- 1Department of Neurology and Laboratory of Neuroscience, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
- 2PhD Program in Neuroscience, School of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milano-Bicocca, Monza, Italy
- 3Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Dino Ferrari Center, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy
- 4Neurology Residency Program, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy
- 5Laboratory of Clinical Chemistry, Department of Laboratory Medicine, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
- 6Aldo Ravelli Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics, Department of Health Sciences, International Medical School, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
- 7ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, San Paolo University Hospital, Milan, Italy
- 8IRCCS Ca’ Granda Foundation Maggiore Policlinico Hospital, Milan, Italy
- 9Department of Medical Biotechnology and Translational Medicine, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy
Background: This study aimed at testing the ability of the frontal assessment battery (FAB) to differentiate between patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (AD), as well as comparing its discriminative power to that of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE).
Methods: The present retrospective cohort included N = 107 Aβ-positive patients diagnosed with either MCI due to AD (N = 40) or probable AD dementia (ADD; N = 67). A two-step multiple logistic regression (MLR) was run to predict an MCI vs. ADD diagnosis based on FAB scores. Within the baseline step, demographics, disease duration, MMSE scores, and information on cognitive phenotypes were entered, with the FAB being added within the second step. Receiver-operating characteristics analyses were also run to derive intrinsic and post-test diagnostics.
Results: Within the baseline MLR step, only lower MMSE scores predicted the occurrence of ADD; by adding the FAB, which likewise was able to discriminate between MCI and ADD (p = 0.016), a significant increase in model fit was detected (p = 0.007). The diagnostic efficiency of the FAB (AUC = 0.85) was comparable (p = 0.583) to that of the MMSE (AUC = 0.82), also yielding good intrinsic and post-test diagnostics, which were comparable to those of the MMSE.
Discussion: The FAB is a diagnostically sound screener to discriminate between MCI and ADD, independently of patients’ overall cognitive profile. In doing so, the FAB is comparable to the MMSE, and the complementation of the latter with the former is advisable in order to increase the accuracy in differentiating between MCI and ADD within screening sessions.
Background
Executive functioning (EF) deficits are a feature of both mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Reinvang et al., 2012) and dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Guarino et al., 2019), which negatively affect performance in everyday activities (Marshall et al., 2011; Amanzio et al., 2018), and, as a marker of cognitive involution overall (Kirova et al., 2015), independently predict the conversion from MCI to dementia (Rozzini et al., 2007; Jung et al., 2020). Therefore, the availability of sound cognitive screening tests for the early detection of EF deficits is clinically crucial (Kirova et al., 2015).
In this regard, the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) (Dubois et al., 2000), a widespread and easy-to-perform screening test, has proved effective in detecting EF deficits in both MCI and dementia due to AD (ADD) (Hurtado-Pomares et al., 2018). Moreover, it has been validated against AD-specific neuroradiological biomarkers (Kume et al., 2011; Nagata et al., 2011; Oshima et al., 2012) and shown to be predictive of functional outcomes (Ikezaki et al., 2020). Nevertheless, currently available evidence on the capability of the FAB to discriminate between MCI and ADD is limited to one report (Yamao et al., 2011) that addressed a relatively small sample (N = 48) of selected, clinically diagnosed amnestic MCI and early-stage patients with ADD. Hence, information that is fully representative of the AD spectrum is still lacking, albeit being potentially relevant at a diagnostic and prognostic level (Rozzini et al., 2007; Marshall et al., 2011; Yamao et al., 2011; Reinvang et al., 2012; Kirova et al., 2015; Amanzio et al., 2018; Guarino et al., 2019; Jung et al., 2020); this is particularly true for AD as biologically defined according to the 2018 National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) amyloidosis/tauopathy/neurodegeneration (ATN) framework (Jack et al., 2018).
Given the above premises, this study aimed at testing the ability of the FAB to differentiate between neurochemically confirmed MCI and ADD, also comparing its discriminative power to that of a gold-standard screener for ADD – i.e., the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Mitchell, 2017).
Materials and methods
Participants
The present retrospective cohort included N = 107 Aβ-positive patients diagnosed with either MCI due to AD (N = 40) (Albert et al., 2011) or probable ADD (N = 67), referred to the Department of Neurology, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy between June 2009 and April 2022, for whom FAB scores were available, and without evidence of (1) further neurodegenerative diseases or other neurological disorders impairing cognition, (2) ongoing organ/system failure, or (3) uncorrected sensory deficits.
