Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 December 2022
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Language Embodiment, volume II: Interdisciplinary Methodological Innovations View all 12 articles

Comprehending scientific metaphors in the bilingual brain: Evidence from event-related potentials

  • 1School of Foreign Studies, Anhui Polytechnic University, Wuhu, China
  • 2Jinling High School, Nanjing, China

While the processing mechanisms of novel and conventional metaphors were widely investigated in previous monolingual studies, little attention has been devoted to how metaphoric utterances are processed by the bilingual brain as well as how scientific context might modulate such processes. Using event-related potentials (ERPs), this paper investigates the way in which scientific metaphors are electrophysiologically processed in Chinese (L1) and English (L2), with the aim of investigating the different mechanisms for understanding metaphorical language in first (L1) and second (L2) languages. By time-locking the N400 and later LPC time windows, the research show how meaning integration differs between L1 and L2 at different stages when comprehending figurative language. We found that compared with Chinese scientific metaphors, English scientific metaphors elicited greater N400, smaller late positive component (LPC), and greater late negativity, and English literals elicited greater late negativity. Our findings suggest that the dynamics of processing figurative meaning in bilingual brains over time show a complex pattern, with language, context, inference and salience jointly modulating temporal dynamics and possible cerebral asymmetries, supporting the revised hierarchical model.

Introduction

Metaphors are often used in the scientific field to talk about a less familiar domain (e.g., lymph) with a more familiar domain (e.g., police). In the LYMPH IS POLICE metaphor, the systematic function parallelisms of patrolling (in our body and in a place) is formed across languages. Such linguistic pattern suggests that the use of concrete terms is quite practical to describe abstract even intangible scientific concepts.

According to previous studies (Tang et al., 2017a,b), the target and source domains in scientific metaphors (e.g., The lymph is a policeman) are from different contexts, namely, the scientific target (lymph) and the daily source (policeman). Consequently, retrieving the stored conceptual knowledge associated with scientific metaphors is much more demanding than retrieving knowledge of literal expressions. Besides, the processing of scientific metaphors might also involve a secondary integration of meaning compared with literal expressions.

Moreover, the processing of scientific metaphors may also involve a later conceptual mapping according to some psycholinguistic models. The structural-mapping model (Gentner, 1983) and the career of a metaphor model (Gentner and Wolff, 1997; Bowdle and Gentner, 2005) suggested the mapping process involves comparing similarities between the source and the target. Accordingly, with more complicated contexts and deeper integration for analogical inference, compared with conventional metaphors (e.g., Books are friends), scientific metaphors should show more clearly the slower and more difficult mapping process of metaphors due to the absence of well-defined associations between the two domains. For example, in “The intervening lymph nodes can trap and destroy the cancer cells.,” the target concept LYMPH and the source concept POLICE are aligned by the predicate “trap and destroy,” and therefore, the role played by the lymphatic system in the body’s immune system “lymph” is described as the role played by the political system in a place.

Then, what is the basic mechanism underlying understanding scientific metaphors in L2 (second language) for late learners, and to what extent do they overlap with the mechanisms involved in scientific metaphor processing in L1 (first language)? According to the non-selective access/integrated view, words in the L2 should behave like low-frequency words in the L1 (Dijkstra et al., 1998; Lemhofer et al., 2008). However, according to the revised hierarchical model (Kroll and Stewart, 1994), it would be an unlikely possibility for late learners of L2 to process words in the similar way L1 learners do, and the mechanisms for processing words in L2 and L1 are quite different. At least, anecdotally, many L2 learners report using translation into L1 as a general heuristic for processing L2 words (van Der Meij et al., 2011), resulting in the switch cost (slower response latencies; Verhoef et al., 2009; Calabria et al., 2011).

Actually, the comparison between performance in L2 and that in L1 might be modulated by the frequency characteristics of the words in each language (Midgley et al., 2009). Exposure to L1 words is generally much higher than exposure to L2 words. Therefore, the subjective frequencies of L2 words might be lower than those of L1 words, which could be driving the behavioral effects. Furthermore, age-of-acquisition could be another factor (Wang and Chen, 2020), and L2 words are learned later than the majority of L1 words. Finally, the lexical-semantic connection between L2 words is much weaker than that between L1 words (Midgley et al., 2009; Naranowicz et al., 2022). In order to observe the modulation effects of the factors mentioned above, the current study adopted scientific metaphors as the target stimuli. Scientific terms used in scientific metaphors distinguish them from other kinds of metaphors. Generally speaking, either in L1 or L2, exposure to scientific words is lower than exposure to daily words; they are used and learned later than daily words; the interconnection between scientific words and daily words are weaker than that between different daily words.

The aim of the present study was to examine the neural mechanisms for bilingual speakers (L1 Chinese and L2 English) to process figurative language with scientific metaphors as the stimuli. Adopting scientific metaphors with complicated contextual structure and knowledge-inferencing process might be of more significance to show the difference between the processing of L1 and L2.

Models for metaphor processing

According to the graded salience hypothesis (Giora, 2003), the time-course of meaning processing is mainly determined by the degree of meaning salience which refers to those meanings foremost in speakers’ minds at time of speaking characterized by conventionality, prototypicality, familiarity, and frequency. The meaning of literal expressions is commonly salient, and is processed first in the left hemisphere. In contrast, for a novel or unfamiliar metaphor, the salient meaning is the literal one, and the figurative meaning is inferred later by contextual mechanisms and is processed mainly in the right hemisphere. In other words, how linguistic stimuli are processed is determined by the salience-non-salience continuum instead of the literal metaphoric distinction. This hypothesis predicts a later and right-hemisphere biased processing of nonsalient meanings (such as scientific metaphors in L1) and an earlier and left-hemisphere biased processing of salient meanings (such as literal expressions in L1).

The Fine-Coarse Semantic Coding Theory (Beeman, 1998, 2005) is another psycholinguistic theory that addresses hemispheric functions in semantic processing, which proposed that language is processed qualitatively differently by the two cerebral hemispheres. The right hemisphere loosely activates and maintains larger semantic fields containing more distant associates and more unconventional meanings (coarse semantic coding) whereas the left hemisphere focuses on a single dominant interpretation (fine semantic coding). Since the distance between the source and target domains is usually semantically longer than that for literal expressions, the theory predicts semantic processes in the right hemisphere may be more apt for scientific metaphor comprehension in L1.

More importantly, these two models might have some important implications when comparing L1 and L2 metaphor processing. For native speakers, a literal expression possesses a highly salient interpretation since native speakers have naturally encountered the expressions quite often in their daily life. Thus, on encountering a highly conventional expression, the left hemisphere engages in a fine coding and strong activation of small semantic fields. And for scientific metaphors, it might be of little difficulty for L1 speakers to understand the literal meaning of scientific metaphors. It might be difficult for them to achieve the mapping between the distant domains covering two different contexts and to make scientific inference to get some sort of new knowledge, probably involving a coarse processing of meanings in the right hemisphere.

