Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 January 2023
Sec. Educational Psychology

Well-being and mathematics achievement: What is the role of gender, instructional clarity, and parental involvement?

  • 1Department of Educational Psychology, Faculty of Education, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
  • 2Africa Unit for Transdisciplinary Health Research, Faculty of Health Sciences, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa
  • 3Inclusive Economic Development, Human Sciences Research Council, Durban, South Africa

Introduction: The aim of the present study was to explore the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement among a nationally representative sample of Grade 9 learners in South Africa.

Methods: Using the Trends in Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS 2019) based on a sample of 20,829 learners (females = 11,067 and males = 9,719), and employing structural equation modelling (SEM), we examined the nature of the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement, considering the role of gender, parental involvement, and instructional clarity.

Results: Our findings showed that satisfaction with life is positively related to mathematics achievement, but is not moderated by gender. Additionally, instructional clarity contributes to, and is a partial mediator of, the relationship between life satisfaction and mathematics achievement. This suggests that greater instructional clarity is positively associated with high achievement in mathematics, over and above the relationship with satisfaction with life. By contrast, parental involvement negatively mediates this relationship, suggesting that mathematics achievement is negatively associated with certain forms of parental involvement, such as setting time aside for homework; and checking if homework is done.

Discussion: Given the tested linear relationship between life satisfaction, instructional clarity and mathematics achievement, the results of the study suggest that if wellbeing is improved and existing instructional practices are constantly reviewed, mathematics scores could be positively affected. In addition, the emerging finding on the negative role of parental involvement in the hypothesised mediated relationship suggests that learners could benefit from properly tailored, and government-sponsored, afterschool tutoring.

Introduction

In South Africa, the focus of education research has been on academic achievement and how home, school and classroom factors affect achievement—without detailed consideration of how individual wellbeing may play a role. The current evidence on predictors of academic achievement in South Africa also focuses primarily on self-efficacy (Juan et al., 2018), learning experiences (Visser et al., 2019), socio-economic status (SES) and school resources (Juan and Visser, 2017), among others. However, for the first time, as part of the Trends in Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) national dataset, data on subjective wellbeing indicators (satisfaction with life, one indicator of wellbeing) was gathered from Grade 9 learners to investigate the relationship between a wellbeing indicator and mathematics achievement. To further understand the nature of this relationship, the current study asks for whom this relationship is significant (males or females) and under what external circumstances (i.e., instructional clarity and parental involvement). The latter are key factors in the learning process and they cover both what happens at school and home.

Previous studies have painted an inconsistent picture of the relationship between gender and subjective wellbeing- defined as the satisfactory evaluation of life, presence of positive emotion and absence of negative affect (Wilson and Somhlaba, 2018). While some studies found no gender differences (Batz and Tay, 2018), others have pointed to females experiencing greater subjective wellbeing (Graham and Chattopadhyay, 2013; Tay et al., 2014). With respect to the lack of gender differences, Batz and Tay (2018) opined that men and women adapt to their surroundings and habitual conditions of living (for instance inequality), making differences in wellbeing either small or non-existent (see also Hyde, 2005). Females experiencing more subjective wellbeing may be linked to social norms that allow women to be more emotionally expressive whether positive or negative emotions (Simon and Nath, 2004).

Furthermore, in South Africa, and some parts of the world, females tend to experience less favourable objective conditions of wellbeing (such as, material resources), which could have implications for their subjective wellbeing and associated life domains (Joshanloo and Jovanović, 2020), including academic achievement. Given the inconsistent findings on gender and wellbeing, it will be interesting to understand the extent to which gender might moderate the relationship between subjective wellbeing and mathematics achievement. Previous research has also shown that school characteristics and parental involvement are important for learners’ educational achievement (Bryce et al., 2019). Such a relationship is built on the premise that parents and teachers could provide different forms of support to enable learning. Therefore, in this study, we also seek to address the question of which (gender), and under what circumstances wellbeing might be beneficial for mathematics achievement in our attempt to explore the potential benefits (if any) of promoting wellbeing in South African schools.

Subjective wellbeing and academic achievement

Both subjective wellbeing and academic achievement are indicators of positive psychological functioning (Suldo et al., 2006), and, according to the OECD (2017), these are variables found in a high-performing education system. Successful students not only perform well academically, they are also satisfied at school, probably owing to a mutually reinforcing relationship (OECD, 2017). That is, when students are well, they tend to succeed academically and vice versa (Zakaria and Abdul Halim, 2017). Support for this statement is underscored by schools being not only a place for academic learning, but also for social interaction, personality development and becoming acquainted with larger society (Bücker et al., 2018). Subjective wellbeing and academic achievement constitute the essence of educational endeavours and are mutually dependent.

Following the finding on subjective wellbeing predicting academic performance among a sample of Mexican students (Ojeda et al., 2011; Whitley et al., 2012), the authors argued that positive wellbeing experiences expand psychological resources, such as the intellectual ability needed for academic success. More so, greater subjective wellbeing might be linked to higher levels of vigour in school activities, and positive emotions while completing academic tasks, which in turn might lead to excelling in academic endeavours (see Cohn and Fredrickson, 2009). Another explanation for the relationship between satisfaction with life and academic achievement is that experiencing such optimal psychological state enables learners to broaden their thought-action repertoire, which is deemed essential for acquiring durable academic resources (Fredrickson, 2013).

Among a group of undergraduate Filipino students, wellbeing predicted objective academic achievement (after controlling for relevant demographic variables) (Datu, 2018). This is in line with the positive education paradigm (a dual emphasis on wellbeing and achievement in schools), as it posits that wellbeing is beneficial because it cultivates different indicators of academic success. Similarly, in a group of USA students, Heffner and Antaramian (2016) found that greater life satisfaction predicted increased academic achievement among 7th and 8th Graders at an average age of 13 years. In contrast to Heffner and Antaramian (2016), a meta-analysis of 47 studies including 151 effect sizes, Bücker et al. (2018) showed that low-achieving learners do not necessarily have lowered subjective wellbeing, and learners scoring low on wellbeing were not necessarily low achieving. However, they did find small to moderate significant correlations between academic achievement and subjective wellbeing. Despite the relatively weak degree of association between wellbeing and academic outcomes, Noftle and Robins (2007) note that even empirical investigations with small effect sizes may have valuable practical impact, because of the importance attached to experiencing wellbeing. The claims of inconsistent findings (see Bücker et al., 2018) in the relationship between subjective wellbeing and academic achievement warrant further exploration (Heffner and Antaramian, 2016).

