Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 December 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology

An explorative study of psychological and social factors impacting littering behavior in Vietnam

\r\nAnran Zhang*Anran Zhang1*Bo PangBo Pang2Jeawon KimJeawon Kim2Tuyet-Mai Nguyen,Tuyet-Mai Nguyen2,3Phong Tuan NhamPhong Tuan Nham4
  • 1School of Economics and Management, Xidian University, Xi’an, China
  • 2Social Marketing @ Griffith, Griffith University, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 3Department of Information and E-Commerce, Thuongmai University, Hanoi, Vietnam
  • 4School of Business Administration, VNU University of Economics and Business, Hanoi, Vietnam

Littering is a worldwide problem and Vietnam is one of the most affected countries. To enact change, not only individual cognitive determinants but also social and natural-, or environment-related variables should be taken into consideration. Although there is a large body of literature researching littering, most researchers do not distinguish the level of these factors. Thus, this research aims to investigate the interactive mechanism of these different level factors influencing the intention of the Vietnamese to stop littering, with the multi-level social-ecological model used to guide model building. The data were collected through a self-reported online questionnaire and the Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modelling (PLS-SEM) method was employed to examine the proposed conceptual framework. The results indicated that perceived behavioral control and connectedness to nature are the two main factors influencing the Vietnamese people’s intention to stop littering. Multi-group analysis results suggested the moderating effects of injunctive and descriptive norms. This research proposed a new conceptual framework and achieved unique insights into littering behavior in Vietnam, which could benefit and guide behavioral change experts, academics, and practitioners to design appropriate marketing strategies/campaigns to reduce littering.

Introduction

The world in the past few decades has been undergoing climate changes and other serious environmental alterations, which have threatened the Earth’s life-support system and human lives (Sachs, 2012). At the micro-level, littering is one of the problems hindering the sustainable development of the entire world because of its negative impacts on the environment, health, biodiversity, and even the economy (Chaudhary et al., 2021). The generation of sulfur oxides, nitrogen oxides, and carbon monoxide pollutes the air, which would further causes climate change (Thanh and Matsui, 2013). Besides, the spread of odors, flies, mosquitoes, rodents, and dust has detrimentally affected surrounding communities (World Bank Group, 2017). Cleaning up litter costs a significant amount of a city’s and/or a country’s budget, thus, preventing or reducing littering behavior or behavioral intention is important to solve this problem (Almosa et al., 2017a).

The social marketing approach—understanding behavior and designing interventions to promote voluntary change, was proven to be effective in influencing individual behavior and bringing about environmental, organizational, and even systemic change (Stead et al., 2007). As for littering, although there is a large body of literature researching littering [e.g., Cialdini et al., 1990; Schultz et al., 2013; Bateson et al., 2015; for a systematic review, refer to Almosa et al. (2017a) and Singh and Kaur (2021)], most studies focused on unilevel factors, including examining the impact of one factor (e.g., individual cognitive factor, self-monitoring, Ojedokun and Balogun, 2013), and exploring the effectiveness and mechanism of one intervention (e.g., Wever et al., 2010; Bateson et al., 2013). However, littering, as a social problem, is complex and influenced by multiple interactive factors and it would be solved effectively when understanding the interactive mechanism of relevant factors (Carvalho and Mazzon, 2019). Understanding littering behavior incorporating multi-level factors is needed. In addition, a recent systematic review of litter reduction programs by Almosa et al. (2017a) suggested that there is still a lack of social marketing use in litter reduction efforts to date in developing countries.

Vietnam is one of the most affected countries by environmental pollution and littering (Hoang et al., 2019). According to statistics from the Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment, nearly 1.8 million tonnes of trash is produced every year in Vietnam and this amount is predicted to increase in the next few years (Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment, 2019). Vietnam has taken actions to promote environmental protection and littering reduction (Thanh and Matsui, 2013; Schneider et al., 2017). For example, the Vietnam government has adopted legal documents regarding environmental protection and management policies, including the Law of Environment Protection 2014 and Decree NO. 38/NÐ-CP ON the control of wastes. There is also a fine policy about littering that imposes heavy fines of up to VNÐ 7 million (USD 301) on those who litter in public places (Vietnam News, 2018). Furthermore, the Vietnam non-government organizations (NGOs) also took actions to raise public awareness and influence behavioral change and littering, such as the events Clean-up and Recycling Day and Working with Fisherman organized by Greenhub (Nguyen et al., 2015). However, the reported behavioral change is meager (Tuoitrenews, 2015). Previous studies have demonstrated that policy or a campaign delivered without understanding what the public wants and needs creates resistance (Pang and Kubacki, 2015). Therefore, it is necessary to solve the littering problem in Vietnam with social marketing—understanding the Vietnamese littering behavior and designing effective interventions and/or campaigns to promote voluntary change.

