women’s ministerial career paths. Polit. Gend. 18, 1–27. doi: 10.1017/S1743923X21000118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krook, M. L., and O’Brien, D. Z. (2012). All the president’s men? The appointment of female cabinet ministers worldwide. J. Polit. 74, 840–855. doi: 10.1017/S0022381612000382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B., and Sugimoto, C. R. (2013). Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science. Nature 504, 211–213. doi: 10.1038/504211a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larkin, M., Watts, S., and Clifton, E. (2006). Giving voice and making sense in interpretative phenomenological analysis. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 102–120. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp062oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medin, D. L. (2012). A dangerous dichotomy: Basic and applied research. APS Observer, 25. Available online at: https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/a-dangerous-dichotomy-basic-and-applied-research (accessed November 30, 2022).

Google Scholar

Meredith, T. (2013). A journal of one’s own. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 7, 354–360. doi: 10.1037/h009918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mintz, A., and Mograbi, E. (2015). Political Psychology: Submissions, acceptances, downloads, and citations. Polit. Psychol. 36, 267–275. doi: 10.1111/pops.12277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, A. K., and Drury, V. B. (2003). Legitimising the subjectivity of human reality through qualitative research method. Qual. Rep. 8, 70–80. doi: 10.46743/2160-3715/2003.1899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odic, D., and Wojcik, E. H. (2020). The publication gender gap in psychology. Am. Psychol. 75, 92–103. doi: 10.1037/amp0000480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, T. (1995). Trust in numbers. The pursuit of objectivity in science and public life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9780691210544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rios, K., and Roth, Z. C. (2020). Is “me-search” necessarily less rigorous research? Social and personality psychologists’ stereotypes of the psychology of religion. Self Identity 19, 825–840. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2019.1690035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teele, D. L., and Thelen, K. (2017). Gender in the journals: Publication patterns in political science. PS Polit. Sci. Polit. 50, 433–447. doi: 10.1017/S1049096516002985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelwall, M., Bailey, C., Tobin, C., and Bradshaw, N. A. (2019). Gender differences in research areas, methods and topics: Can people and thing orientations explain the results? J. Informetr. 13, 149–169. doi: 10.1016/j.joi.2018.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolleson-Rinehart, S., and Carroll, S. J. (2006). Far from ideal:” The gender politics of political science. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 100, 507–513. doi: 10.1017/S0003055406062368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, J. D., Jacquet, J., King, M. M., Correll, S. J., and Bergstrom, C. T. (2013). The role of gender in scholarly authorship. PLoS One 8:e66212. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gender gap, research profession, political psychology, authorship, gender differences

Citation: Lönnqvist J-E (2022) The gender gap in political psychology. Front. Psychol. 13:1072494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1072494

Received: 17 October 2022; Accepted: 15 November 2022;
Published: 13 December 2022.

Edited by:

Marta Evelia Aparicio-Garcįa, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain

Reviewed by:

David Stuart Smith, Robert Gordon University, United Kingdom
Cynthia Whissell, Laurentian University, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Lönnqvist. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jan-Erik Lönnqvist, jan-erik.lonnqvist@helsinki.fi

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.