Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 November 2022
Sec. Positive Psychology

The relationship between trait awe and procrastination: A mediated model with moderation

  • School of Education Science, Shanxi Normal University, Taiyuan, China

The current study aims to examine the effect of trait awe on college students’ procrastination with a focus on confirming the mediating role of stress and the moderating role of future time perspective (FTP). Measures of procrastination, trait awe, stress, and future time perspective were completed by 512 Chinese college students. The results indicate that trait awe had a negative effect on procrastination, that stress was a significant mediator between trait awe and procrastination, and that FTP moderated the mediation effect. These findings not only demonstrate the crucial role of awe in alleviating procrastination but also elucidate the underlying mechanisms and relevant populations. Limitations and directions for future research were also discussed.

Introduction

Procrastination refers to a voluntary but irrational delay of an intended course of actions (Steel, 2007). Adults, especially college students, procrastinate to varying degrees (Ferrari et al., 2005; Steel, 2007; Chu et al., 2010). Procrastination is known to harm an individual’s learning and work efficiency (He et al., 2012), even to cause physical or mental illness (Sirois, 2007; Stead et al., 2010). Therefore, it is essential to investigate the factors and mechanisms that influence college students’ procrastination.

Trait awe and procrastination

According to the conceptual model of procrastination (Procee et al., 2013), there are three prominent factors influencing procrastination: task-related, personality-related, and other factors (e.g., emotions, temptations, and coping strategies). As for personality-related factors, research indicates a significant and positive correlation between procrastination and neuroticism (Steel, 2007), and under the effect of neuroticism, individuals are more likely to procrastinate (Rozental and Carlbring, 2014). In contrast, in the Big Five, awe was significantly and negatively correlated with neuroticism (Dong, 2016). As for other factors (such as emotions), research indicates that both positive valence and negative valence of emotions are significantly correlated with procrastination (Cheng et al., 2010; Tang et al., 2017). Awe is an emotion elicited by something enormous, unexpected, and beyond one’s current knowledge structure or mental frameworks (Keltner and Haidt, 2003). Extensive research on the positive effects of awe has revealed that awe has a significant positive effect on the mental health, cognition, and behavior of an individual (Shiota et al., 2007; Cappellen et al., 2012; Rudd et al., 2012; Valdesolo and Graham, 2013). Moreover, trait emotions reflect the frequency and intensity with which individuals experience a specific emotion (Keltner, 1996; Rosenberg and Erika, 1998) and frequently exhibit social cognitions similar to state emotions (Keltner and Lerner, 2010). Trait awe is one of the trait positive emotions (Shiota et al., 2007). Since it reflects the frequency and intensity with which individuals experience the emotion of awe (Dong et al., 2013), it is unknown if it can further influence individuals’ procrastination. The broaden−and−build theory of positive emotions suggests that positive emotions broaden the range of people’s momentary thoughts and actions (Isgett and Fredrickson, 2004). Individuals who experience positive emotions are more focused and receptive to trying new approaches, creating new strategies, and pursuing originality (Gao and Tong, 2010). In other words, in this process of open-mindedness, new ideas, experiences, and actions greatly expand an individual’s thinking and actions (Strumpfer, 2006). Thus, we hypothesized that trait awe has a negative direct effect on procrastination.

Stress as a mediator

Stress is shaped by the extent to which the individual appraises the present context (Folkman, 2013). According to studies, positive emotions (e.g., gratitude, love) alter perceptions of the self and the social environment, which can reduce stress levels (Emmons and McCullough, 2003; Miller et al., 2015). According to the prototype theory, awe has two core appraisals: vastness, which refers to the perception of stimuli as perceptually and conceptually vast; and need for accommodation, which refers to the need to fill, fix, or even rebuild the current knowledge structure in order to accommodate new information from experience (Keltner and Haidt, 2003). State and trait awe have been found to predict greater comfort with revising mental schema to assimilate novel information (Shiota et al., 2007) and the perception of having more time to help others (Rudd et al., 2012). Indeed, awe shifts individuals’ appraisals of the social environment and the self. As per our research, state and trait awe are related to shifts in self-appraisals, producing a “small self.” Reducing self-focus and altering self-evaluation are strong predictors of reduced stress (Libby et al., 2005; Ayduk and Kross, 2008; Ayduk et al., 2010; Kross and Ayduk, 2011). According to Bai et al. (2021), individuals with higher trait awe report less daily stress.