Gender, subjective wellbeing, and academic achievement

Evidence on gender differences in subjective wellbeing (Bradshaw et al., 2011) is also unclear for a number of reasons. For example, whereas some studies have supported the notion that females have a higher tendency to report happiness (Tomyn and Cummins, 2011; Casas et al., 2013; Cummins, 2014), the converse has been found in studies such as by Lyons et al. (2014) that males have greater wellbeing. Still others (Proctor et al., 2009) suggest that there are no gender differences. One of the reasons for this inconsistency is the use of multidimensional versus general overall satisfaction with life measures (Dinisman and Ben-Arieh, 2016), where single-item measures shows no difference but multidimensional assessment tools demonstrates some differences because males and female might have unique experiences when it comes certain domains of wellbeing. For instance, females might do better on satisfaction with interpersonal relationships (Casas et al., 2013).

Another factor that may explain the lack of a consistent pattern between subjective wellbeing and gender is the different gender roles (Eagly and Karau, 2002). That is, expectations and norms in gender roles influence the perceptions regarding the appropriate occupations for men and women, the type of traits men and women should possess, and even the type of emotions that are acceptable to experience and demonstrate for men and women (Eagly and Karau, 2002), all of which can impact on wellbeing. Meisenberg and Woodley (2015) contend that socio-cultural conditions moderate the correlation between gender and life satisfaction. Specifically, the African tradition and culture have socialised boys and girls into gender roles which might have implications for wellbeing.

In a similar vein, males and females may have different educational experiences. Regarding academic achievement, a meta-analysis by Voyer and Voyer (2014) found that female students generally earn higher grades than male students. In South Africa, a similar finding of female nursing students academically outperforming males emerged in a systematic review (Mthimunye and Daniels, 2019). One explanation offered for this trend is the tendency for parents to encourage females to exert greater effort in their studies, while assuming that males do not require extra assistance to succeed in their academics (Voyer and Voyer, 2014). Research by Varner and Mandara (2014), for example, confirmed this suggestion among a sample of African American adolescents, concluding that “reducing gaps in parenting may help reduce the gender gap in achievement (p. 12).”

Pertaining to the possible moderating role of gender, in the meta-analyses by Bücker et al. (2018), gender played an insignificant role in the relationship between subjective wellbeing and academic achievement. Similarly, Cadime et al. (2016) found no gender difference in the relationship between academic achievement and wellbeing. It was suggested that because children and young adults spend a substantial amount of their time at school, students’ overall life satisfaction and their academic satisfaction might be highly overlapping and not as distinct as expected (Bücker et al., 2018).

On the one hand, other evidence has however shown that existing gender difference in this relationship might be attributed to internal distress that girls may experience, while evaluating themselves more negatively than boys, and being more prone than boys to worry about their performance at school (Pomerantz et al., 2002). On the other hand, girls doing better than boys may be as a result of them compensating for lower wellbeing with a higher readiness to perform well in school. This phenomenon has been referred to as a higher “conformity of girls towards school requirements” (e.g., Sparfeldt et al., 2009). However, the evidence of the moderating role of gender is still inconclusive.

Role of instructional clarity and parental involvement

The teacher-learner relationship is an important factor of the classroom climate. Instructional clarity refers to the teacher’s ability to explain content clearly to students and to provide clear directions during teaching. Moreover, trusting, respectful and caring interactions between the teacher and learner provide the emotional and intellectual context for engagement and related educational outcomes (Ryan and Deci, 2002; Guthrie et al., 2006). Previous reviews have shown that teachers’ instructional practices may have an impact on students’ learning and may be more important than class size and classroom climate—even more important than the teacher’s years of experience and formal training (Seidel and Shavelson, 2007; Timperley and Alton-Lee, 2008; Konstantopoulos and Chung, 2011). It is hence important for the teacher to adopt instructional tools and techniques that guarantee students’ clear understanding of tasks (Arends, 2021).

Instructional practices in mathematics were found to predict achievement among upper and middle elementary learners in the USA (Kane and Staiger, 2012; Blazar and Kraft, 2017). Yagan (2021), for example, found a significant relationship between teachers’ classroom management and instructional clarity skills on the one hand, and students’ mathematics achievement and attitude toward mathematics on the other. These studies support the notion that quality teaching practices have the potential to positively influence achievement (Fischer and Neumann, 2012), especially in contexts where minimal conditions for learning are in place. Additionally, teacher clarity has been found to influence positive outcomes with regard to students’ academic motivation and critical thinking (Loes and Pascarella, 2015), as well as their affective learning (Chesebro and McCroskey, 2001; Titsworth et al., 2015).

The relationship between instructional clarity and mathematics achievement is underscored by a number of factors. First, positive teaching and learning practices are argued to increase academic achievement because they facilitate learning and engagement (Osher and Kendziora, 2010; Thapa et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2017). Second, the degree of explicitness, clarity of learning goals, and content-oriented instructions (when in place) provide a clear pathway for learning to occur (Klette et al., 2017). Third, high teacher expectations provide motivation for learners to exert the necessary effort at ensuring improved achievement (Graham et al., 2017). Despite the evidence provided, not much of this has been replicated in the South African context; hence this study.

Education researchers have always expressed interest in the relationship between parental involvement and academic achievement (Fan and Chen, 2001). Parents involved in a learner’s education contribute to their child’s social, emotional, and intellectual growth (Green et al., 2007). Despite this, some inconsistency still surrounds the existing findings on parental involvement and its association with students’ academic achievement (McNeal, 2012). This is mostly attributed to a lack of conceptual clarification and measurement of parental involvement (Bakker and Denessen, 2007; Wilder, 2014). For instance, parental involvement is usually associated with investment in education, provision of resources (LaRocque et al., 2011) and actual learning support for children by their caregivers. It could also include providing support for education at home and engagement with teachers at school (Comer, 1995).

A systematic review of 75 studies conducted between 2003 and 2017 revealed that several parental involvement indicators, including parental modelling, expectations, encouragement and school involvement, are related to academic achievement (Gubbins and Otero, 2016; Boonk et al., 2018). Parental encouragement, for example, has been shown to foster student academic achievement through letting children know parents care about them and their performance (Rogers et al., 2009). Adding to this, is the provision of an appropriate learning environment at home to support homework and learning (Sheldon and Epstein, 2005; Gonida and Cortina, 2014).