All patients were administered a non-fixed, individual case-adapted cognitive battery (Supplementary Table 1) covering (1) attention, (2) executive functioning, (3) short-term memory, (4) long-term memory, (5) language, and (6) visuo-spatial/praxic abilities, which allowed us to classify them based on the type and number of impaired domains.
Patients with ADD—all of whom, at variance with MCI ones, had evidence of significant functional impairment in daily living—were further classified, based on ad hoc nosographic systems, as amnestic-predominant ADD (McKhann et al., 2011), logopenic variant-primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA) (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011), posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) (Crutch et al., 2017), or behavioral variant-AD (bvAD) (Ossenkoppele et al., 2015).
Materials
Appollonio et al.’s (2005) Italian version of the FAB was administered, and its scores were adjusted for age and education accordingly. MMSE scores (age- and education-adjusted) (Measso et al., 1993) were also available for all patients.
Patients’ ATN status was retrieved based on cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) neurochemical biomarkers according to the 2018 NIA-AA framework (Jack et al., 2018). Biomarker quantification and consequent classification were conducted as follows. For patients evaluated until September 2019, Aβ42, phosphorylated tau (P-tau181), and total tau (T-tau) were measured by using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). Patients were categorized as A+ or A− when having CSF Aβ42 levels ≤ or >647 pg/ml, respectively (Palmqvist et al., 2014), as T+ or T− when having CSF P-tau181 levels ≥ or <61 pg/ml (Vanderstichele et al., 2006), and as N+ or N− when having CSF T-tau ≥ or <three different age-determined cut-offs (≤50 years: 300 pg/ml; 51–70 years: 450 pg/ml; and >70 years: 500 pg/ml) (Sjögren et al., 2001). For patients evaluated from October 2019 onward, Lumipulse chemiluminescence enzyme immunoassays (CLEIAs) were used, with the Aβ42/Aβ40 – instead of the measurement of Aβ42 alone – defining A status (≤0.069, A+; >0.069, A−), and P-tau181 and T-tau having cut-offs of 56.5 and 404 pg/ml, respectively [the three cut-offs for CLEIAs are provided by the manufacturer and are comparable to those adopted in other studies (Gobom et al., 2022)].
Statistics
A two-step multiple logistic regression (MLR) model was run to predict an MCI vs. ADD diagnosis based on FAB-adjusted scores (Appollonio et al., 2005). Within the baseline step, disease duration (months), ADD phenotype (typical – i.e., amnestic-predominant – vs. atypical – i.e., lvPPA, PCA, and bvAD), MMSE adjusted scores (Measso et al., 1993), amnestic vs. non-amnestic status, extra-mnestic, non-executive/-attentive cognitive status (i.e., at least one domain impaired vs. unimpaired among language, short-term memory, or visuo-spatial/praxic abilities), executive/attentive cognitive status (at least one domain among attention and/or executive functioning impaired vs. unimpaired), and the total number of impaired domains (range = 0–6) were entered. The FAB was then entered into the second step in order to test its incremental validity. Within both steps, model fit was assessed via Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) and MLR-based efficiency statistics computed – i.e., sensitivity (Se), specificity (Sp), and area under the curve (AUC). The difference in fit between the two steps was tested via a χ2-statistic. Collinearity was diagnosed in the presence of a variance inflation factor (VIF) >10 and a tolerance index (TI) <0.1 (Midi et al., 2010).
In addition, the AUC of the FAB was compared to that of the MMSE via receiver-operating characteristics (ROC) analyses by means of DeLong’s test for paired ROC curves (Robin et al., 2011). Intrinsic – i.e., Se and Sp – and post-test diagnostics – i.e., positive and negative predictive values (PPV; NPV) and likelihood ratios (LR+; LR−) – of the FAB were also computed at the optimal cut-off identified through Youden’s J statistic.
Analyses were run with jamovi 2.3.121 and R 4.1.02; the significance level was set α = 0.05.
Results
Table 1 shows MCI and ADD patients’ background and clinical variables.