Folkman’s (2013) model of stress and coping argues that individuals’ perceptions of stress are evaluated in two processes: a primary assessment focusing on the nature of the event and the threat level, and a secondary assessment focusing on personal resources and coping strategies. The study found that stress is associated with negative coping styles (Arsenio and Loria, 2014) and that individuals were more likely to adopt avoidant coping styles in the face of high stress (Restubog et al., 2011; Na et al., 2015). Procrastination is an avoidance behavior (Zhang et al., 2019a) and its antecedent is the motivational trade-off between avoidance and approaching the task (Zhang and Feng, 2017). According to empirical research, stress is significantly positively correlated with procrastination (Stead et al., 2010) and has a positive effect on procrastination (Dümez and Barut, 2016; Dou et al., 2019). Thus, we hypothesized that stress mediates the relationship between trait awe and procrastination.

FTP as a moderator

Future time perspective (FTP) is an individual’s cognitive, affective, and action tendencies toward future social development and self-development, which can be considered an individual’s personality traits toward the future (Song, 2004). FTP is closely associated with an individual’s emotions, cognition, motivation, and social adjustment (Kooij et al., 2018). The expectancy-value theory (Volder and Lens, 1982) suggests that individuals with a high FTP are better able to assess the future utility of their current behavior and thus initiate and thus regulate it in time (Ni et al., 2018). Awe increases the patience to immerse oneself in the present moment (Rudd et al., 2012; Dewitte et al., 2016; Zhou, 2019) and the willingness to spend more time in the present (Chen-Bo et al., 2010; Devoe et al., 2013). According to the Protective-Protective model (Fergus and Zimmerman, 2005), there may be an interaction between different protective factors that affect the same outcome variable. For instance, the interaction between one protective factor (trait awe) and the outcome variable (procrastination or stress) may differ based on the other protective factor (FTP). The two interaction hypotheses are enhancing and antagonistic. The enhancing hypothesis suggests that individuals with a high FTP will regulate their current behavior based on the value of future tasks, and those also with a high trait awe may be more patient and focused on the current task rather than procrastinating. Likewise, individuals with a better assessment of the future have a reduced focus on the self, which relieves stress. According to the antagonistic hypothesis, individuals with higher FTP will not be more facilitated by trait awe. Given the paucity of prior research on trait awe and FTP, this study hypothesized that FTP might moderate the effect of trait awe on procrastination without making any assumptions about the moderating model.

FTP moderated the effect of negative factors on individual behavior (Surendra et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2016; Yang et al., 2021). According to the limited self-control theory (Baumeister and Tice, 2007), self-control resources are limited and can be depleted by stress (Tan and Guo, 2008; Zhan and Ren, 2012), which can negatively predict college students’ procrastination (Zhang et al., 2017, 2019b). FTP is a motivational characteristic with a motivational factor (Lv and Huang, 2011). Motivation alleviates the depletion of self-control resources (Muraven et al., 1998, 2006), thereby reducing procrastination. Higher FTP with a more robust motivational component may reduce procrastination and the effect of self-control resource depletion in stressful situations, whereas lower FTP may not. Thus, we hypothesized that FTP moderated the mediating effect of stress.

A summary of the current study

We intend to investigate the association between trait awe and procrastination, as well as the potential mechanisms underlying this association. Based on the above arguments and evidence, the following hypotheses can be formulated: (1) Trait awe has a negative direct effect on procrastination. (2) Stress mediates the relationship between trait awe and procrastination. (3) FTP moderated the effect of stress as a mediator. Specifically, people higher in trait awe reported lower stress levels, which reduced procrastination, all moderated by FTP. The conceptual model is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The hypothesized moderated mediation model.

Materials and methods

Participants

As we found no existing literature on this topic, we adopted the effect size below the medium (one-tailed, 0.10 ≤ r ≤ 0.30) to calculate the sample size by G*power 3.1 prior to the data collection (Faul et al., 2009), as there is no existing literature on this topic. The sample size analysis of the point biserial correlation model (α = 0.05 and 80% power) revealed that we required between 64 and 614 participants at most.