In contrast, Boonk et al. (2018) argue that not all parental involvement fosters academic achievement. Specifically, involvement in homework was found to have either a negative relationship (Lee and Bowen, 2006) or no relationship (Driessen et al., 2005) in American and Dutch samples when parents lacked the requisite skills. Other evidence also indicates that the nature of homework involvement (autonomy support, control, interference, cognitive engagement) determines its relationship with achievement (Gonida and Cortina, 2014). For instance, when parents are trained to assist learners (Tam and Chan, 2009) and there is some level of autonomy (Gonida and Cortina, 2014), then parental involvement positively predicts achievement. In South Africa, according to TIMSS reports (Reddy et al., 2022), (while being cautious of high missing values) only 38% of households have at least one parent with a post-secondary education, implying that the benefits of involvement in learners’ education might not be so apparent for the greater sample of learners as a result of the educational level of parents. As a result, this study tests the nature of relationship between parental involvement and mathematics achievement.

The present study

As psychologists increase their use of strengths-based assessments, knowing the role of subjective wellbeing beyond that of other affective states (such as happiness) alongside school functioning may help inform educational practice. In this study, we explore whether this association is moderated by gender and further mediated by parental involvement and instructional clarity. The moderating and mediating variables were introduced because of evidence on their relationship with achievement. We used structural equation modelling (SEM) in this paper because it allows for the measurement of a structural relationship and estimates the multiple and interrelated dependence of variables in a single analysis. This relationship includes the direct relationships between life satisfaction, parental involvement and instructional clarity and mathematics achievement, the moderation by gender and mediation by parental involvement and instructional clarity. Since the technique allows for the measurement of several predictors of mathematics achievement in one model, our study will be a step in the direction of attempting to promote academic achievement alongside subjective assessments of the quality of life of South African learners.

Research hypothesis

1. There will be a direct positive relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement.

2. Gender will moderate the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement.

3. Parental involvement and instructional clarity is a positive predictor of mathematics achievement.

4. Parental involvement and instructional clarity mediate the relationship between satisfaction with life and achievement in mathematics.

Methodological approach

The TIMSS 2019 assessment was its seventh cycle in South Africa. The cycles have been conducted every 4 years since 1995. To inform educational policy in the participating countries, TIMSS also collects extensive background information on the home and school contexts in which teaching and learning take place. This background information is collected through a series of questionnaires for learners, parents, mathematics and science educators, school principals and curriculum specialists.

South African learners who participated in TIMSS 2019 completed a paper-based assessment booklet containing an even distribution of both mathematics and science items. These booklets were designed to be administered in two sessions, separated by a short break. Each session was 45 min in duration. In addition to completing the achievement booklet, each learner also completed a background questionnaire.

The Learner Questionnaire asks about aspects of learners’ home and school lives, their home environment, their school climate for learning, and their perceptions of and attitudes toward mathematics and science. Local items about satisfaction of life were included in the Learner Questionnaire specific to South Africa alone.

Sample and participants

To conduct TIMSS in South Africa, a representative sample was drawn from schools offering Grade 9 classes. TIMSS 2019 followed the sampling procedures described in the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA) TIMSS Methods and Procedures Manual (Martin et al., 2020). In the two-stage stratified cluster sampling design, schools were randomly selected at the first stage and an intact Grade 9 class was selected at the second stage.

A total of 524 schools were selected for the study, of which 520 participated. This sample is higher than in most countries, as South Africa over-sampled schools in two provinces, Gauteng and Western Cape, to be able to better estimate the provincial achievement scores (Reddy et al., 2022).

This sample included 20,829 learners (females = 11,067 and males = 9,719), with a higher number of learners from the Gauteng and Western Cape Provinces. Weights allocated to each learner ensured that the national sample was representative of the Grade 9 South African population.

Data gathering and ethical considerations

The main survey was administered by an external data collection agency with relevant qualifications and experience in the field of data collection. Grade 9 learners took part in the assessment in August 2019. The Human Sciences Research Council (HSRC) Ethics Committee approved the study (REC/4/16/03/11).

Instruments

In this study, the following items are included from the Grade 9 TIMSS 2019 dataset: Satisfaction with life, mathematics achievement, instructional clarity, parental involvement, and gender.

Satisfaction with life

In order to measure subjective wellbeing, four items on the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) were adapted and used (Diener et al., 1985). Items used to compute this index included satisfaction with school, peers, home, and life as a learner. Items were scored on a scale ranging from 1 = Highly dissatisfied to 4 = Highly satisfied. Higher scores indicated greater satisfaction with life.

Mathematics achievement

The value for achievement was computed using the IEA IDB analyser, combining five plausible values. Plausible values are not intended to be estimates of individual student scores. Instead, they are imputed scores for students with similar response patterns and background characteristics in the sampled population that may be used to estimate population characteristics correctly. However, the limitation is that they are generally biased estimates of the proficiencies of the individuals with whom they are associated. Thus, taking the average of the plausible values will still not yield suitable estimates of individual student scores (Martin et al., 2016). On the other hand, using plausible values in analysis provides unbiased estimates of the student population.

Instructional clarity in mathematics

To measure instructional clarity, some of the items reported by learners were: understanding what the teacher expects; teacher is easy to understand; teacher explains again. Items on the scales were scored as 1 = Agree a lot to 4 = Disagree a lot. In order to ensure that greater scores reflect the dimension being measured, we had to reverse score the items.

Parental involvement

This scale measured the extent to which parents are involved in learner’s schoolwork. Examples of questions on this scale included talking about schoolwork; setting time aside for homework; and checking if homework is done. Items on this scale were scored from 1 = Every or almost every day to 3 = Never or almost never. Items were reverse scored to ensure that greater values reflected greater parental involvement. This was with the exception of the item “Homework is too difficult for parents,” which did not need to be recoded.

Data analysis

The IEA IDB analyser was used in conjunction with the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (IBM, SPSS) to compute descriptive statistics and correlations. Mplus software (Muthén and Muthén, 1998-2019) was also used for analysis related to structural equation modelling. Descriptive statistics, including means and standard deviation of each variable, were computed and a summary is available in Table 1. Next, inter-correlations across the variables were performed as this is a prerequisite for structural equation modelling. Cronbach alphas were also computed to assess the reliability of the composite measures of satisfaction with life, parental involvement, and instructional clarity (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of correlations and means by gender.