However, for later learners of L2, the picture is more complicated. At the early stage of processing the meaning, the literal meaning of scientific metaphors may be less salient, resulting in relatively longer period of processing and stronger activation of the right hemisphere of coarse semantic coding. At the later stage of mapping and inference, it might be very difficult to deeply integrate the meaning in L2, probably resulting in delayed reintegration of meaning, stronger activation of the left hemisphere of fine semantic coding and the weaker activation of large and diffuse semantic fields in the right hemisphere.

Event-related potentials (ERPs) and bilingual metaphor processing

The mean amplitudes of N400 were widely investigated to reveal the processing mechanism of metaphors. Previous studies show that N400 amplitudes are sensitive to semantic violations (Kutas and Hillyard, 1980) and are modulated by several variables on the mechanism of figurative language processing, such as familiarity, difficulty, context or task (Schmidt and Seger, 2009). Meanwhile, some bilingual ERP studies reported a modulation of N400 amplitudes by language (Newman et al., 2012; Heidlmayr et al., 2015), with L2 eliciting lower N400 compared with L1.

The late positive component (LPC) is considered to reflect integration or reprocessing at the sentence level (Kaan et al., 2000) and is found to be modulated by the degree of conventionality (Weiland et al., 2014) with novel metaphors eliciting greater LPCs than conventional metaphors and literal expressions. Other studies reported a late negativity overlapping in the LPC time window resulting in smaller LPCs elicited by novel metaphors compared with conventional metaphors indicating a sustained difficulty in fusing two concepts of the source domain and the target domain (Arzouan et al., 2007a,b; Goldstein et al., 2012; Rutter et al., 2012).

The feasibility of investigating the processing mechanism of scientific metaphors using amplitudes of N400 and late negativity was confirmed in our previous monolingual studies (Tang et al., 2017a,b). Compared with conventional metaphors and literal expressions, scientific metaphors and poetic metaphors elicited higher N400s reflecting the modulation of conventionality. More importantly, scientific metaphors elicited larger late negativity compared with poetic metaphors and conventional metaphors (Tang et al., 2017b).

Moreover, for the bilingual studies, N400 peak latency was also proved to be susceptible to biological and cognitive influences (Moreno et al., 2008). For example, a delay in N400 latency is associated with increasing the age of participants and increasing stimulus presentation rate (Midgley et al., 2009). Most of the studies examined the N400 event-related response with a semantic violation paradigm and reported a significant delay in its peak latency for L2 compared with L1 (Hahne and Friederici, 2001; Moreno and Kutas, 2005). For example, the peak latency of the N400 effect elicited by non-dominant language target word was significantly later (approximately 27 ms) than that elicited by dominant language target words (Moreno and Kutas, 2005). It appears that both the age of exposure to the language and the proficiency of the language are factors in the delay of the peak latency of the N400 response to semantic incongruities. However, the factors contributing to delays in the N400 response in bilinguals in their L2 vs. L1 demands further investigation because an early age of exposure does not always guarantee a fast response to semantic incongruity. Other modulations of N400 parameters such as amplitude, onset latency, and scalp distribution are not consistent across studies (Anna and Roberto, 2011; Mashal et al., 2015).

The present study

While much research has been done on novel and conventional metaphor comprehension in the monolingual context, little attention has been devoted to how metaphoric utterances are processed by the bilingual brain as well as how scientific context might modulate such processes. The present study aims to investigate the electrophysiological correlates of scientific metaphor comprehension in intermediate Chinese-English bilingual speakers. Our predictions are as follows. In the N400 window, due to the complicated contextual structure, scientific metaphors, either English or Chinese, might elicit higher N400 reflecting higher cognitive load in retrieving the stored information for meaning integration. More importantly, there should be N400 differences when processing Chinese (L1) and English (L2) scientific metaphors due to the fact that the lexical-semantic connection of Chinese scientific metaphors is stronger than that of English ones for the native Chinese speakers. English scientific metaphor processing should show higher amplitude, longer latency, and right-biased activation when compared with Chinese ones.

In the LPC window, scientific metaphors might elicit more negative late negativity for further integration of meaning to achieve the later knowledge-understanding inference. Moreover, there should be different patterns of late components when processing Chinese (L1) and English (L2) scientific metaphors due to the fact that the L2 structure especially involving complicated reasoning tends to be processed in a different way with the L1 structure. Chinese scientific metaphors with looser semantic relations might elicit greater right hemisphere activity compared with literal expressions. However, it should be very difficult for deep-level reintegration of meaning to occur in L2. Thus, compared with Chinese scientific metaphor processing, English scientific metaphor processing should show lower amplitude, shorter latency, and left-biased activation.

Experiment 1: Comprehension of scientific metaphors in native language (Chinese)

Experiment 1 was set up as a control experiment to examine whether subjects showed the same pattern when processing scientific metaphors in L2 as in L1.

Participants

All participants were undergraduate students at Shaanxi Normal University who were paid for participation. Twenty-three (10 males, 13 females, average age 20.6) right-handed subjects spoke Chinese as their native language and started to learn English from elementary period. They have passed CET-4 but have not passed CET-6 (CET is a national test of English for non-English majors in China), which means their proficiency of English is not high. Past or present mental or neurological disorders or major head injury, or crime are exclusion criteria. Handedness was tested using the Edinburgh Handedness Scale (Oldfield, 1971). The resulting average score was above 40 (M = 120.71, SD = 36.67), which means all participants were right-handed (left-handedness: lower than-40; ambidexter: between-40 and 40). The local Human Participation Research Review Board approved the experimental criteria for the study. Prior to participation, each subject provided written informed consent. However, due to a lack of available trials (lower than 85% of the total 80 trials based on artifact detection), data from six subjects were not included in the analysis, resulting in a final sample size of 17 subjects (eight males and nine females).

Stimuli

For experiment 1, the materials were from our previous study (Tang et al., 2017a), in which several pretests had been done on familiarity, metaphoricity, and meaningfulness (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The results of pretests.