A total of 528 Chinese college students were recruited from a Chinese university and 512 valid questionnaires were obtained for a response rate of 96.9%. On average, participants were 19.7 (SD = 1.7) years from 18 to 25. There were 233 females (43.5%) and 289 males (56.5%) in the sample.

Procedure

The procedures were approved by the ethics board of the first author’s university, and the data was collected by recruiting participants from the university. Within 10 min of reading the instructions, participants completed several scales and provided demographic information. Participants were then thanked and compensated.

Data analysis

SPSS 22.0 was used to calculate descriptive statistics and correlations between study variables. Then, using the PROCESS macro for SPSS and 5,000 bootstrap samples, direct and indirect effects were calculated, and the mediation and moderation effects of stress and FTP were examined for statistical significance.

Measures

Procrastination

General Procrastination Scale was developed by Lay (1986) and localized by Chu et al. (2010). The scale consists of 20 items (e.g., “I often find myself doing work that should have been done a few days ago”), scored on a 5-point scale (“1” for not at all and “5” for fully), and higher scores indicate severe procrastination. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87.

Trait awe

The Dispositional Positive Emotions Scale was developed by Shiota et al. (2006) and localized by Dong (2016) for localized revision. The six-item scale (e.g., “I often feel awe”) was scored on a 7-point scale (“1” for strongly disagree and “7” for strongly agree). Moreover, higher scores indicated that the individual had a higher level of trait awe. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85.

Stress

Stress Scale for College Students developed by Li and Weng-Boey (2002) contained 30 items (e.g., desire for true love and not getting it) and was scored on a 4-point scale (“1” for no stress and “4” for severe stress) and higher scores indicating significant stress. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93.

FTP

The Future Time Perspective Scale developed by Song (2004) consists of 20 items (e.g., I know that there are many tasks to be done in the future). On a 4-point scale (“1” means not at all and “4” means fully), the scale was scored, with higher scores indicating greater levels of FTP. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90.

Control variable

In some relevant studies (Piff et al., 2015; Li et al., 2019), we found that gender and age were frequently significantly associated with the studied variables, which may have had an impact on the results; therefore, age and gender were controlled for.

Results

Common method variance

Harman’s single-factor test was utilized to statistically verify the common method variance bias. The results indicated that the eigenvalues of the variance of 16 factors were greater than 1, and the initial eigenvalues of the variance of the first component were 16.61%, which was below 40%. At the same time, the VIFs of the Collinearity Diagnostics results of all predictors were less than 1.07 (VIF < 10). Therefore, the sample data contained no common method bias.

Preliminary analyses

Table 1 presents the mean values, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations of the variables measured in this study. As predicted, trait awe was significantly negatively correlated with stress (r = −0.22, p < 0.01) and procrastination (r = −0.32, p < 0.01), and significantly positively correlated with FTP (r = 0.16, p < 0.01). Stress was significantly positively correlated with procrastination (r = 0.35, p < 0.01) and significantly negatively related to FTP (r = −0.14, p < 0.01). FTP was significantly negatively correlated with procrastination (r = −0.19, p < 0.01).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean values, standard deviations, and correlations for the key variables.

The mediating effect of stress

Using Hayes’s (2013) Model 4 within the SPSS macro program Process to examine the mediating effect of stress while controlling for gender and age. The results are shown in Tables 2 and 3, where trait awe significantly predicted procrastination (β = −0.33, t = −7.80, p < 0.001), and trait awe similarly significantly predicted procrastination when stress was introduced (β = −0.25, t = −6.08, p < 0.001). Trait awe significantly negatively predicted stress (β = −0.25, t = −6.07, p < 0.001), and stress significantly positively predicted procrastination (β = 0.30, t = 6.82, p < 0.001). The 95% CI [−0.11, −0.04] for the mediating effect of stress, which did not contain “0” (Table 3), indicated a significant mediating effect of stress, with the direct effect (−0.253) and the mediating effect (−0.074) accounting for the total effect (−0.327) of 77.37 and 22.63%.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mediated model for stress.