Two steps were taken to determine whether the observed data fit the model. First, a measurement model was used to determine the extent to which the indicators loaded strongly on their respective latent variables (satisfaction with life; instructional clarity; parental involvement; see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediated structural model. Sat, satisfaction with life; InsClar, instructional clarity; Maths 5, 5th plausible value for mathematics achievement; ParInv, parental involvement.

This was followed by testing the direct relationships between mathematics achievement and each of the latent variables—life satisfaction, instructional practices and parental involvement—within a structural model (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conceptual model of the potential mediating effects of instructional clarity and parental involvement on the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. ParInv, parental Involvement; InsClar, instructional clarity; Sat, satisfaction with life; Maths, mathematics achievement.

In addition to the chi-square value (χ2-value), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardised root mean square residual (SRMR) were reported. The criteria for determining if a model has good fit indices were determined by the following cut-off points: CFI and TLI values above 0.90 indicated reasonable model fit and values above 0.95 indicated good model fit. For the RMSEA and SRMR, values smaller than 0.08 indicated reasonable model fit, and values below 0.05 good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Byrne, 2012). In addition to model fit indices, the direct and the path coefficients of the hypothesised relationships were provided. The ML estimator was used in MPLUS.

Moderation analysis

Following the testing of the direct relationships, a structural model comprising satisfaction with life and achievement, as moderated by gender, was tested using the Wald Test of Parameter of Constraints (see Figures 3, 4). A moderating factor may strengthen, diminish, negate, or otherwise alter the association between independent and dependent variables. Moderation analysis was performed using the multigroup approach with latent and observed variables. A statistically significant p-value of the Wald test indicates moderation as well as a relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Conceptual model of the potential moderating effect of gender on the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. Sat, satisfaction with life; Maths, mathematics achievement.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Statistical model of the potential moderating effect of gender on the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. Sat, satisfaction with life; Maths, mathematics achievement.

Mediation analysis

We also tested a mediated model comprising instructional clarity and parental involvement. A mediated model seeks to identify and explain the mechanism that underlies an observed relationship between the dependent variable (achievement) and the independent variable (satisfaction with life) through a third variable (instructional clarity and parental involvement). To perform the mediation analysis, it is a requirement that the relationships between satisfaction with life and mediator (parental involvement and instructional clarity) (a′), the relationship between the mediator and mathematics achievement (b′), and the direct relationship between the life satisfaction and mathematics achievement (c′) are statistically significant (see Figure 5). Furthermore, the indirect relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement was estimated to determine the level of mediation. Full mediation is indicated by a statistically non-significant relationship between the independent and the dependent variable before the mediator is introduced. However, should this relationship be statistically significant after the mediator was introduced and if the direct effect (c′) was reduced, there is only partial mediation. Ten thousand bias-corrected bootstrapping samples with a 95% confidence interval (CI) were applied. This was done to increase the validity of the findings.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Statistical model of the potential mediating effects of instructional clarity and parental involvement on the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. ParInv, parental Involvement; InsClar, instructional clarity; Sat, satisfaction with life; Maths, mathematics achievement.

Results

Correlations

Our findings show that mathematics achievement was significantly positively related to both satisfaction with life and instructional clarity (Table 1). However, a negative relationship emerged between mathematics achievement and parental involvement (over 80% of learners reported that parents supported them with homework showing very little variance in the data). Satisfaction with life was positively related to instructional clarity (r = 0.17) but negatively to parental involvement (r = -0.27). See a summary of correlations in Table 1. Satisfaction with life, instructional clarity and parental involvement all had reasonable reliability coefficients.

Measurement model

We tested a unidimensional model for each of the three variables: satisfaction with life, parental involvement, and instructional clarity. Satisfaction with life, which was adapted from the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985), emerged with a perfect fit index showing that the hypothesised model fits the data or sample used.

The measurement models for satisfaction with life (CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.00) and instructional clarity (CFI = 0.970; RMSEA = 0.053) displayed good model fit. The CFI and RMSEA values for parental involvement (CFI = 0.934; RMSEA = 0.080) were reasonable, while the TLI (0.890) and SRMR (0.098) values were unsatisfactory and indicated that the hypothesised model did not fit our sample. This implies that the hypothesised measurement model does not accurately reflect the responses from of the sample. A high modification index (MI = 272.192) indicated a high correlation between the residual terms of items (“Homework is too difficult for parents” and “Homework is in a language parent understand”). The residual terms of these items were allowed to correlate, which produced a model with good fit (CFI = 0.976; RMSEA = 0.051). The fit indices of the measurement models are presented in Table 2, showing that the hypothesised model fits the data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of fit indices of measurement and structural models.

Structural model

The model fit statistics for all the structural models emerged with good fit indices (see Table 2). For mathematics achievement, the simple path coefficients (a1, b1, a2, b2, and c’) (Table 3) were statistically significant (p < 0.05), indicating a significant relationship with life satisfaction, parental involvement and instructional clarity. The results of the moderation model pointed to a lack of gender differences in the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. All indirect effects from 10,000 bootstrapping samples were also statistically significant, with the bootstrapping 95% confidence interval not including zero to improve confidence in the results of the model (see Table 5). Considering that the strength of the relationship between satisfaction and mathematics achievement was reduced when instructional clarity and parental involvement were introduced into the relationship, and further considering that these relationships were still statistically significant, we conclude that the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement was only partially mediated by instructional clarity and parental involvement, respectively. This suggests instructional clarity and parental involvement are factors contributing to the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. A breakdown of the path coefficients is presented in Table 3.1

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of direct effects.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the moderation analysis for plausible value (PV) 1 mathematics achievement.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardised direct and indirect effects.

Satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement

Our first research hypothesis aimed to test the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement (see Table 3). We found that there was a significant positive relationship (β = 0.103, p < 0.001) after computing the average coefficient, standard error and t-value across the five plausible values for mathematics achievement. This means that increased satisfaction with life is linked to better mathematics achievement.

Parental involvement and mathematics achievement

In addition to the direct relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement, we tested the role of parental involvement. We assessed the extent to which parental involvement is related to achievement. Our findings revealed a significant negative relationship (β = -0.26, p < 0.001) between parents’ involvement in their child’s schoolwork and mathematics achievement, suggesting that parental involvement did not result in better mathematics scores. This finding might be attributable to the lack of variance in parental involvement as 85% of learners indicated that parents were involved in their homework.