Conducting formative research with theories helps to understand a behavior (Stead et al., 2007; Brown et al., 2010). However, using one theory always could not fully explain a complex behavior, while using multiple theories or theoretical contracts is better to explain or predict one behavior (Carvalho and Mazzon, 2019). For example, the theory of planned behavior (TPB), which argues that behavioral intention is determined by attitude, perceived behavioral control (PBC), and social norms (Ajzen, 1991), is one of the widely used theories to explain human behavior (Leeuw et al., 2015). However, social norms are always examined as injunctive and descriptive norms (e.g., Pang et al., 2018), which come from the theory of normative social behavior (Rimal, 2008). In addition to intrinsic factors, the extrinsic nature- or environment-related variables (Deci and Ryan, 2000), particularly emotional connectedness to nature and environmental knowledge, were incorporated into the TPB and were found could better explain or predict pro-environmental behavior (Clark et al., 2019), such as protecting the remnant vegetation (Gosling and Williams, 2010) and tourists bringing litter down the mountain (Hu et al., 2018). It is worth noting that environmental knowledge is the cognitive dimension of environmental consciousness (Lafuente and Sanchez, 2010; Sharma and Bansal, 2013), which has not been examined for predicting littering behavioral intention. In addition, previous studies that added variables to TPB still examined them in an extended TPB framework, which did not distinguish the level of these factors (Yadav and Pathak, 2016; Hu et al., 2018; Ibrahim et al., 2021). This manuscript, thus, explores the factors influencing Vietnamese littering behavior by incorporating connectedness to nature and environmental consciousness into the TPB. When considering all the individual-, social-, and environmental-level factors, the multi-level social ecological model (SEM) is needed as a theoretical framework to guide model building and intervention development (Almosa et al., 2017b).

Based on the above practical limitations and research gaps, the purpose of this research is 3-fold. First, this study aims to insight into the littering problem in Vietnam by predicting Vietnamese intention to stop littering. Second, this study aims to explore the multi-level factors affecting Vietnamese littering intention by incorporating emotional connectedness to nature and environmental consciousness with the individual cognitive and social factors in the TPB. Third, this study adopted SEM to guide model building with all the individual-, social-, and environmental-level factors. Extensions and modifications will be made to the original model to provide any specificity on the context, which will also contribute to the literature in the areas of behavioral change and social marketing. The results show that individual cognitive PBC and emotional connectedness to nature are the two main factors predicting Vietnamese intention to stop littering, while environmental consciousness does not. Attitude is not always a predictor of Vietnamese intention to stop littering because of the significant moderating effect of injunctive and descriptive norms. The insights into Vietnamese littering behavior will provide implications to improve the effectiveness of existing policies and campaigns and/or to develop new policies and marketing campaigns to reduce littering behavior in Vietnam.

Literature review and hypotheses development

Littering behaviors and solutions

Littering is defined as individuals’ careless and incorrect disposal of minor amounts of waste at public venues such as on roads, at cafes, and in parks (Sibley and Liu, 2003). The waste is mainly paper, bottles, cigarette butts, food scraps, and plastic containers that are small but often have a significantly negative impact on the environment and society (Torgler et al., 2012). Since the 1970s, numerous scholars have called for the urgency of understanding the mechanism behind littering behavior (Krauss et al., 1978; Cialdini et al., 1990). To date, the literature has reached a few general agreements. For example, many researchers have found that littering is influenced by situational factors such as proximity/availability of trash bins (e.g., Schultz et al., 2013; Rasool et al., 2022) or the presence of companion (e.g., Hu et al., 2018), or by the individual demographic characteristics (e.g., age, gender, and education level) (Ojedokun and Balogun, 2013). However, littering would be not situational when the cognitive/psychological tendency and/or social norms are strong enough (Ojedokun, 2011; Ong and Sovacool, 2012). For example, the Japanese always do not litter in public places whatever the scenario (Ong and Sovacool, 2012). Besides, littering researchers also identified the impact of cognitive factors (e.g., individual cognitive factors, self-monitoring, Ojedokun and Balogun, 2013) and social factors (e.g., injunctive and descriptive norms, Bateson et al., 2015). However, most research focused on unilevel factors (e.g., Farage et al., 2021). Littering, as a social problem, is complex and influenced by multiple interactive factors and it would be solved effectively when understanding the interactive mechanism of relevant factors (Carvalho and Mazzon, 2019). Therefore, understanding littering behavior incorporating multi-level factors is needed.

Individual cognitive factor: Perceived behavioral control

Perceived behavioral control refers to the perceived ease or difficulty of performing the behavior and is determined by control beliefs about the behavior (Ajzen, 1991). According to Kals and Maes (2002), control beliefs are one significant cognition that predicts sustainable behavior. Thus, the PBC is an important cognitive factor that could predict pro-environmental behavioral intention, including recycling, conservation, littering, and many other pro-environmental behaviors (Lokhorst et al., 2014; Leeuw et al., 2015; Hu et al., 2018). For example, Hu et al. (2018) found that tourists’ PBC significantly affects their litter management behavioral intention. Moreover, the attitudinal component is directly related to intention (Kelly and Breinlinger, 1995) and the attitude toward behavior mediates the relationship between PBC and behavioral intention (Hu et al., 2018). Since littering has a negative impact on the environment and society and intention is a strong predictor for the actual behavior (Ajzen, 1991), researchers have examined the attitude toward stopping littering and the intention to stop littering to predict behavioral change (Ojedokun and Balogun, 2013). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:

H1: Perceived behavioral control impacts the intention to stop littering through a partial mediating effect of attitude toward stopping littering.

H1.1: Perceived behavioral control has a positive influence on the attitude toward stopping littering.

H1.2: Perceived behavioral control has a positive influence on the intention to stop littering.

H1.3: Attitude toward stopping littering has a positive influence on the intention to stop littering.