Within the baseline MLR step (AIC = 85.9; AUC = 0.93; Se = 0.9; Sp = 0.75), only lower MMSE scores predicted the occurrence of ADD (b = −0.37; z = −3.04; p = 0.002). By adding the FAB within the second step (AIC = 80.5), a significant increase in model fit was detected [χ2(1) = 7.4; p = 0.007], with overall comparable, although slightly better, MLR-based diagnostics (AUC = 0.94; Se = 0.88; Sp = 0.8). Within such a step, lower FAB scores were predictive of the occurrence of ADD (b = −0.49; z = −2.42; p = 0.016) – with the MMSE being the only other variable yielding significance (b = −0.34; z = −2.55; p = 0.011). Within both the MLR steps, no collinearity was diagnosed (VIF ≤ 2.59; TI ≤ 0.95). The full results of both models are provided in Supplementary Tables 2, 3.
When tested within single-test ROC analyses, the diagnostic efficiency of the FAB (AUC = 0.85; SE = 0.04; CI 95% [0.74, 0.9]) was comparable (z = 0.55; p = 0.583) to that of the MMSE (AUC = 0.82; SE = 0.04; CI 95% [0.77, 0.92]). At the optimal cut-off of <12.95 (J = 0.55), the FAB yielded good intrinsic (Se = 0.67; Sp = 0.88) and post-test diagnostics (PPV = 0.9; NPV = 0.61; LR+ = 5.37; LR− = 0.38), which were overall comparable to those of the MMSE (J = 0.54; Se = 0.81; Sp = 0.73; PPV = 0.83; NPV = 0.69; LR + = 2.93; LR− = 0.27).
Discussion
The present study demonstrates that the FAB is a diagnostically sound screener for discriminating between MCI and ADD in a cohort of Aβ-confirmed patients representative of the AD spectrum.
Indeed, the FAB showed good both intrinsic and post-test features in doing so, also having a diagnostic efficiency that was overall comparable to that of the MMSE – the latter being widely acknowledged as a gold-standard screener for ADD (Mitchell, 2017). Herewith, an age- and education-adjusted (Appollonio et al., 2005) cut-off of <12.95 is proposed for discriminating between MCI and ADD patients with CSF-confirmed evidence of underlying AD pathophysiology. Notably, such an adjusted score falls below the normality threshold of <13.4 proposed by Appollonio et al. (2005), this being in line with the fact that the cut-off proposed by the present study has to be addressed within the context of differentiating between two clinical conditions.
Furthermore, findings herewith show that the supplementation of the MMSE with the FAB results in a significantly higher power as to the discrimination between MCI and ADD – in agreement with a previous report, which, however, addressed the two screeners to the identification of ADD or its differentiation from non-AD dementias (Kim et al., 2014). Therefore, considering the relatively limited total amount of time that would be required (approximately 20 min), it might be reasonable to perform both batteries during the first evaluation of AD-spectrum patients.
Of note, the diagnostic efficiency of the FAB per se and its incremental validity against the MMSE alone apply to the whole spectrum of AD cognitive profiles – including amnestic features, which are characteristic of both MCI and ADD and predominant in the present cohort – and atypical ADD presentations (i.e., lvPPA, PCA, and bvAD). Taken together, such findings align with the notion of EF deficits being by themselves predictive of an overall greater disease severity/advanced disease across the AD spectrum (Rozzini et al., 2007; Kirova et al., 2015; Jung et al., 2020) – i.e., independently of other cognitive features, including long-term memory impairment (Rozzini et al., 2007).
Finally, a number of limitations of the present study have to be acknowledged. First, in order to control for the heterogeneity of cognitive/CSF data that had been originally collected for clinical purposes, a set of categorical classifications have been herewith adopted – this inherently implies a partial loss of information. Nevertheless, the availability of normative cut-offs for both cognitive and CSF measures, as well as the fact that diagnostic judgments were formulated by clinicians with experience in the field of cognitive disorders, likewise grant a sufficient degree of validity, and thus generalizability, of such classifications. On the other hand, it cannot be ruled out that an alternative screening test exploring another single cognitive domain – e.g., language – could perform equally well in discerning dementia from MCI due to AD: this issue was beyond the scope of the present investigation but deserves examination in the setting of a further, comparative study. Second, the present study does not include genetic, neuroimaging, and follow-up clinical information (e.g., related to MCI-dementia conversion): thus, future investigations are advisable that address also such data in relation to FAB scores within the AD spectrum.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the FAB is a diagnostically sound screener to discriminate between MCI and ADD, independently of patients’ overall cognitive profile. In doing so, the FAB is comparable to the MMSE, and the complementation of the latter with the former is advisable in order to increase the accuracy in discriminating between MCI and ADD within screening sessions.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article are accessible, upon reasonable request of interested researchers to the corresponding author, at the following repository: https://zenodo.org/record/7288073#.Y2USunbMK3A.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano (I.D: 2021_05_18_04). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
EA: conceptualization, analyses, drafting, and revision. IM, EG, and LC: data collection and revision. AD, AR, NT, ET, and VS: resources and revision. RF and AP: revision. FV and BP: resources, conceptualization, drafting, and revision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was funded by the Italian Ministry of Health (Ricerca Corrente, Project 23C125). IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano covered publication fees.
