Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Psychol., 09 January 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Multilingualism and Neurodevelopmental Disorders View all 5 articles

Multi-domain computerized cognitive training for children with intellectual developmental disorder: A randomized controlled trial

Jingsong Wu&#x;Jingsong WuJuan Peng&#x;Juan PengZhaoying Li&#x;Zhaoying LiHaiyin Deng&#x;Haiyin DengZhenming HuangZhenming HuangYouze HeYouze HeJingnan TuJingnan TuLei CaoLei CaoJia Huang
Jia Huang*
  • College of Rehabilitation Medicine, Fujian University of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Fuzhou, Fujian Province, China

Purpose: To verify the effects of multi-domain computerized cognitive training on intellectual function and adaptive functioning in children with intellectual developmental disorder (IDD).

Methods: Children with IDD were randomized to a multi-domain computerized cognitive training (CCT) group (n = 30) and control group (n = 30). Both groups received a 5-week training program. Intellectual function was assessed by Chinese-Wechsler Young Children scale (C-WYCSI) and adaptive functioning was assessed by the Chinese Vineland Adaptive Behavior Rating Scale (VABS-C), which were used at baseline, post-training, and 3-month follow-up.

Results: There were significant differences for intellectual function and adaptive functioning between the two groups. The CCT group showed significant improvements in total full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) score the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (F[60] = 31.97, p < 0.01) and its subdomain VIQ score (F[60] = 33.83, p < 0.01). For adaptive functioning, CCT had a better adaptive developmental quotient (ADQ) score (F[60] = 28.05, p < 0.01), and subdomain communication (F[60] = 10.86, p < 0.01) and socialization scores (F[60] = 4.35, p < 0.015). Moreover, there was a positive correlation between FSIQ changes and ADQ changes in the CCT group (rs = 0.74, p < 0.01). A greater increase in VIQ score was associated with a greater increase in adaptive functioning (bootstrapping CI: [0.16, 3.30]) in the CCT group.

Conclusion: Multi-domain CCT improves the intellectual function and adaptive functioning of children with IDD.

1. Introduction

Intellectual developmental disorder (IDD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by substantial limitations in intellectual function and adaptive functioning (American Psychiatric Association and American Psychiatric Association, 2013), with a worldwide prevalence of approximately 1%–3% (McKenzie et al., 2016). Intelligence measures general cognitive functions (Deary, 2012), including visual perception (Wan et al., 2017; Cui et al., 2019), attention (Kirk et al., 2016; Kirk H. E. et al., 2017), memory (Söderqvist et al., 2012; Purugganan, 2018), and reasoning (Hilger et al., 2017; Ma et al., 2017). Impairment of intellectual function implies impairment of cognitive domains (Yang et al., 2020), which may result in poor participation in daily activities, learning disabilities, and social difficulties in children, and restriction of overall development (Kirk et al., 2016; Schwartz et al., 2016; Ke et al., 2020).

In recent years, computerized cognitive training (CCT) has gradually emerged as a new treatment technology to improve neurodevelopmental disorders (Chen et al., 2013). Numerous studies have shown that CCT is effective for IDD (Söderqvist et al., 2012; Henrik et al., 2015; Kirk et al., 2016; Kirk H. E. et al., 2017). Studies have also shown that children with IDD prefer computer technology in leisure and learning activities (Hartin et al., 2016). Using CCT could target impaired cognitive domains by providing timely feedback from multi-sensory channels such as those of sight, hearing, and touch, which could improve the performance, learning initiative, and motivation of these children (Serret et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2017; Denervaud et al., 2020). Furthermore, studies have proven that cognitive functions, such as attention, can be positively influenced via targeted training (Henrik et al., 2015; Kirk et al., 2016). However, these gains are generally restricted by the training domain, and the generalization effect is limited due to the untrained tasks (Cui et al., 2019b).

Multi-domain cognitive training may be more helpful to children with developmental disorders (Kirk H. et al., 2017). According to the characteristics of cognitive impairment in children with IDD, designing a comprehensive multi-domain CCT training program may be more conducive to the recovery of children with IDD. Moreover, studies have shown a strong link between cognitive function and adaptive behavior (Tassé et al., 2016; Cordeiro et al., 2020), which means that multi-domain CCT may also be beneficial to improve adaptive functioning.

We aimed to verify the effect of multi-domain CCT on intellectual function and adaptive functioning in children with IDD. We hypothesized that multi-domain computer-assisted cognitive training could enhance the comprehensive cognitive function of children with IDD and, thereby, improve their intellectual performance and adaptive functioning.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Study design

We conducted a single-blinded randomized controlled trial, whereby clinical assessments were administered to all participants at pre-intervention, post-intervention (5 weeks), and at follow-up (3 months after post-intervention). The protocol for this trial was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Rehabilitation Hospital Affiliated to Fujian University of Traditional Chinese Medicine (Approved number: 2019KY-004-01) in 2019. This study was performed in accordance with the guideline of the Declaration of Helsinki, and informed consent was obtained from all participants and their legal guardians before screening. The details of this trial were also registered in the China Clinical Trial Registry (Registration number: ChiCTR1900024413; Registration date: 10/07/2019).