The stimulus pool consisted of 80 sentences, all in Chinese (see Table 2 for examples), which fell into two categories: scientific metaphoric (SM) and daily literal (LT), with 40 sentences in each sentence category. Grammar and syntactic structures were matched across different categories by adopting the “X 是 (shi) Y” format in all sentences and the lengths of the target words were balanced between categories, thus excluding possible confounding factors such as sentence length or complex syntactic processing. The frequencies of the target words were tested through BCC Corpus (Xun et al., 2016). A paired sample t-test showed that there was no significant difference (t = −1.93, p = 0.061) of the frequencies between the target words of Chinese scientific metaphors (M = 34032.9, SD = 7032.32) and those of Chinese literal expressions (M = 72092.1, SD = 19377.3). In order to control the concreteness of the target words across categories, only words considered as concrete were selected as stimuli and 40 raters with similar educational backgrounds, English levels and ages to the participants were asked to judge whether each target word is concrete or not on a 1–7 scale (1 = highly abstract, 7 = highly concrete). A paired sample t-test showed that there was no significant difference (t = −0.46, p = 0.648) of the concreteness between the target words of scientific metaphors (M = 5.3, SD = 0.22) and those of literal expressions (M = 5.36, SD = 0.28). Several pretests of meaningfulness, figurativeness and familiarity were conducted in our previous study.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Chinese sample stimuli.

Procedure

The experiment took place in a sound-attenuated, electrically shielded room. The sentences were presented in white color on a black background word by word in a quasi-random order. Stimuli on each trial were presented in the following time sequence: fixation cross (800 ms), blank (200–500 ms), subject (1,000 ms), verb (600 ms), blank (200–500 ms), object (1,000 ms) and question mark (3,000 ms). At the sight of the question mark, participants gave their judgments about whether the sentence was metaphoric or not by pressing a corresponding key with the right and left index fingers. Response period was limited to 3 s and was followed by a 1 s intertrial interval. The overall sequence of events for a trial is illustrated in Figure 1. Before the ERP experiment, in order to make sure that the academic knowledge involved in scientific metaphors could be understood during the experiment, participants firstly were asked to read a list of scientific terms, from which scientific metaphors were created, together with their brief explanations. Before the main session of the experiment, there was a brief practice session to familiarize the participants with the experimental procedure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm of experiment 1.

Electrophysiological recording

Scalp voltages were collected with the CURRY 7 system (Compumedics Neuroscan, Texas, United States) with 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes, monitored by the CURRY recorder software and connected to a SynAmp amplifier (Compumedics Neuroscan, Texas, United States). Amplified analog voltages were digitized at 1,000 Hz. Impedances of individual sensors were kept below 5 kΩ. Eye movements were monitored through bipolar electrodes which were placed above and below the right eye, as well as at the left and right canthi. EEG was measured online with reference to the left mastoid, with a ground electrode on the medial frontal aspect, and later was analyzed offline with re-reference to an average of the left and right mastoids.

EEG was analyzed with the SCAN 4.5 software (Compumedics Neuroscan, Texas, United States) and Matlab using the ERPLAB toolbox (Lopez-Calderon and Luck, 2014). The EEG was digitally filtered at 0.1–30 Hz bandpass. Eye movements were corrected with an ocular artifact correction algorithm (Gratton et al., 1983). Artifacts with amplitudes exceeding ±75 μV were removed from analyses. ERPs were time-locked to the onset of the last word of the sentence and were obtained by stimulus-locked averaging of the EEG recorded in each condition. Epochs were 1,000 ms long with a 200 ms pre-stimulus baseline and the epochs baseline corrected using the-200–0 ms time window. The time windows of N400 (300–500 ms) and LPC (550–800 ms) were selected based on the Grand average ERP waveforms. Within these time intervals, the mean amplitude values of N400 and LPC as well as the 25% fractional area latency of N400 were determined. The resulting amplitudes of N400 and LPC as well as N400 latency values were entered into repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA). All ANOVA results were Greenhouse–Geisser corrected if assumption of sphericity was violated.

Event-related brain potentials were time-locked to the onset of the last word of the sentence. The resulting amplitudes of N400 were entered into 2 type (scientific metaphors, literal expressions) × 3 region (frontal F3, Fz, F4, central C3, Cz, C4, parietal P3, Pz, P4) × 3 hemisphere (left F3, C3, P3, midline Fz, Cz, Pz, right F4, C4, P4) three-way ANOVAs for repeated measures.

Results

Behavioral performance

To achieve the target of this analysis, we calculated the mean response time for correct trials and the accuracy rate for each sentence type for each participant. A paired samples t-test revealed significant effects of type for both reaction times (t = 3.7, p = 0.002) and accuracy rates (t = −3.66, p = 0.002). Reaction times were longer for scientific metaphors (M = 520.11 ms, SD = 216.94 ms) than for literal expressions (M = 459.55 ms, SD = 186.75 ms). Accuracy rates were significantly lower for scientific metaphors (M = 0.83, SD = 0.087) than for literal expressions (M = 0.95, SD = 0.069).

Electrophysiological data

In the time window of N400 (300 ~ 500 ms), the type (Chinese scientific metaphors, Chinese literal expressions) × region × hemisphere ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of type for amplitudes elicited by Chinese pairs [F (1, 16) = 103.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.87]. Scientific metaphors elicited more negative N400 (M = 0.55 μV, SD = 3.37 μV) than literals (M = 3.42 μV, SD = 3.2 μV; see Figure 2). For the N400 latency values, a similar repeated measures ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of type (p = 0.381).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grand average event-related potential (ERP) waveforms recorded at the 9 chosen electrodes of Chinese pairs.

In the time window of LPC (550 ~ 800 ms), the type × region × hemisphere ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of type for Chinese pairs [F (1, 16) = 9.09, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.36]. Consistent with Tang et al. (2017a), as shown in Figure 2, the ERPs of scientific metaphors (M = 1.53 μV, SD = 3.94 μV) were less positive than those of literal sentences (M = 3.15 μV, SD = 4.38 μV). There were significant type × region interactions [F (2, 32) = 5.55, p = 0.014, η2p = 0.26]. Simple effect tests showed significant differences between scientific metaphors and literal sentences in the parietal and central regions [Parietal: F (1, 16) = 11.37, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.42; Central: F (1, 16) = 5.8, p = 0.028, η2p = 0.27] but not in the frontal region (p = 0.42; See Figure 2).

Discussion

The two main findings regarding N400 for the contextual factor and the late negativity for the knowledge-understanding inference factor are consistent with the findings of other studies (Tang et al., 2017a,b). From the conceptual blending view (Coulson and Van Petten, 2002; Yang et al., 2013), during the semantic processing to integrate these elements in metaphor comprehension, the amplitudes of N400 indicate the degree of difficulties in retrieving the stored conceptual knowledge. Consistent with previous studies (Rutter et al., 2012; Lai and Curran, 2013; Schneider et al., 2014), this study found that scientific metaphors elicited more negative N400s relative to literal expressions. Another possible explanation for these effects is the different salience between scientific metaphors and literal expressions. According to the graded salience hypothesis (Giora, 2003), the metaphoric meanings of scientific metaphors are less salient than the literal meanings which results in a more cognitive costly processing for meaning retrieving than literal expressions. In addition, consist with our previous study which reported higher N400 elicited by Chinese scientific metaphorical and literal expressions compared with Chinese daily literal expressions (Tang et al., 2016), the electrophysiological differences of N400s between Chinese scientific metaphors and Chinese literal expressions in present study might be related not only to metaphoric factor but also to scientific factor reflecting the modulation of complex scientific context of scientific metaphors.