Individual emotional factor: Connectedness to nature

Previous studies suggested that people-environment relations might play an important role in ecological behavior (Lokhorst et al., 2014). Connectedness to nature refers to one’s connection with the natural world (Andrews, 2018). Capaldi et al. (2014) regarded connectedness to nature as a personality construct that reflects individual differences in cognitive, affective, and experiential connection with the natural environment, and found a relationship between subjective nature connectedness and happiness. However, many other researchers approved its psychometric properties and treated it as an individual’s trait level of emotional connection to the natural world (Mayer and Frantz, 2004). From this perspective, connectedness to nature was found to be a strong predictor of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors (Gosling and Williams, 2010; Lokhorst et al., 2014). For example, Gosling and Williams (2010) found that connectedness to nature has a positive effect on a farmer’s vegetation protection behavior. Lokhorst et al. (2014) found that the more landowners feel connected to nature, the higher their attitude and intention to conserve it. Reducing/stopping littering is good behavior for the natural environment (World Bank Group, 2017). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:

H2: Connectedness to nature impacts the intention to stop littering through the partial mediating effect of attitude toward stopping littering.

H2.1: Connectedness to nature has a positive influence on attitude toward stopping littering.

H2.2: Connectedness to nature has a positive influence on the intention to stop littering.

Social factors: Injunctive and descriptive norms

Littering, a behavior taking place in public places, is influenced by social pressure (Brown et al., 2010). One’s psychological decision-making process about littering is affected by whether the behavior is approved by the society in which they live, an injunctive norm, or how most others behave, a descriptive norm (Hu et al., 2018). To date, many researchers have examined the influence of injunctive and descriptive norms on people’s behavioral intentions (Leeuw et al., 2015). For example, Smith et al. (2012) found that injunctive and descriptive norms could interact to influence people’s environmental intentions. Furthermore, several studies suggested the moderating role of injunctive and descriptive norms (Liang and Shiau, 2018). For example, Liang and Shiau (2018) found the moderating effects of injunctive and descriptive norms on the relationship between customer satisfaction and repurchase intention. Fleury and Lee (2006) also found that where community members are provided with examples of success, social norms could enhance efficacy in goal formation and motivation related to physical activity. According to the social-ecological model, injunctive and descriptive norms are social/interpersonal factors, which are at a higher level than intrapersonal factors and influence individuals’ psychological decision-making process (Almosa et al., 2017b). The higher one’s injunctive and descriptive norms, the less the effects of individuals’ factors on their behavioral intention. In the littering context, injunctive and descriptive norms would moderate the formation of one’s littering intention. Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:

H3: Injunctive norms have a moderating effect on the whole model.

H4: Descriptive norms have a moderating effect on the whole model.

Environmental consciousness

In the pro-environmental context, many studies have found that environment-related variables, such as environmental information/knowledge, environmental beliefs/values, and environmental awareness/concern, play an important role in predicting behavior or behavioral intention. Lafuente and Sanchez (2010) and Sharma and Bansal (2013) argued that these are cognitive and affective dimensions of environmental consciousness. Environmental consciousness is a general concept that refers to the degree to which a person is oriented toward concern for the environment (Lin and Chang, 2012). Many studies found a significant direct effect of environmental consciousness and/or its different dimensions (e.g., environmental knowledge) on pro-environmental behavior or behavioral intention (Hu et al., 2018). Besides, researchers also supported the moderating role of environmental consciousness in predicting pro-environmental behavior or behavioral intention (Lin and Chang, 2012; Leaniz et al., 2018). For example, Leaniz et al. (2018) found that environmental consciousness moderates the relationship between the green attributes of a hotel and customer perception of the hotel’s green image, which will promote customers. Environmental consciousness is related to one’s personal values (Lafuente and Sanchez, 2010). The more one perceives themselves as environmentally conscious, the more other attributes will influence their pro-environmental behavior or behavioral intention. In turn, that will satisfy their personal values (Lin and Chang, 2012). Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:

H5: Environmental consciousness has a moderating effect on the whole model.

In summary, we propose the following modified version of a social ecological model to explain the intention to stop littering (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual model for explaining littering behavior.

Materials and methods

Data collection

The questionnaire was originally developed in English and translated to Vietnamese, and then back-translated to ensure consistency by two bilingual experts to ensure the meanings were consistent. Qualtrics was used to design the online questionnaire. Data were collected in Hanoi, the capital of Vietnam. Given the results of many field experiments which suggested that young people usually litter more than older (Ojedokun and Balogun, 2013) and the fact that students usually respond to and fill out the questionnaire positively and seriously (Bateson et al., 2013), this study selected students as potential participants. In addition, considering that people below 18 years might not fully form their own beliefs and/or values, we intended to only collect responses from adults. Taking together, the students in Hanoi above 18 years of age were selected as a sample to represent the Vietnamese. For convenience sampling, an anonymous link to the questionnaire was posted in Facebook groups of Vietnamese students in Hanoi. Two funneling questions, “Are you over 18 years old?” and “Do you live in Hanoi?” were placed at the beginning of the online survey to ensure the eligibility of the respondents. The ones who did pass the two questions would go to the end of the survey. In total, 623 valid responses were finally obtained to do data analysis.

Measurements

All the variables were measured using 7-point Likert scales. Connectedness to nature items was adopted by Mayer and Frantz (2004). The PBC and attitude items were adopted from Fielding et al. (2008). The injunctive norms were adopted from Fielding et al. (2008) and Huber et al. (2018), and the descriptive norms were adopted from Fielding et al. (2016). The environmental consciousness scale was adopted by D’Amico et al. (2016). Intention to stop littering was used to measure behavioral intention because of the negativity of littering and the scale items were adopted from Rundle-Thiele et al. (2013).