Acknowledgments
The authors are thankful to patients and their caregivers. The authors also thank healthcare professionals involved in patient care and laboratory personnel.
Conflict of interest
VS received compensation for consulting services and/or speaking activities from AveXis, Cytokinetics, Italfarmaco, Liquidweb S.r.l., and Novartis Pharma AG and receives or has received research supports from the Italian Ministry of Health, AriSLA, and E-Rare Joint Transnational Call. He is on the Editorial Board of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis and Frontotemporal Degeneration, European Neurology, and American Journal of Neurodegenerative Diseases. BP received compensation for consulting services and/or speaking activities from Liquidweb S.r.l. NT received compensation for consulting services from Amylyx Pharmaceuticals and Zambon Biotech SA.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1054321/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging−Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
Amanzio, M., Palermo, S., Zucca, M., Rosato, R., Rubino, E., Leotta, D., et al. (2018). Neuropsychological correlates of instrumental activities of daily living in neurocognitive disorders: A possible role for executive dysfunction and mood changes. Int. Psychogeriatr. 30, 1871–1881. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218000455
Appollonio, I., Leone, M., Isella, V., Piamarta, F., Consoli, T., Villa, M. L., et al. (2005). The frontal assessment battery (FAB): Normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol. Sci. 26, 108–116. doi: 10.1007/s10072-005-0443-4
Crutch, S. J., Schott, J. M., Rabinovici, G. D., Murray, M., Snowden, J. S., van der Flier, W. M., et al. (2017). Consensus classification of posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimers Dement. 13, 870–884. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.014
Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., and Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology 55, 1621–1626. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621
Gobom, J., Parnetti, L., Rosa-Neto, P., Vyhnalek, M., Gauthier, S., Cataldi, S., et al. (2022). Validation of the LUMIPULSE automated immunoassay for the measurement of core AD biomarkers in cerebrospinal fluid. Clin. Chem. Lab. Med. 60, 207–219.
Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Hillis, A. E., Weintraub, S., Kertesz, A., Mendez, M., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2011). Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 76, 1006–1014. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6
Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., and Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: A systematic review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:437. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437
Hurtado-Pomares, M., Carmen Terol-Cantero, M., Sánchez-Pérez, A., Peral-Gómez, P., Valera-Gran, D., and Navarrete-Muñoz, E. M. (2018). The frontal assessment battery in clinical practice: A systematic review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 33, 237–251. doi: 10.1002/gps.4751
Ikezaki, H., Hashimoto, M., Ishikawa, T., Fukuhara, R., Tanaka, H., Yuki, S., et al. (2020). Relationship between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric symptoms and impaired instrumental activities of daily living among patients with very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 35, 877–887. doi: 10.1002/gps.5308
Jack, C. R. Jr., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., et al. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 14, 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018
Jung, Y. H., Park, S., Jang, H., Cho, S. H., Kim, S. J., Kim, J. P., et al. (2020). Frontal-executive dysfunction affects dementia conversion in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Sci. Rep. 10:772. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-57525-6
Kim, J. W., Lee, D. Y., Seo, E. H., Sohn, B. K., Choe, Y. M., Kim, S. G., et al. (2014). Improvement of screening accuracy of mini-mental state examination for mild cognitive impairment and non-Alzheimer’s disease dementia by supplementation of verbal fluency performance. Psychiatry Investig. 11:44. doi: 10.4306/pi.2014.11.1.44
Kirova, A. M., Bays, R. B., and Lagalwar, S. (2015). Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Biomed Res. Int. 2015:748212. doi: 10.1155/2015/748212
Kume, K., Hanyu, H., Murakami, M., Sato, T., Hirao, K., Kanetaka, H., et al. (2011). Frontal assessment battery and brain perfusion images in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 11, 77–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2010.00645.x