Consistent with previous studies (Jankowiak et al., 2017, 2021; Tang et al., 2017a,b), scientific metaphors might elicit a more negative component partly overlapping in space and time with the LPC reducing the amplitudes of LPC. Such a late negativity might reflect secondary semantic integration processes of novel metaphors (Arzouan et al., 2007a,b; Goldstein et al., 2012; Rutter et al., 2012), showing a sustained reinterpretation process after an initial failure to reach meaning (Jiang et al., 2009) supporting the structural-mapping model (Gentner, 1983) and the career of a metaphor model (Gentner and Wolff, 1997; Bowdle and Gentner, 2005). Moreover, such electrophysiological differences of late negativity might be related to scientific factor consist with our previous study reflecting higher cognitive cost caused by the late knowledge-understanding inference of scientific language (Tang et al., 2016).

Experiment 2: Comprehension of scientific metaphors in non-native language (English)

Participants

The participants of Experiment 2 were the same as those of Experiment 1. In order to avoid the learning effects, experiment 2 was conducted 1 month later.

Stimuli

The 80 English sentences form the stimulus pool and these sentences are the counterparts of the Chinese stimuli (see Table 3 for examples), which fell into two categories: literal and scientific metaphoric, with 40 sentences in each sentence category (Table 4). Similar to the pilot surveys we have done on the Chinese stimuli in our previous study used in Experiment 1 (Tang et al., 2017a), the English stimuli were tested by several groups of raters who did not participate in the ERP experiment. Only participants who had majored in a scientific discipline were selected so that they could avoid difficulties in understanding the scientific terms associated with the scientific metaphor categories (Table 4).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. English sample stimuli.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Experimental materials.

Firstly, 40 raters were asked to decide whether each English sentence is meaningful or not on a 1–3 scale (1 = not meaningful, 2 = somewhat meaningful, 3 = highly meaningful). According to the results of the meaningfulness judgment, sentences rated by at least 75% of the judges as metaphorically/literally plausible (> 2.5) were selected as expressions with either a metaphoric or a literal meaning. Then those selected sentences were rated by another group of 40 raters on a 1–3 scale regarding their figurativeness (1 = not figurative, 2 = somewhat figurative, 3 = highly figurative). In the present study, literal expressions, i.e., expressions averaging less than 1.5, and scientific metaphors averaging greater than 2.5. Finally, we asked another group of 40 raters to rate these expressions on a 7-point familiarity scale ranging from 1 (highly unfamiliar) to 7 (highly familiar). Expressions scoring more than 4 on this scale were selected as scientific metaphors (mean rating 5.33) and literal expressions (mean rating 6.32).

The frequencies of the target words of those English stimuli were also tested through Brown Corpus. A paired sample t-test showed that there was no significant difference (t = −0.99, p = 0.33) of the frequencies between the target words of English scientific metaphors (M = 77.08 per million, SD = 25.9) and those of English literal expressions (M = 106.98 per million, SD = 28.45).

Procedure

Experiment 2 was conducted following the same procedure of Experiment 1 but in English (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Experimental paradigm of experiment 2.

Electrophysiological recording

The electrophysiological recording of Experiment 2 was the same as that of Experiment 1.

Results

Behavioral performance

For the purposes of this analysis, we calculated the mean response time for correct trials and the accuracy rate for each sentence type for each participant. A paired samples t-test revealed significant effects of type for accuracy rates (t = −4.89, p < 0.001). Accuracy rates were significantly lower for English scientific metaphors (M = 0.84, SD = 0.07) than for English literal expressions (M = 0.85, SD = 0.07). The difference in reaction times between scientific metaphors and literal expressions was not found to be significant (p = 0.471, SM: M = 591.04 ms, SD = 176.21 ms; LT: M = 591.59 ms, SD = 177.64 ms).

Electrophysiological data

300 ~ 500 ms

As shown in Figure 4, in the time window of N400 (300 ~ 500 ms), there were significant main effects of type (English scientific metaphors, English literal expressions) [F (1, 16) = 62.26, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.79]. Scientific metaphors elicited more negative N400 (M = −1.35 μV, SD = 3.01 μV) than literal sentences (M = 1.5 μV, SD = 2.59 μV). For the N400 latency values, similar to the result of experiment 1, no significant main effect of type (p = 0.288) was found.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Grand average ERP waveforms recorded at the nine chosen electrodes of English pairs.

550 ~ 800 ms
LPC

In the time window of LPC (550 ~ 800 ms), there were significant main effects of type [F (1, 16) = 57.63, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.78]. As shown in Figure 4, the ERPs of scientific metaphors (M = −1.43 μV, SD = 3.08 μV) were less positive than those of literal sentences (M = 0.98 μV, SD = 2.85 μV). There were significant type × region interactions [F (2, 32) = 13.89, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.47]. Simple effect tests showed great significant differences between English scientific metaphors and literal sentences in the parietal and central regions [Parietal: F (1, 16) = 61.73, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.79; Central: F (1, 16) = 76.51, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.83] (see Figure 4).

Discussion

Similar to the Chinese pairs, English (L2) scientific metaphors also elicited more negative N400 than literal ones reflecting more demanding retrieving of stored conceptual knowledge due to the mapping between the scientific target and the daily source. Two aspects could be explained this discrepancy: either English scientific metaphors are difficult to process because of the need to reject literal meanings and retrieve appropriate metaphorical meanings (Wang and Jankowiak, 2021), or bilinguals have difficulty transferring explicit L2 knowledge to their implicit language skills (Chen et al., 2013). Furthermore, English scientific metaphors also elicited more negative late negativity reflecting the secondary integration of meaning due to the late analogical comparison involved in processing scientific metaphors.

Comparative analysis between L1 and L2

Results

Behavioral performance

A further language (Chinese, English) × type (scientific metaphor, literal expression) repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effects of language for accuracy rates [F (1, 16) = 4.81, p = 0.043, η2p = 0.12]. The accuracy rates were significantly lower for English expressions (M = 0.85, SD = 0.02) than those for Chinese expressions (M = 0.89, SD = 0.01). Moreover, a repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effects of language for reaction times [F (1, 16) = 4.66, p = 0.046, η2p = 0.23]. The reaction times of English expressions (M = 591.32 ms, SD = 42.91 ms) were significantly longer than those of Chinese expressions (M = 489.83 ms, SD = 48.4 ms).