Data analysis

IBM SPSS 26.0 was used to analyze the descriptive statistics of the sample. Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) was then employed to examine the measurements and structural model using SmartPLS 3.0. Employing a component-based approach for estimation purposes (Lohmöller, 1989), the PLS-SEM is an alternative method to covariance-based SEM (usually AMOS) but it can be used with fewer indicators (1 or 2) per construct. PLS is better suited for explaining complex relationships because it avoids two problems: inadmissible solutions and factor indeterminacy (Fornell and Bookstein, 1982). Thus, this research adopted PLS-SEM to accommodate the two indicator variables (descriptive norms) and moderating effects.

There is three multi-group analysis (MGA) approaches Parametric Test, Welch–Satterthwait Test, and Henseler’s approach (Sarstedt et al., 2011). The parametric approach that initially was proposed by Keil et al. (2000) adopts the equal variances standardized t-test. The data provided should achieve the requirement of normal distribution, which does not fit the PLS path modeling’s distribution-free character. Chin et al. (2003) modified it by performing the unequal variance of standardized t-test (Welch–Satterthwait), which is more conservative but always shows the same testing results as parametric. Henseler’s PLS MGA is the most conservative approach (Henseler, 2007). Overall, the parametric test is the most widely used method (Sarstedt et al., 2011). Therefore, this research reports the p-value of the parametric, together with the Path Coefficients difference and t-value, to examine each of the proposed hypotheses.

Results

Respondents’ profiles

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristic of the respondents. All respondents were over 18 years old with a majority aged between 18 and 25 years. Almost all the respondents had attended university (97.7%). Respondents had a monthly income of around AU$50–AU$300 (74.9%). The majority were women (80.6%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Respondents’ demographic statistics.

Measurement model

A reliability and validity analysis was conducted to assess the adequacy of the measurement model. The indicator reliability was determined by loadings and the results suggested a high degree of individual item reliability of 0.7 (Nunnally, 1978). Composite reliability (CR) and Cronbach’s alpha were both used to measure the internal consistency reliability of the items in terms of a unidimensional construct as suggested by Hulland (1999). All constructs achieved acceptable or excellent reliability (all between 0.71 and 0.98, refer to Table 2). The convergent validity was assessed through the average variance extracted (AVE). All AVE values were above the acceptable value of 0.5 (Fornell and Larcker, 1981; Bagozzi and Yi, 1988).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the measurement model.

Structural model

Bootstrapping analysis was used to evaluate the direct and indirect effects of all hypothesized relationships using SmartPLS 3.0. Table 3 shows the results. The hypotheses testing results showed that PBC had a significantly positive effect on attitude toward stopping littering (β = 0.180, t = 4.395, p < 0.001) and intention to stop littering (β = 0.390, t = 8.385, p < 0.001). However, the results indicated the attitude toward stopping littering was not significantly related to the intention to stop littering (β = 0.026, t = 0.629, p > 0.1). Thus, H1.1 and H1.2 were supported, whereas H1.3 was not supported. Furthermore, the indirect effect of PBC on behavioral intention through attitude was not significant (β = 0.004, t = 0.593, p > 0.1) and therefore did not support H1. In addition, the results suggested that connectedness to nature was positively related to the intention to stop littering (β = 0.252, t = 5.991, p < 0.001) but was not significantly related to attitude toward stopping littering of general trash (β = 0.034; t = 0.849, p > 0.1). Thus, H2.1 was not supported, whereas H2.2 was supported. Furthermore, the indirect effect of connectedness to nature on behavioral intention through attitude was not significant (β = −0.001, t = 0.356, p > 0.1) and did not support H2.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the structural model.

Moderation

This study considers injunctive norms, descriptive norms, and environmental consciousness as moderators of the relationships in the proposed model. For each moderator, we first computed the mean-value using SPSS and then separated the sample into two groups: high and low. For the descriptive norms, for example, the records whose value was higher than the mean (MDNs = 3.22) were added to the high descriptive norms group (n = 293), and the records whose value was lower than the mean were added to low descriptive norms group (n = 330). Multi-group analysis (MGA) was then conducted to analyze the moderating effects using SmartPLS 3.0 as shown in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of multi-group analysis.

The results showed that the path coefficients between PBC and intention to stop littering had a significant difference between the high injunctive norms group and the low injunctive norms group (Δβ = 0.26, t = 2.79, p < 0.01). More specifically, the strength of the relationship for the high injunctive norms (βL–INs = 0.53, t = 8.51, p < 0.001) between PBC and intention to stop littering was significantly greater than for the low injunctive norms (βH–INs = 0.27, t = 4.25, p < 0.001). This result indicated that for Vietnamese who feel most others’ disapproval of littering, individuals’ PBC had a greater effect on behavioral intention to stop littering than Vietnamese who feels the society’s approval of littering. Therefore, H3 was supported.

The results also suggested the moderating role of descriptive norms. On one hand, the strength of the relationship between connectedness to nature and intention to stop littering had a significant difference between the high and low descriptive norms group (Δβ = 0.18, t = 2.30, p < 0.05). More specifically, the strength of the relationship for the low descriptive norms (βL–DNs = 0.34, t = 6.23, p < 0.001) between connectedness to nature and intention to stop littering was significantly greater than for the high descriptive norms (βH–DNs = 0.16, t = 2.79, p < 0.01). On the other hand, the path Coefficients between attitude and intention to stop littering had a significant difference between the high and low descriptive norms group (Δβ = 0.20, t = 2.67, p < 0.01). More specifically, for the Vietnamese who found most others in the community littering, the attitude toward stopping littering had a significantly positive effect on the intention to stop littering (βL–DNs = 0.13, t = 2.14, p < 0.05), while the attitude could not predict behavioral intention to stop littering for those Vietnamese who found most others do not litter (βH–DNs = −0.07, t = 1.47, p > 0.05). Thus, H4 was supported.