The moderating effect of FTP

Hayes’s (2013) Mode1 15 in Process 4.0 was utilized to test the mediated model with moderation, controlling for gender and age. The results (see Table 3) revealed that trait awe × FTP and stress × FTP significantly predicted procrastination (trait awe × FTP: β = 0.10, t = 2.67, p < 0.05; stress × FTP: β = 0.10, t = 2.14, p < 0.05), indicating that FTP moderated the direct effect of trait awe on procrastination and the mediating effect of stress.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Analysis of FTP as moderator.

For individuals lower in FTP (M – 1SD), simple slope analysis indicated (Figure 2) that trait awe significantly negatively predicted procrastination (simple slope = −0.33, t = −6.19, p < 0.001), whereas for individuals higher in FTP (M + 1SD), trait awe also significantly negatively predicted procrastination but to a lesser extent (simple slope = −0.14, t = −2.52, p < 0.05). As individuals’ FTP level increases, the predictive effect of trait awe on procrastination decreases.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The moderating role of FTP in the relationship between trait awe and procrastination.

Figure 3 demonstrates that for individuals with lower FTP (M – 1SD), stress significantly positively predicted FTP (simple slope = 0.17, t = 2.70, p < 0.05), whereas for individuals with higher FTP (M + 1SD), stress also significantly positively predicted FTP but to a greater extent (simple slope = 0.37, t = 6.09, p < 0.001). The results suggest that the predictive effect of stress on procrastination increased as the FTP of the participants increased.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The moderating role of FTP in the relationship between stress and procrastination.

Discussion

Trait awe and procrastination

First, trait awe negatively predicted procrastination, i.e., individuals higher in trait awe had lower degrees of procrastination. Although no empirical studies directly examine the relationship between these two variables, it is consistent with previous research on positive emotions (Tang et al., 2017). Consistent with the broaden−and − build theory of positive emotions (Isgett and Fredrickson, 2004), the results suggest that positive emotions can prompt individuals to consider as many action possibilities as possible, thereby expanding their action scope and reducing procrastination. The question of “do it now or do it later?” is the crux of whether procrastination behaviors occur (Zhang and Feng, 2017), and awe focuses people’s attention on what is occurring in the present (Rudd et al., 2012). Therefore, those higher in trait awe make a decision: do it now.

The mediating role of stress

Second, consistent with the model of stress and coping, stress significantly predicts procrastination negatively (Folkman, 2013). Procrastinators procrastinate because, after assessing, they must reduce negative experiences or perceptions, such as stress-induced anxiety, through avoidance. The result examined the indirect effects of trait awe on procrastination via stress and found that individuals higher in trait awe experienced less stress, which, in turn, reduced individual procrastination, which is consistent with hypothesis and previous research (Dümez and Barut, 2016; Dou et al., 2019; Bai et al., 2021). Short-Term Mood Regulation (Sirois and Fuschia, 2014; STMR) suggests that procrastination is a failure of self-regulation due to individuals abandon future-valuable behaviors in order to avoid negative emotions (Ackerman, 2005). Trait awe reduces an individual’s stress so that they do not have to give up future-valuable behaviors, thereby reducing procrastination.

The moderating role of FTP

This study discovered that FTP moderated the direct effect of trait awe on procrastination, as the effect of trait awe on procrastination decreased as FTP increased.

The result tested the antagonistic hypothesis rather than the enhancing hypothesis (Fergus and Zimmerman, 2005). Both trait awe and FTP, as protective factors, enable individuals to focus or initiate present behavior, but awe only increases the patience to immerse oneself in the present moment (Rudd et al., 2012; Dewitte et al., 2016; Zhou, 2019), and the willingness to spend more time on the current behavior without pointing toward the future. However, individuals high in FTP will initiate the behavior in a timely manner when they recognize its value (Tabachnick et al., 2008). In addition, they can also use future objectives to regulate current behavior (Yang et al., 2021). Individuals reduce procrastination as a result of more specific future goals and greater behavioral commitment (Song and Feng, 2017). At high levels of FTP, trait awe did not have a more positive effect. Therefore, the effect of trait awe on procrastination decreased with increasing FTP.