2.2. Participants

Eligible participants were recruited from special education schools, child rehabilitation training centers, and local nonprofit organizations in Fuzhou City, China. We included children aged 4–6.5 years who met the DSM-5 IDD criteria as diagnosed by clinicians, as well as children with a the full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) score < 70, as measured by the Chinese-Wechsler Young Children Scale of Intelligence (C-WYCSI; Gong Yaoxian, 1992); and an adaptive developmental quotient (ADQ) < 70, as measured by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-Chinese Version (VABS-C; Yao and Gong, 1991). We excluded children with any visual, auditory, or motor impairment to the extent that it was impossible to complete the assessment and training; those who failed to complete the assessments and training due to the inability to follow instructions or poor cooperation; and those who participated in any activity within the past 30 days that may have affected the results of this study, such as performing the same test or participating in another clinical research trial.

2.3. Intervention

All therapists were licensed rehabilitation therapists with at least 3 years of clinical experience in the treatment of children with IDD, and all training sessions were based on intervention manuals and arranged according to the individual characteristics of each child. The therapists for the CCT group were only responsible for switching the machine on and off and monitoring the treatment status of the children, and did not participate in the cognitive training process. Therapists for the control group carried out one-to-one training according to the intervention. The interventions were conducted in quiet treatment rooms.

During the intervention period, each child received five training sessions, five times a week, for 20 min each time. Training for both groups covered four cognitive domains, namely visual perception, attention, memory, and reasoning, but the medium and content of training were different. The CCT group was trained through an electronic platform, while the control group was trained by a therapist.

2.3.1. CCT experimental group

The children performed training sessions on electronic tablets. The training program task consisted of four blocks (visual perception, attention, memory, and reasoning) and each included two trials (eight trials in total) which are most relevant to the cognitive impairment of children with IDD (Figure 1). Each trial had three levels and was adaptive: the computer-assisted programs provided immediate feedback according to the child’s performance and adjusted the difficulty of the exercises and reinforcement methods. When a child reached 80% correct answers, the difficulty of the training increased. In the training process, the embedded human-computer interaction function of the system provided visual and verbal guidance. Participants received encouragement and feedback after completion of each level, such as stickers and fireworks/thumbs up on the screen as the task reward. During each training session, four of the eight trials were randomly selected. Each session is 20 min long, each trial lasted for 5 min, for a total training time of 20 min. The trials were presented as follows.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The eight trials of computerized cognitive training. (A) Visual perception block, including the spatial perception and perceptual speed trials. (B) Attention block, including the select reaction time trial and space exploration trial. (C) Working memory block, including the memory span trial and visual memory trial. (D) Reasoning block, including the deductive reasoning and figure recognition trials.

2.3.1.1. Perceptual speed trial (visual perception)

The children help a ticket inspector choose the corresponding small animal ticket. There are several animals on an advancing train and, according to the animals randomly appearing in the upper left corner of the screen, the children click on the matching animals to make them enter the train.

2.3.1.2. Spatial perception trial (visual perception)

This task aims to classify similar objects in the room and then put them back where they should be stored, such as vegetables, fruits, and electrical appliances. These are all common items in the children’s daily lives.

2.3.1.3. Select reaction time trial (attention)

The task module interface is a randomly changing color of asteroids revolving around a large planet. The children need to compare the randomly appearing and changing color of asteroids to determine consistency with the color of the giant planet and make a quick response.

2.3.1.4. Space exploration trial (attention)

A monkey is in a maze and wants to eat a banana. The children need to analyze the complicated topography and, using as few attempts and steps as possible, help the monkey cross the maze to successfully eat the banana.

2.3.1.5. Memory span trial (working memory)

The task module interface involves a peacock which has 20 large feathers and is looking for friends. The distance between the peacock and the friend starts as five steps. Each time the peacock illuminates a feather, the children need to remember the corresponding feather position and respond correctly. Each time the selection is correct, the distance between the peacock and the friend reduces by one step. The position of the feathers is entirely random.

2.3.1.6. Visual memory trial (working memory)

A group of small animals is hidden in the eggshell. The children need to memorize the location of the small animals and, when the small animals are hidden, find the position according to the instruction picture provided by the training module. The location of the animal matches the picture.

2.3.1.7. Deductive reasoning trial (reasoning)

The children are required to propose solutions to dilemmas encountered in the project environment based on their daily life experiences. These may include having to go out on rainy days and what items should be taken with.

2.3.1.8. Figure recognition trial (reasoning)

A random picture with a crucial missing part will appear on the screen. The children need to indicate the missing part according to common sense and after observation.