Electrophysiological data

300 ~ 500 ms
N400

A language (Chinese, English) × type (scientific metaphors, literal expressions) × region × hemisphere repeated-measures ANOVA was performed for the N400 amplitudes of English and Chinese scientific metaphoric pairs and literal pairs. As shown in Figure 5, the main effects of language were salient [SM: F (1, 16) = 6.47, p = 0.022, η2p = 0.29; LT: F (1, 16) = 8.39, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.34], with English stimuli eliciting more negative N400 than Chinese stimuli [English stimuli: M = 0.08 μV, SD = 0.43 μV; Chinese stimuli: M = 1.99 μV, SD = 0.61 μV].

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Grand average ERP waveforms recorded at the Pz electrodes of four conditions

Significant language × region interactions were found in both pairs [SM: F (2, 32) = 6.47, p = 0.022, η2p = 0.29; LT: F (2, 32) = 8.49, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.34]. Simple effect tests showed that English sentences elicited more negative N400 in the central and parietal regions (ps < 0.05). Language × hemisphere interactions were not significant for the scientific metaphoric pairs (p = 0.14), but marginally significant for the literal pairs [F (2, 32) = 3.33, p = 0.064, η2p = 0.17]. Simple effect tests showed that English and Chinese literals differed significantly in the midline and the right hemisphere (ps < 0.05). The results indicate that both hemispheres play a significant role in the comprehension of English sentences by Chinese-English bilinguals, with the right parietal area of the right hemisphere necessarily involved.

For the N400 latency values, a similar repeated-measure ANOVA revealed no significant language effect for both scientific metaphoric pairs and literal pairs (SM: p = 0.145, LT: p = 0.978). Language × region interactions were not significant (p = 0.408). However, there were significant language × hemisphere interactions [F (2, 32) = 4.79, p = 0.018, η2p = 0.23]. Simple effects tests showed that the mean N400 latency of English stimuli (M = 380.45 ms, SD = 1.34 ms) was marginally significantly later than that of Chinese stimuli (M = 376.35 ms, SD = 1.76 ms) in the midline (p = 0.073). Moreover, language × type × hemisphere interactions were marginally significant [F (2, 32) = 3.46, p = 0.066, η2p = 0.18]. Simple effects tests showed that the N400 latencies of English scientific metaphors were significantly later than that of Chinese scientific metaphors in the midline and the right hemisphere (ps < 0.05) where no significant difference between N400 latencies of English and Chinese literal expressions was found.

550 ~ 800 ms
LPC

In the time window of LPC (550 ~ 800 ms), for the LPC amplitudes of English and Chinese scientific metaphoric pairs and literal pairs, the main effects of language were both significant [SM: F (1, 16) = 12.18, p = 0.003, η2p = 0.43; LT: F (1, 16) = 4.54, p = 0.049, η2p = 0.22], with English stimuli eliciting less positive LPCs than Chinese stimuli [English stimuli: M = −0.23 μV, SD = 0.48 μV; Chinese stimuli: M = 2.34 μV, SD = 0.73 μV]. Significant language × region interactions were found in both pairs [SM: F (2, 32) = 14.34, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.47; LT: F (2, 32) = 13.21, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.45]. Simple effect tests showed that both English scientific metaphors and literals elicited less positive LPC in the parietal regions (ps < 0.01) and English scientific metaphors also elicited less positive LPC in the central region (p = 0.011). Language × hemisphere interactions were not significant for the two pairs (ps > 0.1).

The sustained late negativity

Moreover, the ERP patterns in the late window (550–800 ms) were further illustrated by subtracting the amplitude of Chinese literal expressions from those of English scientific metaphoric, English literal and Chinese scientific metaphoric expressions respectively, and the resulting difference curves are shown in Figure 6. Following their prominent N400 effects, the two English conditions and Chinese scientific metaphors displayed a second negativity peaking around 760 ms. There were significant main effects of type [F (2, 32) = 9.25, p = 0.005, η2p = 0.37]. The two English conditions elicited more negative amplitudes than Chinese scientific metaphors (ESM: M = −4.58 μV, SD = 5.89 μV; ELT: M = −2.17 μV, SD = 5.84 μV; CSM: M = −1.62 μV, SD = 3.74 μV; E refers to English while C refers to Chinese). The interaction effects between type and region [F (4, 64) = 11.09, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.41] indicated that the differences were more prominent at the central and parietal regions.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Grand average ERP waveforms recorded at the nine chosen electrodes of the late negativity

For the English pairs, there were main effects of type [F (1, 16) = 57.63, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.78]. There were significant type × region interactions [F (2, 32) = 13.89, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.47]. Simple effect tests showed significant differences in all the three regions [Fs (1, 16) > 15, ps < 0.01] with the more significant differences existing in the central and parietal regions [Fs (1, 16) > 60, ps < 0.001].

For English and Chinese scientific metaphoric pairs, there were main effects of language [F (1, 16) = 12.18, p = 0.003, η2p = 0.43]. Language × region interactions were significant [F (2, 32) = 14.34, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.47]. Simple effect tests showed significant differences in the parietal region [F (1, 16) = 37.95, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.7] and the central region [F (1, 16) = 8.26, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.34], but not in the frontal region (p = 0.41).

For English literal and Chinese scientific metaphoric pairs, type × region interaction was significant [F (2, 32) = 7, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.31] with the significant difference existing in the parietal region [F (1, 16) = 7.24, p = 0.016, η2p = 0.31], but not in the frontal and central regions (ps > 0.9). In order to make a clear comparison between the two conditions, separate pairwise ANOVAs for all the three electrodes in the parietal region were performed. The differences were very significant at the right site [P4: F (1, 16) = 16.72, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.51], significant at the central site [Pz: F (1, 16) = 5.79, p = 0.029, η2p = 0.27], and not significant at the left site (P3: p = 0.16).

Discussion

The different familiarities of L1 and L2

Firstly, in this study, English literals elicited more negative N400 than Chinese literals. The N400 indexes the difficulty in retrieving the stored information of a word (Coulson and Van Petten, 2002; Yang et al., 2013). Greater N400 amplitudes for L2 daily literal expressions might result from their unfamiliarity (Proverbio et al., 2002; Moreno et al., 2008; Midgley et al., 2009; Newman et al., 2012; Heidlmayr et al., 2015). For late learners, L1 daily words are used very frequently in people’s life but it is not mostly the case for L2 daily words. Naturally, exposure to L1 words is much higher than exposure to L2 words, resulting in the lower familiarity of L2 words. Therefore, processing the meaning of L2 words appears more difficult than that of L1 words (Wang and Chen, 2020).