FTP moderated the effect of stress on procrastination. FTP can be seen as a motivational trait with a motivational component (Lv and Huang, 2011), and motivation attenuates the effect of resource depletion on self-control (Muraven et al., 1998, 2006), which alleviates the stress-induced depletion of self-control resources and reduce procrastination. In other words, a higher FTP has a greater motivational component than a lower FTP, allowing individuals to retain more self-control resources and avoid procrastination due to the depletion of self-control resources. At high levels of FTP, self-control resources are less depleted, and the effect of stress on procrastination is reduced.

Limitations and future directions

Several limitations of the current study necessitate additional research. First, cross-sectional design requires caution when interpreting the causal direction of associations. It is difficult to meet the time series in causality with cross-sectional research, and it cannot guarantee that the independent variable occurs before the dependent variable. Experimental studies in future research would further strengthen the validity of the conclusions (e.g., Induce awe in the lab.). Second, this study relied solely on self-reported measures. Future research may employ multiple measures to ensure the objectivity of its findings. Thirdly, only Chinese college students were included in the study sample; future research should explore the similarities and differences in procrastination among various age groups, occupations, and cultures.

Implications

Despite the aforementioned limitations, this study aimed to investigate stress and FTP as an explanatory mechanism for the association between trait awe and procrastination. People higher in trait awe are less prone to procrastinate due to decreased stress levels. Theoretically, these results contribute to the current literature and may offer a solid foundation for future study on the effect of awe on individual behavior. This study sheds light, from a practical standpoint, on how to reduce procrastination among individuals. It may be possible to reduce procrastination by cultivating the trait of awe and evoking awe in individuals.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethical approval

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Department of Psychology, Shanxi Normal University. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JS, CZ, and TL contributed to the study’s conception and design and data collection and analysis. JS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CZ made great contributions in the writing and revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the College Teaching Reform and Innovation Project of Shanxi Province (General Project) [number: J2021276] and the Postgraduate Education Innovation Project of Shanxi Province [number 2021Y449].

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ackerman, D. S. (2005). My instructor made me do it: task characteristics of procrastination. J. Mark. Educ. 27, 5–13. doi: 10.1177/0273475304273842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsenio, W. F., and Loria, S. (2014). Coping with negative emotions: connections with Adolescents’ academic performance and stress. J. Genet. Psychol. 175, 76–90. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2013.806293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayduk, Ö., and Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of recovery: Selfdistanced analysis of negative experiences reduces blood pressure reactivity. Psychol. Sci. 19, 229–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02073.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayduk,, Kross, Ö., and Ethan,. (2010). From a distance: implications of spontaneous self-distancing for adaptive self-reflection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 809–829. doi: 10.1037/a0019205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, Y., Ocampo, J., Jin, G., Chen, S., and Keltner, D. (2021). Awe, daily stress, and elevated life satisfaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 120, 837–860. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Tice, V. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappellen, V., Saroglou, P., and Vassilis,. (2012). Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psychol. Relig. Spiritual. 4, 223–236. doi: 10.1037/a0025986

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen-Bo, Z., Sanford, E., and DeVoe,. (2010). You are how you eat. Psychol. Sci. 21, 619–622. doi: 10.1177/0956797610366090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S. P., Li, M., and Zhang, H. (2010). A survey on undergraduates’ procrastination behavior and its relationship with meta-cognitive and emotion. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 18, 238–240.

Google Scholar

Chu, Q., Xiao, R., and Lin, Q. (2010). Research on the procrastination behavior and characteristics of university students. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 18, 970–972. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2010.08.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devoe, S. E., House, J., and Zhong, C. B. (2013). Fast food and financial impatience: a socioecological approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 476–494. doi: 10.1037/a0033484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewitte, S., Joye,, and Yannick,. (2016). Up speeds you down. Awe-evoking monumental buildings trigger behavioral and perceived freezing. J. Environ. Psychol. 47, 112–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, R. (2016). On awe and subjective well-being of undergraduates. Educ. Teach. Res. 30, 31–40. doi: 10.13627/j.cnki.cdjy.2016.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, R., Peng, K. P., and Yu, F. (2013). Positive emotion: awe. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1996–2005.

Google Scholar

Dou, F., Wang, M., and Wang, S. (2019). The impact of perceived stress on academic procrastination of postgraduate: a moderated mediation model. Psychol. Res. 12, 272–277.