2.3.2. Traditional cognitive training (control group)

The children in the control group underwent training administered by the traditional cognitive training program concerning the contents of rehabilitation for IDD in the 2016 Practical Child Rehabilitation Medicine (2nd ed; Li, 2016). All therapists were trained before the trial, and the four control trials involved visual perception, attention, memory, and reasoning ability. The therapist trained each child using rehabilitation teaching aids (such as building blocks, beads, and cards), and judged and adjusted the difficulty of the training based on the child’s performance. If the correct rate of training exceeded 80%, the therapist adjusted the difficulty. Each trial lasted 5 min at each training session, for a total of 20 min. The therapist recorded the completion of daily training tasks using a notebook. The control groups were administered rewards in the same way as the corresponding experimental groups.

2.4. Outcome measures

The primary and secondary outcomes were assessed at baseline, after the intervention, and 3 months after the intervention.

2.4.1. Primary outcome

The Chinese-Wechsler Young Children scale (C-WYCSI; Gong Yaoxian, 1992), revised by Gong in 1986 and based on WPPSI, is an individually administered intelligence test that measures the intelligence level of children with IDD, to generate FSIQ, verbal IQ (VIQ), and performance IQ (PIQ) scores. Moreover, the subtests are linked to many cognitive functions, such as learning ability, attention, memory, and visual perception. The verbal scale includes information, vocabulary, arithmetic, similarities, and comprehension tasks. The performance scale includes animal egg, picture completion, maze, geometric design, and block design or visual analysis tasks. The C-WYCSI is applied to children aged 4–6.5 years, with an internal consistency level of significance > 0.7. A cutoff value of 70 distinguishes normal from IDD.

2.4.2. Secondary outcomes

The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-Chinese Version (VABS-C; Yao and Gong, 1991) is a well-validated multidimensional instrument that measures adaptive behavior skills and overall severity of IDD. It consists of three main scales—daily life skills, communication, and socialization—tested on eight subscales: sensorimotor development (six items); self-care skills (10 items); speech and language development (nine items); personal orientation (10 items); social responsibility (nine items); space–time orientation (four items); labor skills (seven items); and economic activities (four items). The daily life skills scale comprises subscales for sensorimotor development, self-care skills, labor skills, and economic activities; the communication scale, speech and language development and space–time orientation; and the socialization scale, personal orientation and social responsibility.

The VABS-C is suitable for children aged 3–12 years and has a level of internal consistency significance > 0.8. A total adaptive behavior score (ADQ) and three sub-area scores (daily living skills, communication, and socialization) provide an overview of adaptive functioning in children with IDD. Higher scores indicate better adaptive behavior skills. A cutoff value of 70 distinguishes normal from adaptive behavior deficits.

2.5. Statistical analyses

A blinded statistician performed all data analyses using SPSS statistics V.24.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, United States). The statistical significance level was taken as 0.05. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics data (i.e., child’s sex, primary caregiver, and disorder) were assessed using independent samples t-tests and chi-square tests. This study used an intention-to-treat analysis and imputed missing data by multiple imputations of the SPSS statistics software. For each intellectual function and adaptive functioning, a 2 (group) × 3 (time) linear mixed model (LMM) for repeated measures was used to test the effects of the intervention. This study specified time (baseline vs. post-training vs. follow-up) and group (experimental vs. control) as fixed effects in this model. In contrast, between-participant mean differences (i.e., the intercepts) were modeled as random effects. We followed significant interaction effects using post hoc tests with paired-samples t-tests. In addition, Holm–Bonferroni corrections were applied to the results of the interaction effects on the three cognitive abilities measured.

Pearson’s correlation analyses were then performed between the intellectual function and adaptive functioning changes that showed significant time × group interaction effects. We also investigated whether VIQ mediated the group intervention effect on changes in adaptive performance for significant intervention effects on the changes in adaptive functioning that correlated with the change in verbal intelligence due to the intervention. We performed mediation analyses using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013) implemented in SPSS, with the group as the independent variable, VIQ as the mediator variable, and changes in adaptive functioning after the intervention as the dependent variable. In the mediation models, any mediating effect of VIQ was reflected as the indirect effect. The PROCESS macro was based on ordinary least-squares regression and adopted a nonparametric bootstrapping procedure (5,000 times), which gave rise to a bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) for effect size inference (Shrout and Bolger, 2002). A significant effect as p < 0.05 indicated if zero was not included within the 95% CI (Preacher and Hayes, 2008).

3. Results

A total of 414 children were screened, 304 of whom met the exclusion criteria. Therefore, 60 children with mild IDD were finally included in this study (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. CCT study protocol and participant flow according to Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines.