Instructional clarity and mathematics achievement

Given the role of teachers in achievement of learners, we tested the relationship between instructional clarity and mathematics achievement. Emerging from the analysis was a positive relationship (β = 0.057, p < 0.001) between instructional clarity and mathematics achievement, indicating that greater scores in instructional clarity was linked to better mathematics achievement scores.

Moderation by gender

We also examined whether the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement may be moderated by gender. Our findings showed that the p-value of the Wald test was statistically non-significant in each instance, implying this relationship was not statistically significant (p < 0.05). That is, males and females do not differ in the relationship between satisfaction with life and achievement. The results of the moderation analysis are presented in Table 4.

Indirect effects model

Our indirect effects model tested the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement with instructional clarity and parental achievement as mediators. The overall model showed good fit statistics (see Table 2). The hypothesised indirect model, with instructional clarity as the mediator, was also supported by significant statistics (β = 0.484, p < 0.001). Significant findings emerged for parental involvement, although they suggested a negative mediated relationship (β = -0.022, p < 0.001) (see Table 5 and Figure 5). This implies that both instructional clarity and parental involvement affect the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. While instructional clarity further contributes to an increase in mathematics achievement scores, parental involvement is linked to reduced scores.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to determine the nature of the relationships between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement among South African Grade 9 learners. To further explore this issue, we tested the extent to which gender might moderate this relationship, given that males and females might differ in achievement as well as wellbeing levels. Keeping in mind the likely influence of external factors, such as parental involvement and instructional clarity, we tested the mediating role of certain aspects of school and home in the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. Our findings showed that all three models—direct effects, moderated by gender and indirect effects—had good model fit statistics. Regarding the structural relations among the variables, satisfaction with life contributed to mathematics achievement in South Africa, with such relationships being further associated with parental involvement in learning as well as instructional clarity in schools. These findings are discussed further in accordance with the stipulated research questions.

Direct relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement

We explored the extent to which subjective wellbeing—measured as satisfaction with life—is related to mathematics achievement. We found that among Grade 9 South African learners, greater achievement in mathematics is associated with learners’ more positive satisfaction with life. Similar to the present study, a number of studies in Europe and the USA (Suldo et al., 2006; OECD, 2017; Bücker et al., 2018) argue that wellbeing and achievement are indicators of positive psychological functioning because successful students tend to also be satisfied with their lives. Supporting this argument is the notion that schools are an environment for holistic development, over and beyond academic achievement. More so, this relationship between satisfaction with life and achievement is underscored by the fact that wellbeing experiences provide more psychological resources, such as creativity and the intellectual abilities needed for greater achievement (Yang et al., 2014; Ng et al., 2015; Heffner and Antaramian, 2016). Our findings also advocate for a dual focus on wellbeing and achievement as important outcomes for schools, given that improved wellbeing is linked to increased achievement (White and Kern, 2018; Seligman and Adler, 2018).

Previous research in South Africa used proxies of wellbeing, including self-efficacy (Van der Westhuizen, 2013) and enjoyment of science (Juan et al., 2018) as predictors of achievement. The emerging relationship between satisfaction with life and academic achievement among South African learners points to the need to prioritise wellbeing in educational interventions and policies. One of the persistent challenges in South Africa has been to improve learners’ mathematics scores. To achieve this, we suggest learning support programmes that could include wellbeing interventions aimed at improving satisfaction with life across domains of school and home. In our measurement of satisfaction with life, we considered the home, peers, school and life as a learner to provide a multidimensional assessment of satisfaction with life and how this relates to mathematics achievement. These domains provide suggestions on how interventions could be tailored within the context of South Africa.

Indirect effects model

Satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement mediated by parental involvement

In addition to exploring the direct relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement, we further tested the role of parental involvement in this relationship. The indirect path showed a significant relationship, albeit negative, between mathematics achievement and satisfaction with life. This implies that parental involvement negatively impacts or reduces the positive effect of satisfaction with life on mathematics achievement. A systematic review by Boonk et al. (2018) pointed to inconsistent findings on the role of parental involvement in promoting mathematics achievement, due to the varying conceptualisations of this construct. In line with the present study, while reading at home and parental engagement in learning activities were found to improve achievement (Manolitsis et al., 2013; Crosby et al., 2015), involvement in homework did not have a significant relationship with achievement (e.g., Driessen et al., 2005) or was negatively related to achievement (Domina, 2005; Lee and Bowen, 2006; Rogers et al., 2009).

Providing support for studies indicating negative or no relationship between parental involvement and mathematics achievement, our work among Grade 9 South African learners suggests that parental involvement directed at asking about homework holds less benefit for achievement, even with the existing positive relationship between satisfaction with life and achievement. It is possible that the socio-economic landscape of South Africa and the likelihood of most parents being without the requisite skills and resources will cause parental involvement to have a negative effect on the relationship between wellbeing and academic achievement. Using the same TIMSS dataset, Reddy et al. (2022) posit that only 38% of learners reported that one parent has post-secondary school education. It was also reported that 63% of learners had parents who did not understand the language of the home work, while 65% struggled with the content, most likely explaining the emerging negative relationship. Learners whose parents did not struggle with providing homework support were more likely to have higher achievement scores (Reddy et al., 2022). Following the argument by Tam and Chan (2009), parents need to have the requisite skills in order for their involvement in learners’ schoolwork to have a positive impact. Moreover, Grade 9’s are more likely to require specialised support with their homework for any assistance to have the intended outcome of academic success.

Satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement mediated by instructional clarity

Given the importance of teacher-learner relationships for academic achievement, we also tested the role of instructional clarity in the association between mathematics achievement and satisfaction with life. We found that instructional clarity partially mediated the hypothesised relationship. This means that the relationship between life satisfaction and mathematics achievement can be partially attributed to the extent to which learners perceive their teacher’s instruction as clear and meaningful. This finding points to how individual-level characteristics (satisfaction with life), together with the situational factor of instructional clarity, predict mathematics achievement. For Grade 9 learners (in the context of the study), evaluating their lives positively and experiencing clarity of teaching in mathematics have the potential of improving academic achievement.

The importance of instructional clarity is emphasised in the fostering of learning practices that increase engagement among learners (Thapa et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2017). Further explanation of the emerging relationship is that clear instruction enables learners to plan more effectively, set goals and acquire a stronger sense of how to judge their own progress—thus fostering achievement. Clarity also allows learners to process course information in elaborate ways that might improve academic outcomes (Bolkan et al., 2016). The mediating role of instructional clarity is further support for positive education as an approach toward enhancing academic achievement while fostering the wellbeing of the learners. Instructional clarity not only addresses the issue of cognitive load arising from the appropriate design of learning materials, it also has the potential of ensuring engagement among learners, which is one of the core aims of positive education (Seligman and Adler, 2018).