Google Scholar

Dümez, H., and Barut, Y. (2016). Rational emotive behavior based on academic procrastination prevention training Programme of effectiveness. Particip. Educ. Res. 3, 1–13. doi: 10.17275/per.15.36.3.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmons, R. A., and McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fergus, S., and Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annu. Rev. Public Health 26, 399–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, J. R., O’Callghan, J., and Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: arousal and avoidance delay among adults. North Am. J. Psychol. 7, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Folkman, S. (2013). “Stress: Appraisal and Coping” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine. eds. M. D. Gellman and J. R. Turner (New York, NY: Springer). 1913–1915. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-1005-9_215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z. L., and Tong, H. J. (2010). Role of positive emotions: the broaden—and—build theory. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 2:4. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2010.02.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Method. Soc. Sci. 51, 335–337. doi: 10.1111/jedm.12050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, C., Xie, Y. H., and Ma, L. (2012). Impact of procrastination on work performance: the moderating effects of pressure preference and task type. Psychol. Sci. 35, 968–972. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2012.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isgett, S. F., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 359, 1367–1378. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D. (1996). “Chapter 21 - Facial Expressions of Emotion and Personality,” in Handbook of emotion, aging, and the life course. eds. C. Malatesta-Magai and S. H. McFadden (San Diego: Academic Press). 385–401. doi: 10.1016/B978-012464995-8/50022-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D., and Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognit. Emot. 17, 297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D., and Lerner, J. S. (2010). “Emotion” in The handbook of social psychology. eds. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindsay (McGraw-Hill). 5th edn. 312–347.

Google Scholar

Kooij, D. T. A. M., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., and Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 103, 867–893. doi: 10.1037/apl0000306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kross, E., and Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-distancing. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 187–191. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408883

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research article on procrastination. J. Res. Pers. 20, 474–495. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(86)90127-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. J., Dou, K., Wang, Y. J., and Nie, Y. G. (2019). Why awe promotes prosocial behaviors? The mediating effects of future time perspective and self-transcendence meaning of life. Front. Psychol. 10:1140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., and Weng-Boey, K. A. M. (2002). Development of stress scale for college student. Chin. J. App. Psychol. 1, 27–32. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1006-6020.2002.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libby, L. K., Eibach, R. P., and Gilovich, T. (2005). Here’s looking at me: the effect of memory perspective on assessments of personal change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 50–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, H. C., and Huang, X. T. (2011). Conceptual evolution and theoretical model of time perspective. J. South. Univ. 37:4. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1673-9841.2011.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. G., Kahle, S., Lopez, M., and Hastings, P. D. (2015). Compassionate love buffers stress-reactive mothers from fight-or-flight parenting. Dev. Psychol. 51, 36–43. doi: 10.1037/a0038236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., and Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 524–537. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., and Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 774–789. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Na, H., Dancy, B. L., and Park, C. (2015). College student engaging in cyberbullying victimization: cognitive appraisals, coping strategies, and psychological adjustments. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 29, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2015.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, Y. K., Guo, T. F., and Wang, M. H. (2018). Relationship among trait procrastination, intertemporal choice, and future time perspective. Chin. Ment. Health J. 32, 765–771. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2018.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piff, P. K., Dietze, P., Feinberg, M., Stancato, D. M., and Keltner, D. (2015). Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 883–899. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Procee, R., Kamphorst, B. A., Wissen, A. V., Meyer, J., Action, N. H., and Ethiek, L. W. (2013). A formal model of procrastination. Benelux Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 152–159.

Google Scholar

Restubog, S., Garcia, P., Toledano, L. S., Amarnani, R. K., Tolentino, L. R., and Tang, R. L. (2011). Yielding to (cyber)-temptation: exploring the buffering role of self-control in the relationship between organizational justice and cyberloafing behavior in the workplace. J. Res. Pers. 45, 247–251. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg,, and Erika, L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the Organization of Affect. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 247–270. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozental, A., and Carlbring, P. (2014). Understanding and treating procrastination: a review of a common self-regulatory failure. Psychology 5, 1488–1502. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.513160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., and Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands People’s perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1177/0956797612438731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with big five personality and attachment style. J. Posit. Psychol. 1, 61–71. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cognit. Emot. 21, 944–963. doi: 10.1080/02699930600923668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois, F. M. (2007). “I'll look after my health, later”: a replication and extension of the procrastination–health model with community-dwelling adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois,, and Fuschia, M. (2014). Absorbed in the moment? An investigation of procrastination, absorption, and cognitive failures. Pers. Individ. Differ. 71, 30–34. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, Q. Z. (2004). Theoretical and empirical research on future time perspective of university students(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Southwest University.