In addition, the results also showed the different effects of connectedness to nature on the intention to stop littering between different environmental consciousness groups. More specifically, for the Vietnamese who were more concerned about the environment, the connectedness to nature had a significantly positive effect on the intention to stop littering (βL–EC = 0.28, t = 4.79, p < 0.001), while this effect was not significant for the Vietnamese who were less concerned about the environment (βH–EC = 0.05, t = 0.59, p > 0.05). Overall, the relationship between connectedness to nature and behavioral intention was not significant (Δβ = 0.23, t = 1.82, p > 0.05). Thus, H5 was not supported. For more details please refer to Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The measurement model overall and for high/low injunctive norms, descriptive norms, and environmental consciousness. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.

Discussion and conclusion

This study sought to understand the littering behavior of Vietnamese by incorporating emotional connectedness to nature and environmental consciousness into the TPB, which focuses on cognitive and social factors. We also adopted the SEM to guide model building due to the multi-level factors. The results showed that PBC and connectedness to nature have significantly positive influences on Vietnamese intention to stop littering, whereas attitude toward stopping littering could not predict Vietnamese intention to stop littering effectively. Surprisingly, we found that injunctive and descriptive norms have moderating effects. The following section of this article discusses the results in relation to previous studies and social marketing’s potential to prevent littering.

Factors influencing Vietnamese intention to stop littering

Perceived behavioral control and connectedness to nature had significant positive influences on Vietnamese intention to stop littering, while attitude toward stopping littering could not predict Vietnamese intention to stop littering. This was partly inconsistent with previous research, Lokhorst et al. (2014) found that attitude, PBC, and connectedness to nature are all the key factors impacting pro-environmental behavioral intention, more specifically the nature conservation intention. The positive impact of connectedness to nature on intention to stop littering is similar to and extends previous research that identified the influence of connectedness to nature on behavioral intention in the context of reducing plastic use (Clark et al., 2019) and vegetarian protection (Gosling and Williams, 2010). Our finding that attitude toward stopping littering had no effect on the intention to stop littering is different from Ojedokun and Balogun’s (2013) result that attitude toward littering significantly impacts responsible environmental behavior. In addition to social norms, another possible explanation for this finding is the situational limitation, the high prevalence of littering behavior, and the low availability of trash bins in Vietnam, which limits the conversion of attitude toward stopping littering to behavior. Thus, in order to prevent the earth/globe (e.g., the environment, health, biodiversity, and even economy) from the negative effects of littering, more social marketing research in different contexts are needed to explore the factors influencing littering behavior and/or behavioral intention and further to guide targeted interventions to promote behavior change.

The moderating role of injunctive norms

Injunctive norms were found to moderate the relationship between PBC and the intention to stop littering. This is different from the results of most previous studies (e.g., Hu et al., 2018; Clark et al., 2019) where injunctive norms and PBC are treated equally as the independent predictors of pro-environmental behavioral intention. Based on the MGA results, the influence of PBC on intention to stop littering for high injunctive norms was greater than for low injunctive norms. That is, the injunctive norms had a positive moderating effect on the behavioral intention about littering. This is different from Stok et al.’s (2014) results that injunctive norms had a negative effect on behavioral intention. Probably because the behavior in Stok et al.’s (2014) study is promotional behavior (fruit taken), for which high injunctive norms would lead people to feel like they are limited freedom of acting and, thus, induce resistance to the proposed behavior (Stok et al., 2014). However, the behavior in our study is preventive (littering), for which higher injunctive norms would make people feel stronger disapproval of the unwanted behavior and thus act in the proposed direction. More social marketing research should be conducted to further explore the injunctive norms’ role and effect on various promotional and preventive behaviors. With the formative research results, designing corresponding interventions to promote positive behavior and prevent negative behavior change.

The moderating role of descriptive norms

The descriptive norms had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between connectedness to nature and intention to stop littering, and the relationship between attitude and intention to stop littering. This strengthens previous results that descriptive norms play an important role in explaining littering behavior (Bateson et al., 2013) and other pro-environmental behaviors (e.g., Leeuw et al., 2015). Besides, by distinguishing individual- and social-level factors and building a theoretical research model with SEM, we found that the descriptive norms negatively moderate the effect of connectedness to nature on the intention to stop littering. This confirms some recent opinions (e.g., Carvalho and Mazzon, 2019) that social problems are complex and often involve multiple interacting factors. Therefore, in order to solve the littering problem, interventions and/or campaigns should be designed beyond the traditional individual-focused approach and incorporate other social solutions such as promoting environmentalist models, encouraging citizen monitoring, as well as social norms approaches that can create a supportive interpersonal setting to facilitate behavioral changes.

In addition, the descriptive norms significantly moderate the relationship between attitude and intention to stop littering. This is different from most of the previous studies, where the attitude toward the behavior is always the strong predictor of behavioral intention (e.g., Kelly and Breinlinger, 1995; Leeuw et al., 2015). In our study, the attitude had a significantly positive impact on the intention to stop littering only for low descriptive norms but not for high descriptive norms. In particular, when one found most members of their community littering (low descriptive norms), the intention to stop littering was influenced by attitude, more specifically the PBC-attitude-intention indirect path. However, for the Vietnamese whose community members do not litter, their intention to stop littering was only directly influenced by PBC and connectedness to nature. Taking together, in a situation where most others littering, one may process more thoughts to determine their behavior and behavioral intention about littering. This finding reveals that Vietnamese are sensitive to and easily influenced by social cues. Future research could examine this with technology-based experiments, such as brainwave headsets.