Limitations of the study

Our study has a number of positive implications but is not without limitations. One common limitation of cross-sectional data is the inability to draw causal conclusions. As much as we can argue for the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement, there has been evidence for the reverse relationship. Hence, a longitudinal study is still needed to determine the causal directionality of the hypothesised relationship.

In addition, the current study only explored the moderating role of gender and did not control for other confounding variables including SES, race, or province of origin and a host of school-level variables, all of which might influence results. Although beyond the scope of the study, we wonder whether qualitative assessment of satisfaction with life would not provide better information on what aspects of subjective wellbeing require policy-related interventions.

Practical implications

This study has some practical and research implications for education and wellbeing studies, both locally and internationally. Firstly, our work suggests that fostering subjective wellbeing at schools is worthwhile in developing economies like South Africa, because of its relationship with mathematics achievement. Secondly, we have demonstrated that both the school and home contribute to the relationship between subjective wellbeing and mathematics achievement. Specifically, our findings suggest that instructional practices in South African schools need to be improved by education stakeholders by making the instructional process more and more suitable to learning needs if better achievement scores in mathematics are expected. We also show how such improvements in instructional practices could be accompanied by better wellbeing of learners.

It would seem that certain kinds of parental involvement do not relate to improvement in mathematics achievement. This is most likely in low-income contexts including South Africa, where most parents might not have adequate knowledge and expertise to properly support learners in their school work. As a result, additional governmental assistance might be needed in terms of after-school tutoring to improve achievement in mathematics to supplement parental support.

Conclusion

The aim of the present study was to explore the nature of the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement among Grade 9 South African learners. We also tested the roles of gender, instructional clarity and parental involvement in this relationship. The results of the structural equation modelling showed that satisfaction with life is a predictor of mathematics achievement. This relationship is further enhanced by instructional clarity in the teaching of mathematics. However, parental involvement that does not contribute quantitatively to the learner’s schoolwork emerged as a negative mediator of the relationship between satisfaction with life and mathematics achievement. Subjective wellbeing is important for achievement but must be accompanied by appropriate support from the home and school.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://www.timss-sa.org/dataset/timss-2019-grade-9-learner-and-school-context-data.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Human Sciences Research Council Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants or their legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

AWF: conceptualisation, analysis, methodology, and prepared the original draft. VR: principal investigator for TIMSS 2019 in South Africa, project administration, review, and editing. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study emerged from a TIMSS Working Paper, which was funded by the Human Sciences Research Council.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Note that the results for only one plausible value of mathematics achievement are presented in all the tables, instead of the fit indices for each of the five plausible values of mathematics achievement, given that all the fit statistics and coefficients reflect similar findings.

References

Arends, F. (2021). Help them understand: the importance of instructional clarity in teaching and learning. HSRC Rev. 19, 33–34.

Google Scholar

Bakker, J. T. A., and Denessen, E. J. P. G. (2007). The concept of parental involvement: Some theoretical and empirical considerations. Int. J. Parents Educ. 1, 188–199.

Google Scholar

Batz, C., and Tay, L. (2018). “Gender differences in subjective well-being,” in Handbook of well-being, eds E. Diener, S. Oishi, and L. Tay (Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers).

Google Scholar

Blazar, D., and Kraft, M. A. (2017). Teacher and teaching effects on students’ attitudes and behaviors. Educ. Eval. Policy Anal. 39, 146–170. doi: 10.3102/0162373716670260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolkan, S., Goodboy, A. K., and Kelsey, D. M. (2016). Instructor clarity and student motivation: Academic performance as a product of students’ ability and motivation to process instructional material. Commun. Educ. 65, 129–148. doi: 10.1080/03634523.2015.1079329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonk, L., Gijselaers, H. J., Ritzen, H., and Brand-Gruwel, S. (2018). A review of the relationship between parental involvement indicators and academic achievement. Educ. Res. Rev. 24, 10–30. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J., Keung, A., Rees, G., and Goswami, H. (2011). Children’s subjective well-being: International comparative perspectives. Children Youth Servic. Rev. 33, 548–556. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryce, C. I., Bradley, R. H., Abry, T., Swanson, J., and Thompson, M. S. (2019). Parents’ and teachers’ academic influences, behavioural engagement, and first- and fifth-grade achievement. School Psychol. 34, 492–502. doi: 10.1037/spq0000297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bücker, S., Nuraydin, S., Simonsmeier, B. A., Schneider, M., and Luhmann, M. (2018). Subjective well-being and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. J. Res. Pers. 74, 83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural Equation Modeling With Mplus: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cadime, I., Pinto, A. M., Lima, S., Rego, S., Pereira, J., and Ribeiro, I. (2016). Well-being and academic achievement in secondary school pupils: The unique effects of burnout and engagement. J. Adolesc. 53, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casas, J. A., Del Rey, R., and Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2013). Bullying and cyberbullying: Convergent and divergent predictor variables. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 580–587. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesebro, J. L., and McCroskey, J. C. (2001). The relationship of teacher clarity and immediacy with student state receiver apprehension, affect, and cognitive learning. Commun. Educ. 50, 59–68.