Secondly, it is known from previous studies of N400 in bilingual semantic processing (WeberFox and Neville, 1996; Phillips et al., 2004; Moreno and Kutas, 2005; Braunstein et al., 2012) that N400 latencies are longer for English words than for Chinese words. The present study also reported longer N400 latencies of English stimuli compared with that of Chinese stimuli in the midline, which was also consistent with the behavioral results showing reaction times of English stimuli were longer than that of Chinese stimuli. As can be seen from the BIA+ model (Dijkstra and van Heuven, 2002) the time delay assumed by the lower subjective frequency of L2 items may lead to delayed activation of semantic representations in non-native speakers. Thus, automatic operations involved in lexical-semantic access are less frequent when dealing with non-dominant languages with lower resting level activation (Jankowiak et al., 2017). Besides, L2 is acquired in a fundamentally different way from L1, so the lexical-semantic connection of L2 is weaker than that of L1 (Midgley et al., 2009; Palmer et al., 2010). L2 is mostly acquired explicitly during formal classroom instruction at school age, whereas native languages are always acquired implicitly during childhood.

The N400 regarding the scientific contextual factor

Studies have shown that the N400 observed for metaphor is related to context and may be an index of contextual expectations for upcoming words, guiding lexical access and retrieval (Bambini et al., 2016). In summary, scientific metaphors are more contextually complex than traditional metaphors, as they cover both scientific target domains and everyday source domains. It is clear from the results of this study that the N400 model of L1 and L2 processing of scientific metaphors is different.

Firstly, in this study, consistent with our predictions, English scientific metaphoric elicited more negative N400 than Chinese ones. The words in the source domains of either English or Chinese scientific metaphors were from daily life. As mentioned in the above discussion, retrieving the literal meaning of L2 words should be more difficult due to their lower familiarity and frequencies of usage. More importantly, the words used in the target domains of either English or Chinese scientific metaphors were scientific terms. Compared with daily words, exposure to scientific terms should be lower. Compared with L1 scientific terms, late learners’ exposure to L2 scientific terms should be even lower. Therefore, the different degrees of difficulties in processing L1 and L2 words is shown more clearly by adopting scientific terms in this study.

Secondly, following the retrieval of literal meaning of words in the daily source and scientific target domains respectively, processing scientific metaphors involves the integration of the two heterogeneous domains. Another possible reason for greater N400 amplitudes for L2 scientific metaphors lies in their complicated contextual structure, further enhancing the difficulty in integrating meaning. Some studies have shown that the integration of the two domains of L2 conventional metaphors costs more energy because the lexical-semantic connection between L2 words are weaker than that between L1 words (Midgley et al., 2009). Other studies have shown that the integration of the two domains of L1 scientific metaphors is more difficult than that of L1 conventional metaphors due to the longer distance between the two heterogeneous domains of scientific ones than that between those of conventional ones (Tang et al., 2017a,b). Based on the two findings, especially for late L2 learners, integrating the two domains of L2 scientific metaphors should be much more demanding due to the context of weaker lexical-semantic connection of words and longer distance of mapping.

Thirdly, consistent with our predictions, the mean N400 latency of English stimuli was longer than that of Chinese ones in the midline. As mentioned above, recognizing the literal meaning of L2 words, especially scientific terms, takes longer time (Taft et al., 2021). Moreover, the N400 latencies of English scientific metaphors were significantly longer than that of Chinese scientific metaphors in the midline and the right hemisphere revealed the unique time-course processing of scientific metaphors compared with literal expressions. Unlike the difference between L1 and L2 daily words, the scientific terms of both L1 and L2 are mostly learned explicitly with the method of formal classroom teaching at school age. Generally, scientific terms are used in some academic context, where L1 is often used to express difficult and abstract ideas and knowledge. According to the inhibitory control (IC) model (Green, 1998), in the L2 (English) context, it is difficult to inhibit L1 representation for scientific metaphors. The predominantly used non-dominant language may largely incur switching costs (Kalinka et al., 2018).

Moreover, the semantic connection between scientific terms and daily words should be weaker than that between different daily words. Therefore, compared with L1 scientific terms, the semantic connection of L2 scientific terms should be even lower. Hence the mapping of L2 scientific metaphors in the context of two heterogeneous domains might take more time (Jankowiak et al., 2017).

The late component regarding The reasoning factor

Some studies have shown that traditional and poetic metaphors have emotionally stimulating functions, while scientific metaphors have unique properties of knowledge comprehension, involving late reasoning processes (Tang et al., 2017a,b). The findings of the current research indicate that the late component patterns are much more complicated for L2 processing.

In this study, both English conditions (scientific metaphoric and literal) elicited less positive LPC than their Chinese counterparts. The reason might be that in the late time window when structural integration occurs semantic integration is prolonged (Kaan et al., 2000; Friederici, 2002). Firstly, the LPC has been thought to reflect the level of syntactic processing. The higher amplitudes of LPC for L1 might indicate that processing L1 involves more syntactic analysis while processing L2 mostly involves semantic analysis (Kotz, 2009). Secondly, from the perspective of semantic integration, the enhanced LPC for L1 might show the deeper processing of knowledge-reasoning of L1 scientific metaphors (Jankowiak et al., 2017).

Moreover, a late negativity was elicited in the time window of LPC. The late negativity has been found and reported by some monolingual (Arzouan et al., 2007a,b; Goldstein et al., 2012; Rutter et al., 2012) and bilingual studies on metaphor comprehension (Jankowiak et al., 2017; Jankowiak et al., 2021; Wang and Jankowiak, 2021), marking the ongoing difficulty of the secondary semantic integration especially of novel metaphors (Tang et al., 2017a,b) or the activated non-literal routes when understanding complex semantics in non-native contexts (Jankowiak et al., 2017).

In this study, inconsistent with our predictions, the two English conditions elicited more negative amplitudes than Chinese scientific metaphors. Firstly, the enhanced late negativity of English scientific metaphors might show that comprehending L2 scientific metaphors is more difficult, since the late negativity may mark the ongoing difficulty of integrating the two concepts. However, generally speaking, the depth of semantic integration for L2 metaphors could not reach the same magnitude as that for L1 metaphors (Midgley et al., 2009), especially when the late complicated reasoning process is involved in understanding the abstract concept related with scientific metaphors, which was also supported by the behavioral data showing that the difference of reaction times was not significant between English scientific metaphors and English literal expressions but significant between the two English conditions and Chinese scientific metaphors. Secondly, processing daily literals does not involve the late reasoning process for understanding related knowledge. Therefore, the late negativity with enhanced amplitudes observed for processing English conditions might probably imply a switching from L2 to L1 in the late period of processing L2, which elaborated the amplitudes of English conditions in this study. The late negativity elicited by the codes switch from L2 to L1 (from the weaker language to the dominant language) has been reported to suggest that switching may engage the activation costs of the specific lexical forms in the less active language and effortful sentence-level restructuring mechanisms (Palmer et al., 2010; Shukhan Ng et al., 2014; Litcofsky and Van Hell, 2017; Zeller, 2020).