The influence of environmental consciousness

The results showed the difference between the high and low environmental consciousness group and their influence of connectedness to nature on the intention to stop littering, though the difference did not reach a significant level. Hu et al. (2018) regard environmental consciousness as a multi-dimensional variable and found that its cognitive dimension and environmental knowledge significantly impacted tourists’ litter management behavior down the mountain. The current study adopted unidimensional environmental consciousness and found that it had no significant effect on littering behavioral intention. One possible explanation is that tourists are always more sensitive to environmental variables (vs. individuals in the city) because of their higher affinity with the natural environment. Given those formative research, social marketing interventions and/or campaigns targeting littering reduction should be designed to influence individuals’ environmental knowledge. Differently, Leaniz et al. (2018) in the context of purchasing a green product, found the moderating effect of environmental consciousness. Thus, interventions targeting environmental consciousness would be effective for promoting responsible consumption but not for reducing or preventing littering. This finding also confirms the literature on the mixed effectiveness of the approach of education, targeting providing knowledge in the hope of changing behaviors. Education per se is not serving as the behavioral determinant but only provides knowledge which is only a moderating factor.

Theoretical and practical implications

From the theoretical point of view, this study is the first to provide insight into littering behavior in Vietnam using a theoretical lens. Theory use has been scarce in the context of littering and previous research does not distinguish the level of factors influencing littering (e.g., Carvalho and Mazzon, 2019). In this study, we built a theoretical model by incorporating emotional connectedness to nature and environmental consciousness into the TPB and adopting the multi-level SEM’s principles. Therefore, this study contributes to the literature by providing a comprehensive dual-theory model with all the individual cognitive, social, and emotional nature- and environment-related factors. We found that both individuals’ cognition of PBC and emotional connectedness to nature play important roles in their littering intention. Moreover, we also found the moderating effect of injunctive norms on the relationship between PBC and behavioral intention, and the moderating effect of descriptive norms on the relationship between connectedness to nature and behavioral intention, and between attitude and behavioral intention. The findings extend our understanding of the nuisances of littering behavior in Vietnam by disentangling the interactive mechanism of multi-level factors. Overall, this study offers a new research perspective for understanding littering behavior. Such insights can be directly translated into actionable solutions to inform and help future social marketing campaign designs in order to tackle the littering issue.

From a practical review, the results of this study provided some directions for government or social marketing practitioners and even managers to take action to eliminate/reduce littering in Vietnam through three social marketing activities (which are regarded as “products” in the context of 4Ps in social marketing). First, the current study clearly indicated that individuals’ cognition of PBC had significantly positive effects on Vietnamese intention to stop littering. Therefore, improving Vietnamese PBC would be an effective way to reduce littering behavior and then environmental pollution. It can be achieved by displaying verbal prompts, such as “It is easy to stop littering” or “You can control not littering” over the walls, lawns, trash bins, product packages, or digital platforms, which might improve the effectiveness of existing strategy such as providing more trash bins. Displaying these prompts should be performed consistently across urban and rural areas in Vietnam to increase effectiveness, and these campaigns should be promoted through mass media and social media to enhance Vietnamese awareness. Moreover, the government could mobilize citizens to co-design and vote for the displayed prompts and to disseminate the campaign with digital channels (Abbate et al., 2022), through which people’s PBC and further the intervention’s effectiveness would be enhanced (Jong et al., 2019).

Second, the current study clearly indicated that individuals’ intention to stop littering increases with their emotional connectedness to nature. Clark et al. (2019) found that nature-based experiences contribute to connectedness to nature. In Vietnam, the existing initiatives organized by the NGOs, such as Clean-up and Recycling and Working with Fisherman could provide this kind of experience, but the participants are usually volunteers who might already have high connectedness to nature and not litter in public places (Nguyen et al., 2015). Therefore, to successfully promote a littering reduction in the whole of Vietnam, activities should be re-designed to attract and suit everyone’s participation. For example, some programs which can help to build up the connectedness to nature such as sea kayaking, tour aquarium, mountain climbing, spring, and autumn nature tour, and other outdoor activities should be promoted by every school, company, and community to every child, student, worker, and citizen. Through such programs, more connections with nature can be built. During the programs, the detrimental effects caused by littering can be highlighted through digital storytelling to increase awareness to protect the environment (Andriopoulou et al., 2022). The cost for these programs should be low or reasonable to ensure that low-income people would not be disadvantaged by their limited income. These programs should be promoted through all the media channels of schools, companies, and communities to reach wide participants.

Third, the results suggest that a focus on descriptive norms may improve the effectiveness of relevant campaigns or interventions targeting individual cognition of PBC and emotional connectedness to nature. As indicated by the results of this study, PBC has a more significant influence on the intention to stop littering when the Vietnamese hold high injunctive norms about littering. Therefore, in addition to trying to improve Vietnamese PBC, improving their perception of society’s disapproval of littering behavior would also be important. For this, social marketing practitioners could display a picture of watching eyes on the wall in public places to make people aware of injunctive norms regarding littering (Bateson et al., 2013). Littering penalties should be applied as well to show injunctive norms. Although Vietnam has applied some fine penalty policies, a heavier fine could prevent littering behavior. Community service litter clean up hours can be employed as a means of littering penalties. The government could pass a legal document to require the manufacturers to display the picture/text of anti-littering and putting litter into trash bins on product packages and in a conspicuous and prominent way. For this, manufacturing/corporate managers could invite their consumers to communicate and co-design the picture/text on a digital platform (e.g., brand community). This co-creation process would also help the manufacturers/corporates improve their reputation and enhance customer loyalty.