Google Scholar

Cohn, M. A., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2009). Positive emotions. Oxford Handb. Positive Psychol. 2, 13–24. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comer, D. R. (1995). A model of social loafing in real work groups. Hum. Relat. 48, 647–667. doi: 10.1177/001872679504800603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crosby, S. A., Rasinski, T., Padak, N., and Yildirim, K. (2015). A 3-year study of a school-based parental involvement program in early literacy. J. Educ. Res. 108, 165–172. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2013.867472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A. (2014). “Gender dimensions of life quality for adults in Australia,” in Gender, Lifespan and Quality of Life, ed. E. Eckermann (Dordrecht: Springer), 25–47. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-7829-0_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Datu, J. A. (2018). Flourishing is associated with higher academic achievement and engagement in Filipino undergraduate and high school students. J. Happiness Stud. 19, 27–39. doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9805-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Satisfaction with life scale (SWLS). J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.13072/midss.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinisman, T., and Ben-Arieh, A. (2016). The characteristics of children’s subjective well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 126, 555–569. doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-0921-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domina, T. (2005). Levelling the home advantage: Assessing the effectiveness of parental involvement in elementary school. Sociol. Educ. 78, 233–249. doi: 10.1177/003804070507800303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driessen, G., Smit, F., and Sleegers, P. (2005). Parental involvement and educational achievement. Br. Educ. Res. J. 31, 509–532. doi: 10.1080/01411920500148713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol. Rev. 109:573. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., and Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 13, 1–22. doi: 10.1007/s10964-019-01072-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, H. E., and Neumann, K. (2012). “Video analysis as a tool for understanding science instruction,” in Science Education Research and Practice in Europe, eds D. Jorde and J. Dillon (Rotterdam: SensePublishers), 115–139. doi: 10.1007/978-94-6091-900-8_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 1–53. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-407236-7.00001-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonida, E. N., and Cortina, K. S. (2014). Parental involvement in homework: Relations with parent and student achievement-related motivational beliefs and achievement. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 376–396. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, C., and Chattopadhyay, S. (2013). Gender and well-being around the world. Int. J. Happiness Dev. 1:212. doi: 10.1504/ijhd.2013.055648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., Courtney, L., Marinis, T., and Tonkyn, A. (2017). Early language learning: The impact of teaching and teacher factors. Lang. Learn. 67, 922–958. doi: 10.1111/lang.12251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. L., Walker, J. M., Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., and Sandler, H. M. (2007). Parents’ motivations for involvement in children’s education: An empirical test of a theoretical model of parental involvement. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 532–544. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gubbins, V., and Otero, G. (2016). Effect of the parental involvement style perceived by elementary school students at home on language and mathematics performance in Chilean schools. Educ. Stud. 42, 121–136. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2016.1148586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Humenick, N. M., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., and Barbosa, P. (2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on reading motivation and comprehension. J. Educ. Res. 99, 232–246. doi: 10.3200/joer.99.4.232-246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heffner, A. L., and Antaramian, S. P. (2016). The role of life satisfaction in predicting student engagement and achievement. J. Happiness Stud. 17, 1681–1701. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9665-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. Am. Psychol. 60:581. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshanloo, M., and Jovanović, V. (2020). The relationship between gender and life satisfaction: Analysis across demographic groups and global regions. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 23, 331–338. doi: 10.1007/s00737-019-00998-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juan, A., and Visser, M. (2017). Home and school environmental determinants of science achievement of South African students. South Afr. J. Educ. 37, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Juan, A., Hannan, S., and Namome, C. (2018). I believe I can do science: Self-efficacy and science achievement of Grade 9 students in South Africa. South Afr. J. Sci. 114, 48–54. doi: 10.17159/sajs.2018/20170269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, T. J., and Staiger, D. O. (2012). Gathering Feedback for Teaching: Combining High-Quality Observations with Student Surveys and Achievement Gains. Research Paper. MET Project. Seattle, WA: Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation.

Google Scholar

Klette, K., Blikstad-Balas, M., and Roe, A. (2017). Linking instruction and student achievement. A research design for a new generation of classroom studies. Acta Didact. Norge 11:19. doi: 10.5617/adno.4729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konstantopoulos, S., and Chung, V. (2011). The persistence of teacher effects in elementary grades. Am. Educ. Res. J. 48, 361–386. doi: 10.3102/0002831210382888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaRocque, M., Kleiman, I., and Darling, S. M. (2011). Parental involvement: The missing link in school achievement. Prev. Sch. Fail. Altern. Educ. Child. Youth 55, 115–122. doi: 10.1080/10459880903472876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., and Bowen, N. K. (2006). Parent involvement, cultural capital, and the achievement gap among elementary school children. Am. Educ. Res. J. 43, 193–218. doi: 10.3102/00028312043002193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. Y., Lim, W., Kaur, W. K., Ho, T. L., Toh, B. H., and Choy, H. (2017). “Investigating preservice teachers’ written feedback on procedure-based mathematics assessment items,” in Proceedings of the 41st conference of the international Group for the psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol. 3, (Singapore: PME), 137–144.

Google Scholar

Loes, C. N., and Pascarella, E. T. (2015). The benefits of good teaching extend beyond course achievement. J. Scholar. Teach. Learn. 15, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Lyons, M. D., Otis, K. L., Huebner, E. S., and Hills, K. J. (2014). Life satisfaction and maladaptive behaviors in early adolescents. School Psychol. Q. 29, 553–566. doi: 10.1037/spq0000061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., and Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects of home literacy and numeracy environment on early reading and math acquisition. Early Childh. Res. Q. 28, 692–703. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C. S., Polly, D., Wang, C., Lambert, R. G., and Pugalee, D. K. (2016). Perspectives and practices of elementary teachers using an internet-based formative assessment tool: The case of assessing mathematics concepts. Int. J. Technol. Math. Educ. 23, 3–11. doi: 10.1186/s12913-016-1423-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, M. O., von Davier, M., and Mullis, I. V. (2020). Methods and procedures: TIMSS 2019 Technical Report. Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

Google Scholar

McNeal, R. B. (2012). Checking in or checking out? Investigating the parent involvement reactive hypothesis. J. Educ. Res. 105, 79–89. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2010.519410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meisenberg, G., and Woodley, M. A. (2015). Gender differences in subjective well-being and their relationships with gender equality. J. Happiness Stud. 16, 1539–1555. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9577-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mthimunye, K., and Daniels, F. M. (2019). Predictors of academic performance, success and retention amongst undergraduate nursing students: A systematic review. South Afr. J. High. Educ. 33, 200–220. doi: 10.20853/33-1-2631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muth, L. K., and Muth, B. O. (1998-2019). Mplus (Version 8.3) [Computer Software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Ng, T. P., Jin, A., Feng, L., Nyunt, M. S., Chow, K. Y., Feng, L., et al. (2015). Mortality of older persons living alone: Singapore longitudinal ageing studies. BMC Geriatr. 15:126. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0128-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noftle, E. E., and Robins, R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 116–130. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2017). Students’ Well-Being. PISA 2015 Results, Vol. III., Paris: OECD Publishing, 37–57. doi: 10.1787/9789264273856-en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osher, D., and Kendziora, K. (2010). “Building conditions for learning and healthy adolescent development: Strategic approaches,” in Handbook of youth prevention science, eds B. Doll, W. Pfohl, and J. Koon (New York, NY: Routledge), 121–140.