3.1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics

Thirty children in the intervention group and 30 in the control group were analyzed. The baseline characteristics of the intervention and control groups are given in Table 1. The mean chronological age of the intervention group was 5.13 ± 0.74 months, with 24 boys and six girls, and the mean chronological age of the control group was 5.03 ± 0.89 months, with 26 boys and four girls. There was no significant difference between the experimental group and the control group in terms of chronological age, sex, primary caregivers, and parent marital status (p > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics.

3.2. Intervention effects on IQ

3.2.1. FSIQ

The results of the LMM analysis showed that the overall FSIQ scores significantly increased after training (main effect of time: F[58] = 272.06, p < 0.01), and the time × group interaction effect was also significant (F[58] = 31.97, p < 0.01). Specifically, there were significant increases in scores on the FSIQ for the CCT group (z[29] = −4.69, p < 0.01) and the control group (z[29] = −3.71, p < 0.01) after training (Table 2; Figure 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of results of mixed linear model analyses on score of different clinical outcome measures of participants in the multi-domain computerized cognitive training group (Experimental group) and multi-domain traditional cognitive training group (Control group) for the baseline, post-training, and 3-month follow-up assessment occasions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Within-group comparisons of (A) FSIQ, (B) VIQ, (C) PIQ, (D) arithmetic, (E) vocabulary, and (F) adaptive functioning scores for the baseline and post-training assessment occasions. Horizontal lines indicate significant post hoc comparisons with paired t-tests (*p ≤ 0.05, two-tailed). Significant group × time interaction effects were found in FSIQ, VIQ, and arithmetic.

3.2.2. VIQ

The results of the LMM analysis showed that the overall VIQ scores significantly increased after training (main effect of time: F[58] = 175.01, p < 0. 01), and the time × group interaction effect was also significant (F[58] = 33.83, p < 0.01). Specifically, there were significant increases in scores on the VIQ for the CCT group (z[29] = −10.59, p < 0.01) and the MCT group (z[29] = −2.84, p < 0.01) after training (Table 2; Figure 3).

3.2.3. Arithmetic and vocabulary of VIQ

Significant group × time effects were revealed on the arithmetic (F[58] = 29.94, p < 0.01) and vocabulary (F[58] = 17.30, p < 0.01) scores. Both the CCT (z = −4.57, p < 0.01) and MCT (z = −3.46, p < 0.01) groups showed significant increases in arithmetic after completing the training programs. Significant increases were observed in vocabulary in the experimental group from the baseline to post-training (z = −6.01, p < 0.01). In contrast, participants in the control group did not show significant differences between these two assessments (z = 1.00, p = 0.32; Table 2; Figure 3).

3.2.4. PIQ

The results of the LMM analysis showed that the overall PIQ scores significantly increased after training (main effect of time: F[58] = 183.19, p < 0.01), whereas no significant increase was observed for the time × group interaction effect (F[58] = 1.06, p = 0.351). Specifically, there were significant increases in scores on the PIQ for the CCT group (z[29] = −16.59, p < 0.01) and the MCT group (z[29] = −9.83, p < 0.01) after training (Table 2; Figure 3).

3.2.5. Animal egg and block design/visual analysis of PIQ

Significant group × time effects were revealed on the scores of the animal egg (F[58] = 6.143, p = 0.003) and block design/visual analysis (F[58] = 5.046, p = 0.008). Significant increases in scores on the animal egg and block design/visual analysis were observed in both the CCT (z = −4.47, p < 0.01; z = −3.00, p < 0.01, respectively) and MCT groups (z = −3.31, p < 0.01; z = −2.14, p = 0.03, respectively; Table 2).

3.3. Intervention effects on adaptive functioning

For the ADQ, the LMM analysis showed that the main effects of time (F[58] = 676.95, p < 0.01) and time × group interaction effect (F[58] = 28.05, p < 0.01) were significant. Post-hoc tests showed that the ADQ of the participants in the experimental (z[29] = −4.71, p < 0.01) and control groups (z[29] = −4.68, p < 0.01) increased significantly after training compared to the baseline (Table 2; Figure 3).

3.3.1. Communication and socialization of ADQ

Significant group × time effects were revealed for communication (F[58] = 10.86, p < 0.01) and socialization (F[58] = 4.351, p = 0.015) scores. Significant increases in scores for communication and socialization were observed in both the CCT (z = −4.23, p < 0.01; z = −5.51, p < 0.01, respectively) and MCT groups (z = −2.45, p < 0.01; z = −4.78, p < 0.01, respectively; Table 2).