The hemispheric involvement

Consistent with our predictions and previous studies (Jankowiak et al., 2017), during the earlier time window (N400), the processing of English (L2) expressions show a right-biased distribution of the brain. The lexical-semantic connection between L2 words is much weaker than that between L1 words (Midgley et al., 2009; Naranowicz et al., 2022), leading to a right-biased distribution. Based on fine-coarse semantic coding theory (Beeman, 2005), the coarse semantic encoding of the right hemisphere loosely activates and maintains a larger semantic domain, in which the semantic associations are more distant and meanings are more unconventional, while the fine semantic encoding of the left hemisphere focuses on a single dominant interpretation. Moreover, the significant difference of N400 latencies between English scientific metaphors and Chinese scientific metaphors in the midline and the right hemisphere also indicates a unique role of the right hemisphere for the processing of scientific metaphors. During the later time window (the late components), inconsistent with our predictions, the processing of English (L2) expressions show a right-biased distribution of the brain. However, probably coarse semantic coding might be much weaker for a non-native than native language (Miriam et al., 2012). That is to say, the coarse semantic processing might be too difficult for L2 learners so that they might switch to the fine semantic processing of L2. That is to say, the integration of two distant domains involving the scientific reasoning increases the difficulties of L2 processing so that L2 users might switch to their L1 in this case. Therefore, the right-biased distribution of the late negativity elicited by English scientific metaphors might result firstly from the code switch from English to Chinese, and secondly from the coarse semantic processing of scientific metaphors (Ng et al., 2014; Litcofsky and Van Hell, 2017).

Conclusion

Overall, factors that interfere with the processing of metaphors include the different lexical encodings in the two languages of bilinguals (Xue et al., 2014). As demonstrated in the experiments reported here, the dynamics of processing literal and figurative meaning over time suggest a complex pattern, with language, context, inference and salience jointly modulating temporal dynamics and possible cerebral asymmetries, supporting the revised hierarchical model and the graded salience hypothesis. For processing L1 and L2 scientific metaphors, several factors interact complicatedly to determine their different ways throughout the course of semantic integration as well as the unique hemispheric involvement supporting the fine-coarse semantic coding theory.

Due to the unique properties of scientific metaphors, non-native speakers have difficulty experiencing deep integration of meaning. In addition, few studies have addressed the qualitative differences in non-literal representations in the brains of native and non-native speakers. The present study may be of help to give some sort of significant implications for the related further studies. In this important but surprisingly under-researched area, future research could be directed towards comparing the dynamics of processing scientific metaphors with other forms of metaphors such as conventional and poetic metaphors between native and non-native speakers. Meanwhile, the present study only investigated the electrophysiological differences between scientific metaphors and daily literal expressions which could be resulted by both metaphoric effect and scientific effect. Further research could explore the influence of those two effects independently by adding scientific literal expressions as stimuli to reveal the unique processing of scientific metaphors in the bilingual brain.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee in Shaanxi Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

XT contributed to conception and design of the study. XL, YH, and SH performed the data collection. LS, XT, XL, XG, ZY, and ZM performed the analysis. LS and XT wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The current research was funded by the National Social Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 17BYY092).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anna, B. C., and Roberto, R. H. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetries in processing L1 and L2 idioms: effects of salience and context. Brain Lang. 116, 136–150. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arzouan, Y., Goldstein, A., and Faust, M. (2007a). Brainwaves are stethoscopes: ERP correlates of novel metaphor comprehension. Brain Res. 1160, 69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arzouan, Y., Goldstein, A., and Faust, M. (2007b). Dynamics of hemispheric activity during metaphor comprehension: electrophysiological measures. NeuroImage 36, 222–231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bambini, V., Bertini, C., Schaeken, W., Stella, A., and Di Russo, F. (2016). Disentangling metaphor from context: an ERP study. Front. Psychol. 7:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beeman, M. J. (1998). “Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension,” in Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. eds. M. Beeman and C. Chiarello (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 255–284.

Google Scholar

Beeman, M. J. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 512–518. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowdle, B. F., and Gentner, D. (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychol. Rev. 112, 193–216. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braunstein, V., Ischebeck, A., Brunner, C., Grabner, R. H., Stamenov, M., and Neuper, C. (2012). Investigating the influenceof proficiency on semantic processing in bilinguals: an ERP and ERD/S analysis. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 72, 421–438.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F. M., and Costa, A. (2011). Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching. Front. Psychol. 2:399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., Peng, X., and Zhao, Y. (2013). An ERP study on metaphor comprehension in the bilingual brain. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 36, 505–517. doi: 10.1515/cjal-2013-0034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulson, S., and Van Petten, C. (2002). Conceptual integration and metaphor: an event-related potential study. Mem. Cogn. 30, 958–968. doi: 10.3758/bf03195780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, T., and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: from identification to decision. Biling. Lang. Congn. 5, 175–197. doi: 10.1017/s1366728902003012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, T., van Heuven, W. J. B., and Grainger, J. (1998). Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychol. Belg. 38, 177–196. doi: 10.5334/pb.933

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01839-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cogn. Sci. 7, 155–170. doi: 10.1016/s0364-0213(83)80009-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D., and Wolff, P. (1997). Alignment in the processing of metaphor. J. Mem. Lang. 37, 331–355. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giora, R. (2003). On Our Mind: Salience, Context, and Figurative Language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Goldstein, A., Arzouan, Y., and Faust, M. (2012). Killing a novel metaphor and reviving a dead one: ERP correlates of metaphor conventionalization. Brain Lang. 123, 137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Biling. Lang. Congn. 1, 67–81. doi: 10.1017/s1366728998000133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahne, A., and Friederici, A. D. (2001). Processing a second language: late learners' comprehension mechanisms as revealed by event-related brain potentials. Biling. Lang. Congn. 4, 123–141. doi: 10.1017/s1366728901000232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heidlmayr, K., Hemforth, B., Moutier, S., and Isel, F. (2015). Neurodynamics of executive control processes in bilinguals: evidence from ERP and source reconstruction analyses. Front. Psychol. 6:17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankowiak, K., Naranowicz, M., and Rataj, K. (2021). Metaphors are like lenses: electrophysiological correlates of novel meaning processing in bilingualism. Int. J. Biling. 25, 668–686. doi: 10.1177/1367006921996820