Marshall, G. A., Rentz, D. M., Frey, M. T., Locascio, J. J., Johnson, K. A., Sperling, R. A., et al. (2011). Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 300–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.04.005
McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
Measso, G., Cavarzeran, F., Zappalà, G., Lebowitz, B. D., Crook, T. H., Pirozzolo, F. J., et al. (1993). The mini-mental state examination: Normative study of an Italian random sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 9, 77–85. doi: 10.1080/87565649109540545
Midi, H., Sarkar, S. K., and Rana, S. (2010). Collinearity diagnostics of binary logistic regression model. J. Interdiscipl. Math. 13, 253–267. doi: 10.1080/09720502.2010.10700699
Mitchell, A. J. (2017). “The mini-mental state examination (MMSE): Update on its diagnostic accuracy and clinical utility for cognitive disorders,” in Cognitive screening instruments, ed. A. J. Larner (Cham: Springer), 37–48. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-44775-9_3
Nagata, T., Shinagawa, S., Ochiai, Y., Aoki, R., Kasahara, H., Nukariya, K., et al. (2011). Association between executive dysfunction and hippocampal volume in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatr. 23, 764–771. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210002164
Oshima, E., Terada, S., Sato, S., Ikeda, C., Nagao, S., Takeda, N., et al. (2012). Frontal assessment battery and brain perfusion imaging in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, 994–1001. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211002481
Ossenkoppele, R., Pijnenburg, Y. A., Perry, D. C., Cohn-Sheehy, B. I, Scheltens, N. M., Vogel, J. W., et al. (2015). The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer’s disease: Clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. Brain 138, 2732–2749. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv191
Palmqvist, S., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., Vestberg, S., Andreasson, U., Brooks, D. J., et al. (2014). Accuracy of brain amyloid detection in clinical practice using cerebrospinal fluid β-amyloid 42: A cross-validation study against amyloid positron emission tomography. JAMA Neurol. 71, 1282–1289. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1358
Reinvang, I., Grambaite, R., and Espeseth, T. (2012). Executive dysfunction in MCI: Subtype or early symptom. Int. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2012:936272. doi: 10.1155/2012/936272
Robin, X., Turck, N., Hainard, A., Tiberti, N., Lisacek, F., Sanchez, J. C., et al. (2011). pROC: An open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics 12:77. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-77
Rozzini, L., Chilovi, B. V., Conti, M., Bertoletti, E., Delrio, I., Trabucchi, M., et al. (2007). Conversion of amnestic mild cognitive impairment to dementia of Alzheimer type is independent to memory deterioration. Int. J. Geriatric. Psychiatry 22, 1217–1222. doi: 10.1002/gps.1816
Sjögren, M., Vanderstichele, H., Ågren, H., Zachrisson, O., Edsbagge, M., Wikkelsø, C., et al. (2001). Tau and Aβ42 in cerebrospinal fluid from healthy adults 21–93 years of age: Establishment of reference values. Clin. Chem. 47, 1776–1781. doi: 10.1093/clinchem/47.10.1776
Vanderstichele, H., De Vreese, K., Blennow, K., Andreasen, N., Sindic, C., Ivanoiu, A., et al. (2006). Analytical performance and clinical utility of the INNOTEST§PHOSPHO-TAU (181P) assay for discrimination between Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Clin. Chem. Lab. Med. 44, 1472–1480. doi: 10.1515/CCLM.2006.258
Keywords: executive functioning, Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, cerebrospinal fluid, frontal assessment battery
Citation: Aiello EN, Verde F, Milone I, Giacopuzzi Grigoli E, Dubini A, Carelli L, Ferrucci R, Priori A, Ratti A, Torresani E, Ticozzi N, Silani V and Poletti B (2022) The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) effectively discriminates between MCI and dementia within the clinical spectrum of neurochemically confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Psychol. 13:1054321. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1054321
Received: 26 September 2022; Accepted: 24 October 2022;
Published: 30 November 2022.
Edited by:
Silvia Paola Caminiti, San Raffaele Scientific Institute (IRCCS), ItalyReviewed by:
Enrico Premi, University of Brescia, ItalyAntonio Carotenuto, University of Naples Federico II, Italy
Copyright © 2022 Aiello, Verde, Milone, Giacopuzzi Grigoli, Dubini, Carelli, Ferrucci, Priori, Ratti, Torresani, Ticozzi, Silani and Poletti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Barbara Poletti, Yi5wb2xldHRpQGF1eG9sb2dpY28uaXQ=; orcid.org/0000-0003-4398-2051
†These authors have contributed equally to this work