Google Scholar

Song, M. G., and Feng, T. Y. (2017). How time perspective influences procrastination: mediating roles of time discounting. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 33:8. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.06.06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stead, R., Shanahan, M. J., and Neufeld, R. (2010). “I'll go to therapy, eventually”: procrastination, stress, and mental health. Pers. Individ. Differ. 49, 175–180. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychol. Bull. 133, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strumpfer, D. J. W. (2006). Positive emotions, positive emotionality and their contribution to Fortigenic living: a review. South Afr. J. Psychol. 36, 144–167. doi: 10.1177/008124630603600109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surendra, K., Sia, B., Chandra, S., and Duari,. (2015). Gender discrimination and work engagement: moderating role of future time perspective. South Asian J. Hum. Res. Manag. 2, 58–84. doi: 10.1177/2322093715577443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, S. E., Miller, R. B., and Relyea, G. E. (2008). The relationships among Students’ future-oriented goals and subgoals, perceived task instrumentality, and task-oriented self-regulation strategies in an academic environment. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 629–642. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, S. H., and Guo, Y. Y. (2008). A limited resource model of self-control and the relevant studies. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 16:3. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-3611.2008.03.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Q. X., Yang, D. C., Hu, X. L., and Li, Q. Q. (2017). The relationship between personality and academic procrastination in college students: intermediary effect of emotional valence. J. Psychiatry 30:4. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.2095-9346.2017.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., and Graham, J. (2013). Awe, uncertainty, and agency detection. Psychol. Sci. 25, 170–178. doi: 10.1177/0956797613501884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volder, M. D., and Lens, W. (1982). Academic achievement and future time perspective as a cognitive-motivational concept. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 42, 566–571. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-6341-5_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X. J., Fan, C. Y., Zhou, Z. K., Liu, Q. Q., and Lian, S. L. (2021). The relationship between mindfulness and Mobile phone addiction tendency: the roles of boredom proneness and future time perspective. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 37, 419–428. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.03.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhan, J., and Ren, J. (2012). Self-control and self-control resource. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1457–1466. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2012.01457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J. Y., Cui, X. Y., Tao, T., Wang, L. G., and Gao, W. B. (2019b). Relation among personality, self-control, and procrastination in young people. Chin. Ment. Health J. 3:6. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2019.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S. M., and Feng, T. Y. (2017). Decision model for procrastination. Psychol. Sci. 40:6. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20170533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Liu, Q. X., Long, Z., and Ai, T. (2016). The relationship between college Students' trait anxiety and internet addiction: a moderated mediation model. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 32:8. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2016.06.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Wang, S. J., Song, L. L., Chen, Y., and Zhu, Z. H. (2019a). Relation of procrastination to experiential avoidance and psychological capital among employees in international corporation. Chin. Ment. Health J. 33:4. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2019.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, C., Zhai, L., and Wang, C. (2017). Mediating effect of self-control on the relationship between Mobile phone dependence and academic procrastination in college students. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 25:4. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2017.01.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, G. F. (2019). Study of the effect of awe on patience(Unpublished master’s thesis). Sichuan Normal University.

Google Scholar

Keywords: procrastination, trait awe, stress, future time perspective, college student

Citation: Song J, Zhang C and Li T (2022) The relationship between trait awe and procrastination: A mediated model with moderation. Front. Psychol. 13:1030773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1030773

Received: 29 August 2022; Accepted: 05 October 2022;
Published: 09 November 2022.

Edited by:

Rebecca Shankland, Lumière University Lyon 2, France

Reviewed by:

Quentin Hallez, Université Lumière Lyon 2, France
Zhiyi Chen, Army Medical University, China

Copyright © 2022 Song, Zhang and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chao Zhang, cHN5emhhbmdjaGFvQDE2My5jb20=

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.