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankowiak, K., Rataj, K., and Naskręcki, R. (2017). To electrify bilingualism: electrophysiological insights into bilingual metaphor comprehension. PLoS One 12:e0175578. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, X., Tan, Y., and Zhou, X. (2009). Processing the universal quantifier during sentence comprehension: ERP evidence. Neuropsychologia 47, 1799–1815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., and Holcomb, P. J. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Lang. Cognit. Process. 15, 159–201. doi: 10.1080/016909600386084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalinka, T., Christoffels, I. K., and Albert, C. (2018). On the flexibility of bilingual language control: the effect of language context. Biling. Lang. Congn. 22, 555–568. doi: 10.1017/s1366728918000329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotz, S. A. (2009). A critical review of ERP and fMRI evidence on L2 syntactic processing. Brain Lang. 109, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, J. F., and Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 149–174. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, V. T., and Curran, T. (2013). ERP evidence for conceptual mappings and comparison processes during the comprehension of conventional and novel metaphors. Brain Lang. 127, 484–496. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemhofer, K., Dijkstra, T., Schriefers, H., Baayen, H., Grainger, J., and Zwitserlood, P. (2008). Native language influences on word recognition in a second language: a mega-study. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 12–31. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litcofsky, K. A., and Van Hell, J. G. (2017). Switching direction affects switching costs: behavioral, ERP and time-frequency analyses of intra-sentential codeswitching. Neuropsychologia 97, 112–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Calderon, J., and Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mashal, N., Borodkin, K., Maliniak, O., and Faust, M. (2015). Hemispheric involvement in native and non-native comprehension of conventional metaphors. J. Neurolinguistics 35, 96–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., and Grainger, J. (2009). Language effects in second language learners and proficient bilinguals investigated with event-related potentials. J. Neurolinguistics 22, 281–300. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miriam, F., Elisheva, B. A., and Nili, V. (2012). Semantic processing in native and second language: evidence from hemispheric differences in fine and coarse semantic coding. Brain Lang. 123, 228–233. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, E. M., Fornells, A. R., and Laine, M. (2008). Event-related potentials (ERPs) in the study of bilingual language processing. J. Neurolinguistics 21, 477–508. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, E. M., and Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: an electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish–English bilinguals. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 205–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naranowicz, M., Jankowiak, K., Kakuba, P., Bromberek-Dyzman, K., and Thierry, G. (2022). In a bilingual mood: mood affects Lexico-semantic processing differently in native and non-native languages. Brain Sci. 12:316. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12030316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, A. J., Tremblay, A., Nichols, E. S., Neville, H. J., and Ullman, M. T. (2012). The influence of language proficiency on lexical semantic processing in native and late learners of English. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1205–1223. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, S., Gonzalez, C., and Wicha, N. Y. Y. (2014). The fox and the Cabra: an ERP analysis of reading code switched nouns and verbs in bilingual short stories. Brain Res. 1557, 127–140. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, S. D., Van Hooff, J. C., and Havelka, J. (2010). Language representation and processing in fluent bilinguals: electrophysiological evidence for asymmetric mapping in bilingual memory. Neuropsychologia 48, 1426–1437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, N. A., Segalowitz, N., O'Brien, I., and Yamasaki, N. (2004). Semantic priming in a first and second language: evidence from reaction time variability and event-related brain potentials. J. Neurolinguistics 17, 237–262. doi: 10.1016/s0911-6044(03)00055-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proverbio, A. M., Cok, B., and Zani, A. (2002). Electrophysiological measures of language processing in bllinguals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 994–1017. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, B., Kroger, S., Hill, H., Windmann, S., Hermann, C., and Abraham, A. (2012). Can clouds dance? Part 2: an ERP investigation of passive conceptual expansion. Brain Cogn. 80, 301–310. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, G. L., and Seger, C. A. (2009). Neural correlates of metaphor processing: the roles of figurativeness, familiarity and difficulty. Brain Cogn. 71, 375–386. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, S. R., Alexander, M., Haeußinger, F. B., Ernst, L. H., Hamm, F., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2014). Beyond the N400: complementary access to early neural correlates of novel metaphor comprehension using combined electrophysiological and haemodynamic measurements. Cortex 53, 45–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taft, M., Li, J., and Aryanti, T. (2021). Why do non-native English speakers show masked form priming when native speakers do not? J. Second Lang. Stud. 4, 204–223. doi: 10.1075/jsls.21007.taf

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, X., Qi, S., Jia, X., Wang, B., and Ren, W. (2017a). Comprehension of scientific metaphors: complementary processes revealed by ERP. J. Neurolinguistics 42, 12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, X., Qi, S., Wang, B., Jia, X., and Ren, W. (2017b). The temporal dynamics underlying the comprehension of scientific metaphors and poetic metaphors. Brain Res. 1655, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, X., Wei, R., and Hu, W. (2016). An ERP study on the cognitive neural mechanism of scientific language. J. Psychol. Sci. 38, 1071–1079. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20160508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Der Meij, M., Cuetos, F., Carreiras, M., and Barber, H. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of language switching in second language learners. Psychophysiology 48, 44–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01039.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., and Chwilla, D. J. (2009). Role of inhibition in language switching: evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. Cognition 110, 84–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., and Chen, B. (2020). A database of Chinese-English bilingual speakers: ratings of the age of acquisition and familiarity. Front. Psychol. 11:554785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., and Jankowiak, K. (2021). Electrophysiological insights into the role of proficiency in bilingual novel and conventional metaphor processing. Linguist. Approach. Bilingualism 4. doi: 10.1075/lab.20051.wan

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WeberFox, C. M., and Neville, H. J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 231–256. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiland, H., Bambini, V., and Schumacher, P. B. (2014). The role of literal meaning in figurative language comprehension: evidence from masked priming ERP. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, J., Yang, J., and Zhao, Q. (2014). Chinese–English bilinguals processing temporal–spatial metaphor. Cogn. Process. 15, 269–281. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0621-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xun, E., Rao, G., Xiao, X., and Zang, J. (2016). The construction of the BCC corpus in the age of big data. Corpus. Linguist. 3, 93–109+118.

Google Scholar

Yang, F. G., Bradley, K., Huq, M., Wu, D. L., and Krawczyk, D. C. (2013). Contextual effects on conceptual blending in metaphors: an event-related potential study. J. Neurolinguistics 26, 312–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2012.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeller, J. P. (2020). Code-switching does not equal code-switching. An event-related potentials study on switching from L2 German to L1 Russian at prepositions and nouns. Front. Psychol. 11:1387. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: scientific metaphors, bilingual, event-related potentials, N400, late negativity

Citation: Shen L, Li X, Huang S, Huang Y, Gao X, You Z, Mao Z and Tang X (2022) Comprehending scientific metaphors in the bilingual brain: Evidence from event-related potentials. Front. Psychol. 13:1037525. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1037525

Received: 06 September 2022; Accepted: 16 November 2022;
Published: 06 December 2022.

Edited by:

Huili Wang, Dalian University of Technology, China

Reviewed by:

Ying Li, Zhengzhou University, China
Yue Wang, Zhengzhou University, China

Copyright © 2022 Shen, Li, Huang, Huang, Gao, You, Mao and Tang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xuemei Tang, tangxuemei@ahpu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.