3.4. Change of VIQ mediates the intervention effect on ADQ

The change in FSIQ and VIQ (ΔFSIQ and ΔVIQ) were found to be significantly correlated with the change of the ADQ performance after training (ΔADQ; r[60] = 0.74, p < 0.01, r[60] = 0.70, p < 0.01, respectively) across the groups. Thus, we explored whether the ΔVIQ following the intervention mediated the group effect on ΔADQ. We conducted mediation analyses with the group as the independent variable, ΔVIQ as the mediator variable, and ΔADQ as the dependent variable. The analyses revealed that the mediating effect of ΔVIQ was significant and positive (bootstrapping CI: [0.17, 0.95]), and the direct effect was also significant (bootstrapping CI: [0.44, 1.68]). Specifically, the group (experimental group coded as 1, control group coded as 0) had a positive effect on the ΔVIQ (bootstrapping CI: [2.07, 3.44]). This was consistent with the finding that the experimental group showed greater improvement of VIQ following the intervention which, in turn, had a positive effect on ΔADQ performance (bootstrapping CI: [0.32, 0.37]). Therefore, IDD children with a greater improvement of VIQ following the multi-domain CCT showed a more significant increase in ADQ, reflecting better adaptive functioning (Figure 4). Correlation analyses between the changes of the intelligence function (rs = 0.74, p < 0.01) and adaptive functioning (rs = 0.70, p < 0.01) showed significant time × group interaction effects (Figure 5).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The VIQ change (ΔVIQ) due to the intervention mediates the group’s effect on ADQ change (ΔADQ). Group was the independent variable (experimental = 1, control = 0), ΔVIQ was the mediator variable, and ΔADQ was the dependent variable. Beside each arrow is the bootstrapping confidence interval (CI) of each effect. *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation analyses performed between the changes of the intelligent function and adaptive functioning that show significant time × group interaction effects.

4. Discussion

We found that a 5-week multi-domain CCT can significantly enhance the intellectual performance and adaptive functioning of children with IDD. The improved intellectual function is positively connected with improved adaptive functioning. Moreover, the increase of verbal intelligence mediated the training-induced increase in adaptive functioning.

CCT outperforms traditional cognitive training in the FSIQ, VIQ, and animal egg and block design/visual analysis in the sub-items of PIQ. The improvement of intellectual function and cognitive function in children with IDD induced by CCT is consistent with previous studies. Van der Molen et al. (2010) showed that 5 weeks of computerized memory training increased intellect, short-term linguistic memory, and story recall. Kirk et al. (2016) observed that selective attention was enhanced after 5 weeks of computerized attention training in IDD children aged 4–11 years, and the positive impact was still substantial after 3 months. These single-domain CCT studies found a better effect on domain-specific improvement. However, although these studies found that CCT training had a good effect on cognitive impairment in a single domain, its generalization effect to other cognitive domains was limited (Bennett et al., 2013; de Vries et al., 2015).

In this study, we used a multi-domain CCT. Some research reveals that integrating different cognitive functions into a successful cognitive training program can compensate for the restriction and instability of CCT training impact in a single cognitive field (Cortese et al., 2015). The animal egg task is related to hand-eye coordination, attention, and memory. The block design/visual analysis task assesses visual-motor coordination and abstract conceptualization, indicating that multi-domain CCT has certain advantages in improving the attention, memory, and visual-motor coordination of children with IDD. Previous studies have shown that multi-domain cognitive training can increase functional integration within and between different brain networks (Cao et al., 2016) and contribute to the functional remodeling of brain neuroplasticity (Gates and Sachdev, 2014). After training, the children showed enhanced neural activity in the orbitofrontal, superior frontal, middle temporal and inferior frontal cortices in the inhibition paradigm (Hoekzema et al., 2010). As a result, multi-domain CCT may support cognitive ability improvement in different dimensions of IDD children, resulting in superposition and “migration” effects, which can improve overall cognitive function and intelligence.

Several studies also showed that CCT can improve the academic achievement of children with IDD (Söderqvist et al., 2012; Dunning et al., 2013; Kirk et al., 2015; Kirk H. E. et al., 2017). This is consistent with the results of this study, showing that the improved PIQ scores are closely associated with learning ability in children with IDD in post-training. In addition, we noted a decrease in PIQ scores during the follow-up period, which may have decreased due to the reduced cognitive training they received. This suggests that cognitive interventions can help improve PIQ in children with IDD, but longer-term ongoing interventions are needed to ensure stable and sustained improvement in children’s cognitive abilities. Future studies should conduct longer-term interventions and observations to further clarify this point.

We also found that multi-domain CCT brings about a significant improvement in ADQ, communication, and social factor indicators, and FSIQ was positively correlated with ADQ. These findings are consistent with prior CCT research. Computerized attention training increased behavioral control in children with fetal alcohol syndrome disorders, according to Coles et al. (2018), which improved adaptive functioning in daily life. Ko et al. (2020) discovered that computerized attention training could positively affect children’s adaptive behavior with cognitive impairment; Lanfranchi et al. (2017) noticed that computerized memory training could significantly improve the working memory of children with Down syndrome, and this ability after training can be transferred to self-care in daily life. Several studies (Lightbody and Reiss, 2009; Camprodon-Rosanas et al., 2019; Uddin, 2021; Chen et al., 2022) found that activating brain areas associated with cognitive function improves behavioral performance. The neural functions behind specific cognitions influence adaptive behavior in real-time (Lightbody and Reiss, 2009). Therefore, improvements in comprehensive cognitive performance can be transferred to adaptive functioning, resulting in improved adaptive behavior in children.