Limitations and future research

The main limitation of the current study was the use of a self-report method to measure littering in public places. Reliance on this method for the wrongdoing of littering results in potential under-reporting due to socially desirable bias. In future research, objective measurements of actual littering behavior, such as observations or using global positioning system (GPS) technology and digital technologies (e.g., big data, artificial intelligence) could be adopted. Second, our study did not include any infrastructural factors, such as the level of pollution of places and the availability of trash bins. Broader theoretical perspectives are recommended for future research to comprehensively explain littering behavior. Third, this study only collected data from one city in Vietnam, which may limit the generalization of the results. Future research should use a Vietnam-wide sample to ensure the representativeness of the population to overcome this limitation. Finally, the survey was based only on students and most respondents were female, both of which were found related to the littering rate in some developed countries (e.g., the U.S.) (Krauss et al., 1978). Future research should use a more representative, larger scale sample with diverse demographic characteristics (education level, age, gender) to re-examine the psychological and social factors influencing Vietnamese littering intention so as to draw a more rigorous conclusion, and on the other hand, to explore the effects of demographic characteristics on Vietnamese littering rate and/or intention. Furthermore, future studies could adopt a longitudinal perspective of analysis. The interventions and campaigns, especially that on a digital platform or with digital channels, take time to be implemented and communication and knowledge learning happen during this time. Thus, longitudinal studies are needed to examine people’s knowledge, attitude, and practice (KAP) toward littering and other sustainable green behaviors/practices (e.g., waste separation and recycling) over time. Future research should also be able to introduce the temporal effects on expectant outcomes, given the costs associated with recycling behaviors are immediate whereas the returns are in the long term.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was funded by the Fundamental Research Funds for Central Universities (20103227557).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Derek Ong for his assistance with the data analysis. We would also like to thank all our participants for their contribution to this research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abbate, S., Centobelli, P., Cerchione, R., Oropallo, E., and Riccio, E. (2022). Investigating healthcare 4.0 transition through a knowledge management perspective. IEEE Trans. Eng. Manage. 1–14. doi: 10.1109/TEM.2022.3200889 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almosa, Y., Parkinson, J., and Rundle-Thiele, S. (2017a). Littering reduction: a systematic review of research 1995-2015. Soc. Mark. Q. 23, 203–222. doi: 10.1177/1524500417697654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almosa, Y., Parkinson, J., and Rundle-Thiele, S. (2017b). A socioecological examination of observing littering behavior. J. Nonprofit Public Sect. Mark. 29, 235–253. doi: 10.1080/10495142.2017.1326354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews, N. (2018). How cognitive frames about nature may affect felt sense of nature connectedness. Ecopsychology 10, 61–71. doi: 10.1089/eco.2017.0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andriopoulou, A., Giakoumi, S., Kouvarda, T., Tsabaris, C., Pavlatou, E., and Scoullos, M. (2022). Digital storytelling as an educational tool for scientific, environmental and sustainable development literacy on marine litter in informal education environments (Case study: Hellenic center for Marine research). Mediterr. Mar. Sci. 23, 327–337. doi: 10.12681/mms.26942

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., and Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation model. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 16, 74–94. doi: 10.1007/BF02723327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Callow, L., Holmes, J. R., Roche, M. L. R., and Nettle, D. (2013). Do images of ‘watching eyes’ induce behavior that is more pro-social or more normative? A field experiment on littering. PLoS One 8:e82055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Robinson, R., Abayomi-Cole, T., Greenlees, J., O’Connor, A., and Nettle, D. (2015). Watching eyes on potential litter can reduce littering: evidence from two field experiments. PeerJ 3:e1443. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. J., Ham, S. H., and Hughes, M. (2010). Picking up litter: an application of theory-based communication to influence tourist behavior in protected areas. J. Sustain. Tour. 18, 879–900. doi: 10.1080/09669581003721281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L., and Zelenski, J. M. (2014). The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, H. C., and Mazzon, J. A. (2019). Embracing complex social problems. J. Soc. Mark. 10, 54–80. doi: 10.1108/JSOCM-03-2019-0049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhary, A. H., Polonsky, M. J., and McClaren, N. (2021). Littering behavior: a systematic review. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 45, 478–510. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, W. W., Marcolin, B. L., and Newsted, P. R. (2003). A partial least squares latent variable modeling approach for measuring interaction effects: results from a monte carlo simulation study and an electronic-mail emotion/adoption study. Inf. Syst. Res. 14, 189–217. doi: 10.1287/isre.14.2.189.16018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., and Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, E., Mulgrew, K., Kannis-Dymand, L., Schaffer, V., and Hoberg, R. (2019). Theory of planned behavior: predicting tourists’ pro-environmental intentions after a humpback whale encounter. J. Sustain. Tour. 27, 649–667. doi: 10.1080/09669582.2019.1603237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Amico, M., Vita, G. D., and Monaco, L. (2016). Exploring environmental consciousness and consumer preferences for organic wines without sulfites. J. Clean. Prod. 120, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farage, L., Uhl-Haedicke, I., and Hansen, N. (2021). Problem awareness does not predict littering: a field study on littering in the Gambia. J. Environ. Psychol. 77:101686. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fielding, K. S., Kasteren, Y. V., Louis, W., McKenna, B., Russell, S., and Spinks, A. (2016). Using individual householder survey responses to predict household environmental outcomes: the cases of recycling and water conservation. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 106, 90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2015.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fielding, K. S., McDonald, R., and Louis, W. R. (2008). Theory of planned behavior, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 318–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleury, J., and Lee, S. M. (2006). The social ecological model and physical activity in African American women. Am. J. Community Psychol. 37, 129–140. doi: 10.1007/s10464-005-9002-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Bookstein, F. L. (1982). Two structural equation models: LISREL and PLS applied to consumer exit-voice theory. J. Mark. Res. 19, 440–452. doi: 10.1177/002224378201900406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: algebra and statistics. J. Mark. Res. 18, 382–388. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosling, E., and Williams, K. J. (2010). Connectedness to nature, place attachment and conservation behavior: testing connectedness theory among farmers. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 298–304. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J. (2007). “A new and simple approach to multi-group analysis in partial least squares path modeling,” in Proceedings of PLS’07 – the 5th International Symposium on PLS and Related Methods, eds H. Martens and T. Naes (Norwegen: Aksjeselskap), 104–107.