Google Scholar

Ojeda, L., Flores, L. Y., and Navarro, R. L. (2011). Social cognitive predictors of Mexican American college students’ academic and life satisfaction. J. Counsel. Psychol. 58, 61–71. doi: 10.1037/a0021687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomerantz, E. M., Altermatt, E. R., and Saxon, J. L. (2002). Making the grade but feeling distressed: Gender differences in academic performance and internal distress. J. Educ. Psychol. 94:396. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proctor, C. L., Linley, P. A., and Maltby, J. (2009). Youth life satisfaction: A review of the literature. J. Happiness Stud. 10, 583–630. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddy, V., Winnaar, L., Arends, F., Juan, A., Harvey, J., Hannan, S., et al. (2022). The South African TIMSS 2019 Grade 9 Results: Building Achievement and Bridging Achievement Gaps. Cape Town: HSRC Press.

Google Scholar

Rogers, M. A., Theule, J., Ryan, B. A., Adams, G. R., and Keating, L. (2009). Parental involvement and children’s school achievement. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 24, 34–57. doi: 10.1177/0829573508328445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2002). Overview of self-determination theory: An organismic dialectical perspective. Handb. Self Determin. Res. 2, 3–33. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidel, T., and Shavelson, R. J. (2007). Teaching effectiveness research in the past decade: The role of theory and research design in disentangling meta-analysis results. Rev. Educ. Res. 77, 454–499. doi: 10.3102/0034654307310317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., and Adler, A. (2018). Positive Education. Global Happiness Policy Report. New York, NY: Sustainable Development Solutions Network, 52–73.

Google Scholar

Sheldon, S. B., and Epstein, J. L. (2005). Involvement counts: Family and community partnerships and mathematics achievement. J. Educ. Res. 98, 196–207. doi: 10.3200/joer.98.4.196-207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, R. W., and Nath, L. E. (2004). Gender and emotion in the United States: Do men and women differ in self-reports of feelings and expressive behavior? Am. J. Sociol. 109, 1137–1176. doi: 10.1086/382111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparfeldt, J. R., Buch, S. R., Schwarz, F., Jachmann, J., and Rost, D. H. (2009). Arithmetic is boring — boredom in math in elementary school students. Psychol. Educ. Teach. 56, 16–26.

Google Scholar

Suldo, S. M., Riley, K. N., and Shaffer, E. J. (2006). Academic correlates of children and adolescents’ life satisfaction. Sch. Psychol. Int. 27, 567–582. doi: 10.1177/0143034306073411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, V. C., and Chan, R. M. (2009). Parental involvement in primary children’s homework in Hong Kong. Sch. Commun. J. 19, 81–100. doi: 10.1037/e705422007-001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tay, L., Chan, D., and Diener, E. (2014). The metrics of societal happiness. Soc. Indic. Res. 117, 577–600. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0356-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapa, A., Cohen, J., Guffey, S., and Higgins-D’Alessandro, A. (2013). A review of school climate research. Rev. Educ. Res. 83, 357–385. doi: 10.3102/0034654313483907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timperley, H., and Alton-Lee, A. (2008). Reframing teacher professional learning: An alternative policy approach to strengthening valued outcomes for diverse learners. Rev. Res. Educ. 32, 328–369. doi: 10.3102/0091732x07308968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Titsworth, S., Mazer, J. P., Goodboy, A. K., Bolkan, S., and Myers, S. A. (2015). Two meta-analyses exploring the relationship between teacher clarity and student learning. Commun. Educ. 64, 385–418. doi: 10.1080/03634523.2015.1041998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomyn, A. J., and Cummins, R. A. (2011). The subjective wellbeing of high-school students: Validating the personal wellbeing index — School children. Soc. Indic. Res. 101, 405–418. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9668-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Westhuizen, S. (2013). Psychological well-being and postgraduate students’ academic achievement in research methodology at an ODL institution. South Afr. J. High. Educ. 27, 1324–1342.

Google Scholar

Varner, F., and Mandara, J. (2014). Differential parenting of African American adolescents as an explanation for gender disparities in achievement. J. Res. Adolesc. 24, 667–680.

Google Scholar

Visser, M. M., Hannan, S. M., and Juan, A. L. (2019). Early learning experiences, school entry skills and later mathematics achievement in South Africa. South Afr. J. Childh. Educ. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.4102/sajce.v9i1.597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voyer, D., and Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1174–1204. doi: 10.1037/a0036620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M., and Kern, M. L. (2018). Positive education: Learning and teaching for wellbeing and academic mastery. Int. J. Wellbeing 8, 1–17. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v8i1.588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitley, A. M., Huebner, E. S., Hills, K. J., and Valois, R. F. (2012). Can students be too happy in school? The optimal level of school satisfaction. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 7, 337–350. doi: 10.1007/s11482-012-9167-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilder, S. (2014). Effects of parental involvement on academic achievement: A meta-synthesis. Educ. Rev. 66, 377–397. doi: 10.1080/00131911.2013.780009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A., and Somhlaba, N. Z. (2018). Gender, age, religion and positive mental health among adolescents in a Ghanaian socio-cultural context. Child Indic. Res. 11, 1131–1158. doi: 10.1007/s12187-017-9495-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yagan, S. A. (2021). The Relationships Between Instructional Clarity, Classroom Management and Mathematics Achievement: Mediator Role of Attitudes Towards Mathematics, Vol. 3. Sarasota, FL: University of South Florida M3 Center Publishing, 7.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y. C., Schorpp, K., and Harris, K. M. (2014). Social support, social strain, and inflammation: Evidence from a national longitudinal study of U.S. adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 107, 124–135. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakaria, A., and Abdul Halim, N. H. (2017). Relationship between life satisfaction and academic achievement among trainee teachers. Asian J. Univ. Educ. 13, 93–112.

Google Scholar

Keywords: well-being, mathematics achievement, instructional clarity, parental involvement, South Africa, education quality and outcomes

Citation: Wilson Fadiji A and Reddy V (2023) Well-being and mathematics achievement: What is the role of gender, instructional clarity, and parental involvement? Front. Psychol. 13:1044261. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1044261

Received: 14 September 2022; Accepted: 28 December 2022;
Published: 19 January 2023.

Edited by:

Nelly Lagos San Martín, University of the Bío-Bío, Chile

Reviewed by:

Gregory Siy Ching, Fu Jen Catholic University, Taiwan
Gilah Leder, La Trobe University, Australia

Copyright © 2023 Wilson Fadiji and Reddy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Angelina Wilson Fadiji, www.frontiersin.org d2lsc29uLmFuZ2VsaW5hMTMxMUBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.