We found that the 5-week training improved the arithmetic and vocabulary tasks of IDD children. In terms of arithmetic task, the experimental group increased 1.54 points, while the control group increased 0.77 points. In terms of vocabulary task, only the experimental group had significant differences compared with the pre-intervention. Kroesbergen et al. (2014) studied 51 children with low working memory ability and found that 4 weeks of working memory training can promote the improvement of early computing ability. Yang et al. (2017) also found that 3-week computerized working memory training can significantly improve children’s reading fluency. The theory of internal cognitive load shows that cognitive ability is closely related to academic skills, and that academic skills competency requires the use of a variety of cognitive resources (Tabbers et al., 2004). In this study, the multidisciplinary CCT improved vocabulary and arithmetic skills after training and during the follow-up period. This continuous effect may relate to the multidisciplinary cognitive training that improves overall cognitive ability, so that IDD children can perform better in academic skills.

We found that CCT can indirectly promote the improvement of children’s adaptive functioning by improving verbal intelligence. This improvement in adaptive behavior may manifest in communication and socialization. Verbal intelligence is essential for children to communicate and socialize (Soorya et al., 2015; Hodge et al., 2021; Lyons, 2021). Multi-domain CCT promotes the recovery of language intelligence due to the rich audio-visual interaction (Takacs et al., 2015; Jaschke et al., 2018) and daily life scenes (Helo et al., 2021) in the training module. This also suggests that promoting language intelligence may be an essential aspect to improve the adaptive behavior of IDD children, which requires attention in the future training of IDD children.

There are several limitations in the current study. First, this study is not a double-blind randomized controlled clinical trial. We used blinding methods for the outcome evaluators and statisticians to minimize interference with the experimental results. Second, the sample size was small due to resource limitations and strict inclusion criteria. Therefore, future studies should conduct multi-center randomized controlled clinical trials with larger sample sizes to further validate the effects. Third, most children with IDD included in this study had mild intellectual disabilities. Future studies could consider examining the effects of CCT on moderate or severe intellectual disability children with IDD. Finally, this study focused on analyzing the effects of CCT on cognition and related functions in children with IDD after 5 weeks of training. There may be a learning effect for repeated assessments within a short period of time, which limits the extrapolation of CCT efficacy. To better elucidate the long-term effects of CCT on intellectual and adaptive functioning in children with IDD, future studies could be conducted for longer periods of time, and techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging and near-infrared brain imaging could be used to explore the neural mechanisms and provide more physiological evidence for the training effects.

Our findings suggest that multi-domain CCT can improve the results of intelligence and adaptive functioning tests in children with IDD. In addition, the improvement of adaptive functioning induced by cognitive training may be due to the enhancement of verbal IQ.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Fujian University of Traditional Chinese Medicine Affiliated Rehabilitation Hospital Ethics Board for the project. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

JW and JP: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, writing—original draft, and project administration. ZL and HD: resources, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. ZH, YH, JT, and LC: methodology, supervision, and writing—review and editing. JH: methodology, resources, writing—review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Guiding Project of Fujian Provincial Department of Science and Technology, China (2022Y0038).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge and thank all the children and parents who participated in this study; we thank the special education schools, children’s rehabilitation training center, and local nonprofit organizations in Fuzhou, China for their assistance with recruitment. The study was supported by the Guiding Project of Fujian Provincial Department of Science and Technology fellowship.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

ADQ, Adaptive developmental quotient; CCT, Computerized cognitive training; CI, Confidence interval; IDD, Intellectual developmental disorder; ITT, Intention-to-treat; LMM, Linear mixed model; RCT, Randomized controlled trial.

References

American Psychiatric Association and American Psychiatric Association eds. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Bennett, S. J., Holmes, J., and Buckley, S. (2013). Computerized memory training leads to sustained improvement in visuospatial short-term memory skills in children with down syndrome. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 118, 179–192. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-118.3.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camprodon-Rosanas, E., Pujol, J., Martinez-Vilavella, G., Blanco-Hinojo, L., Medrano-Martorell, S., Batlle, S., et al. (2019). Brain structure and function in school-aged children with sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 58, 256–266. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.09.441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, W., Cao, X., Hou, C., Li, T., Cheng, Y., Jiang, L., et al. (2016). Effects of cognitive training on resting-state functional connectivity of default mode, salience, and central executive networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:70. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. N., Lin, C. K., Wei, T. S., Liu, C. H., and Wuang, Y. P. J. R. I. D. D. (2013). The effectiveness of multimedia visual perceptual training groups for the preschool children with developmental delay. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 4447–4454. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Tam, A., Kebets, V., Orban, C., Ooi, L. Q. R., Asplund, C. L., et al. (2022). Shared and unique brain network features predict cognitive, personality, and mental health scores in the ABCD study. Nat. Commun. 13:2217. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-29766-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., Taddeo, E., and Strickland, D. (2018). GoFAR: improving attention, behavior and adaptive functioning in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: brief report. Dev. Neurorehabil. 21, 345–349. doi: 10.1080/17518423.2018.1424263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordeiro, L., Villagomez, A., Swain, D., Deklotz, S., and Tartaglia, N. (2020). Adaptive skills in FXS: a review of the literature and evaluation of the PEDI-computer adaptive test (PEDI-CAT) to measure adaptive skills. Brain Sci. 10:351. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10060351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., Dittmann, R. W., et al. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, J., Zhang, Y., Wan, S., Chen, C., Zeng, J., and Zhou, X. (2019). Visual form perception is fundamental for both reading comprehension and arithmetic computation. Cognition 189, 141–154. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vries, M., Prins, P. J., Schmand, B. A., and Geurts, H. M. (2015). Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 566–576. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deary, I. J. A. R. O. P. (2012). Intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 453–482. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denervaud, S., Gentaz, E., Matusz, P. J., and Murray, M. M. (2020). Multisensory gains in simple detection predict global cognition in schoolchildren. Sci. Rep. 10:1394. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-58329-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., and Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Dev. Sci. 16, 915–925. doi: 10.1111/desc.12068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, N. J., and Sachdev, P. (2014). Is cognitive training an effective treatment for preclinical and early Alzheimer's disease? J. Alzheimers Dis. 42, S551–S559. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong Yaoxian, D. (1992). Manual for the Chinese-Wechsler young children scale of intelligence (C-WYCSI). Changsha, Hunan: Hunan Cartography Publishing House.

Google Scholar

Hartin, P., Nugent, C., McClean, S., Cleland, I., Tschanz, J., Clark, C., et al. (2016). The empowering role of Mobile apps in behavior change interventions: the Gray matters randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 4:e93. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.4878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. pp. 335–337.

Google Scholar

Helo, A., Guerra, E., Coloma, C. J., Aravena-Bravo, P., and Rama, P. (2021). Do children with developmental language disorder activate scene knowledge to guide visual attention? Effect of object-scene inconsistencies on gaze allocation. Front. Psychol. 12:796459. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.796459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrik, D., Valentina, Z., Lisa, P., and Silvia, L. J. F. I. P. (2015). The effectiveness of working memory training with individuals with intellectual disabilities – A meta-analytic review. Front. Psychol. 6:1230. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilger, K., Ekman, M., Fiebach, C., and Basten, U. (2017). Intelligence is associated with the modular structure of intrinsic brain networks. Sci. Rep. 7:16088. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15795-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodge, M. A., Boulton, K. A., Sutherland, R., Barnett, D., Bennett, B., Chan, E., et al. (2021). Predictors of adaptive functioning in preschool aged children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 14, 1444–1455. doi: 10.1002/aur.2501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoekzema, E., Carmona, S., Tremols, V., Gispert, J. D., Guitart, M., Fauquet, J., et al. (2010). Enhanced neural activity in frontal and cerebellar circuits after cognitive training in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1942–1950. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaschke, A. C., Honing, H., and Scherder, E. J. A. (2018). Exposure to a musically-enriched environment; its relationship with executive functions, short-term memory and verbal IQ in primary school children. PLoS One 13:e0207265. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ke, Y., Weng, M., Chhetri, G., Usman, M., Li, Y., Yu, Q., et al. (2020). Trappc9 deficiency in mice impairs learning and memory by causing imbalance of dopamine D1 and D2 neurons. Sci. Adv. 6. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abb7781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, H. E., Gray, K., Riby, D. M., and Cornish, K. M. (2015). Cognitive training as a resolution for early executive function difficulties in children with intellectual disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 38, 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.12.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, H. E., Gray, K. M., Ellis, K., Taffe, J., and Cornish, K. M. (2016). Computerised attention training for children with intellectual and developmental disabilities: a randomised controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 1380–1389. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, H., Gray, K., Ellis, K., Taffe, J., and Cornish, K. (2017). Impact of attention training on academic achievement, executive functioning, and behavior: a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 122, 97–117. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-122.2.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, H. E., Gray, K., Riby, D. M., Taffe, J., and Cornish, K. M. (2017). Visual attention and academic performance in children with developmental disabilities and behavioural attention deficits. Dev. Sci. 20. doi: 10.1111/desc.12468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ko, E. J., Sung, I. Y., Yuk, J. S., Jang, D. H., and Yun, G. (2020). A tablet computer-based cognitive training program for young children with cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Medicine 99:e19549. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000019549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroesbergen, E. H., van Noordende, J. E., and Kolkman, M. E. (2014). Training working memory in kindergarten children: effects on working memory and early numeracy. Child Neuropsychol. 20, 23–37. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.736483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanfranchi, S., Pulina, F., Carretti, B., and Mammarella, I. C. (2017). Training spatial-simultaneous working memory in individuals with down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 64, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. (2016). Practical child rehabilitation medicine (2nd Edition). Beijing: People's Medical Publishing House.

Google Scholar

Lightbody, A. A., and Reiss, A. L. (2009). Gene, brain, and behavior relationships in fragile X syndrome: evidence from neuroimaging studies. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 15, 343–352. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, R. (2021). Impact of language disorders on children's everyday lives from 4 to 13 years: commentary on Le, Mensah, Eadie, McKean, Schiberras, Bavin, Reilly and gold (2020). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 1485–1487. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, J., Kang, H., Kim, J., Jeong, H., Im, J., Namgung, E., et al. (2017). Network attributes underlying intellectual giftedness in the developing brain. Sci. Rep. 7:11321. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11593-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenzie, K., Milton, M., Smith, G., and Ouellette-Kuntz, H. (2016). Systematic review of the prevalence and incidence of intellectual disabilities: Current trends and issues. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 3, 104–115. doi: 10.1007/s40474-016-0085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purugganan, O. J. P. I. R. (2018). Intellectual Disabilities. Pediatr. Rev. 39, 299–309. doi: 10.1542/pir.2016-0116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, L., Holland, A., Dykens, E., Strong, T., Roof, E., and Bohonowych, J. (2016). Prader-Willi syndrome mental health research strategy workshop proceedings: the state of the science and future directions. Orphanet J. Rare Dis. 11:131. doi: 10.1186/s13023-016-0504-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serret, S., Hun, S., Iakimova, G., and Lozada, J. (2014). Facing the challenge of teaching emotions to individuals with low-and high-functioning autism using a new serious game: a pilot study. Mol. Autism. 5:37. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Söderqvist, S., Nutley, S., Ottersen, J., Grill, K., and Klingberg, T. J. F. I. H. N. (2012). Computerized training of non-verbal reasoning and working memory in children with intellectual disability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:271. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soorya, L. V., Siper, P. M., Beck, T., Soffes, S., Halpern, D., Gorenstein, M., et al. (2015). Randomized comparative trial of a social cognitive skills group for children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 208–216.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabbers, H. K., Martens, R. L., and Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: effects of modality and cueing. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 71–81. doi: 10.1348/000709904322848824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., and Bus, A. G. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 85, 698–739. doi: 10.3102/0034654314566989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tassé, M., Luckasson, R., and Schalock, R. L. (2016). The relation between intellectual functioning and adaptive behavior in the diagnosis of intellectual disability. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 54, 381–390. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-54.6.381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q. (2021). Brain mechanisms supporting flexible cognition and behavior in adolescents with autism Spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 89, 172–183. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Molen, M. J., Van Luit, J. E., Van der Molen, M. W., Klugkist, I., and Jongmans, M. J. (2010). Effectiveness of a computerised working memory training in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 54, 433–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01285.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, Y., Chiang, C., Chen, S., and Wuang, Y. (2017). The effectiveness of the computerized visual perceptual training program on individuals with down syndrome: an fMRI study. Res. Dev. Disabil. 66, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Liu, A., He, I., and Bai, Y. (2020). Bioinformatics analysis revealed novel 3'UTR variants associated with intellectual disability. Genes 11:998. doi: 10.3390/genes11090998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Peng, J., Zhang, D., Zheng, L., and Mo, L. (2017). Specific effects of working memory training on the reading skills of Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. PLoS One 12:e0186114. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, S., and Gong, Y. (1991). Manual for the Vineland adaptive behavior scale-Chinese version. Changsha, Hunan: Hunan Medical University Press.

Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Liu, Q., Wen, P., Zhang, J., and Xu, F. (2017). Activation of the dopaminergic pathway from VTA to the medial olfactory tubercle generates odor-preference and reward. Elife 6:e25423. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: computerized cognitive training, intellectual function, adaptive behavior, intellectual developmental disorder, children rehabilitation

Citation: Wu J, Peng J, Li Z, Deng H, Huang Z, He Y, Tu J, Cao L and Huang J (2023) Multi-domain computerized cognitive training for children with intellectual developmental disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Front. Psychol. 13:1059889. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1059889

Received: 02 October 2022; Accepted: 08 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023.

Edited by:

Ai Leen Choo, Georgia State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Kathleen Koth, Medical College of Wisconsin, United States
Magdalena S. Halim, Atma Jaya Catholic University of Indonesia, Indonesia

Copyright © 2023 Wu, Peng, Li, Deng, Huang, He, Tu, Cao and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jia Huang, ✉ amFzbWluZTE4NzRAMTYzLmNvbQ==

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.