Google Scholar

Hoang, T. C., Black, M. C., Knuteson, S. L., and Roberts, A. P. (2019). Environmental pollution, management, and sustainable development: strategies for Vietnam and other developing countries. Environ. Manage. 63, 433–436. doi: 10.1007/s00267-019-01144-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, H., Zhang, J., Chu, G., Yang, J., and Yu, P. (2018). Factors influencing tourists’ litter management behavior in mountainous tourism areas in China. Waste Manag. 79, 273–286. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2018.07.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, J., Viscusi, W. K., and Bell, J. (2018). Dynamic relationships between social norms and pro-environmental behavior: evidence from household recycling. Behav. Public Policy. 4, 1–25. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2017.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulland, J. (1999). Use of partial least squares (PLS) in strategic management research: a review of four recent studies. Strateg. Manag. J. 20, 195–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0266(199902)20:2<195::AID-SMJ13>3.0.CO;2-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibrahim, H., Mariapan, M., Lin, E. L. A., and Bidin, S. (2021). Environmental concern, attitude and intention in understanding student’s anti-littering behavior using structural equation modeling. Sustainability 8:4301. doi: 10.3390/su13084301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jong, M. D., Neulen, S., and Jansma, S. R. (2019). Citizens’ intention to participate in governmental co-creation initiatives: comparing three co-creation configurations. Gov. Inform. Q. 36, 490–500. doi: 10.1016/j.giq.2019.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kals, E., and Maes, J. (2002). “Sustainable development and emotions,” in Psychology of Sustainable Development, eds P. Schmuck and W. P. Schultz (New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media), 97–122. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keil, M., Tan, B. C., Wei, K. K., Saarinen, T., Tuunainen, V., and Wassenaar, A. (2000). A cross-cultural study on escalation of commitment behavior in software projects. MIS Q. 24, 299–325. doi: 10.2307/3250940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, C., and Breinlinger, S. (1995). Attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a study of women’s participation in collective action. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1430–1445. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02625.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krauss, R. M., Freedman, J. L., and Whitcup, M. (1978). Field and laboratory studies of littering. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 14, 109–122. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(78)90064-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafuente, R., and Sanchez, M. J. (2010). Defining and measuring environmental consciousness. Rev. Int. Sociol. 68, 731–755. doi: 10.3989/ris.2008.11.03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leaniz, P. M. G., Crespo, A. H., and López, R. G. (2018). Customer responses to environmentally certified hotels: the moderating effect of environmental consciousness on the formation of behavioral intentions. J. Sustain. Tour. 26, 1160–1177. doi: 10.1080/09669582.2017.1349775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeuw, A. D., Valois, P., Ajzen, I., and Schmidt, P. (2015). Using the theory of planned behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-school students: implications for educational interventions. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 128–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, C. C., and Shiau, W. L. (2018). Moderating effect of privacy concerns and subjective norms between satisfaction and repurchase of airline e-ticket through airline-ticket vendors. Asia Pac. J. Tour. Res. 23, 1142–1159. doi: 10.1080/10941665.2018.1528290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y. C., and Chang, C. A. (2012). Double standard: the role of environmental consciousness in green product usage. J. Mark. 76, 125–134. doi: 10.1509/jm.11.0264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohmöller, J. B. (1989). “Predictive vs. structural modeling: pls vs. ml,” in Latent Variable Path Modeling with Partial Least Squares, ed. J. B. Lohmöller (Heidelberg: Physica-Verlag), 199–226. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-52512-4_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lokhorst, A. M., Hoon, C., Rutte, R. L., and Snoo, G. D. (2014). There is an I in nature: the crucial role of the self in nature conservation. Land Use Pol. 39, 121–126. doi: 10.1016/j.landusepol.2014.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, F. S., and Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment (2019). Vietnam National Report 2019 – Solid Waste. Available online at: http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/724701468308959503/pdf/331510rev0PAPER0VN0Env0Monitor02004.pdf (accessed March 14, 2022).

Google Scholar

Nguyen, T. T. P., Zhu, D., and Le, N. P. (2015). Factors influencing waste separation intention of residential households in a developing country: evidence from Hanoi, Vietnam. Habitat Int. 48, 169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.habitatint.2015.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Ojedokun, O. (2011). Attitude towards littering as a mediator of the relationship between personality attributes and responsible environmental behavior. Waste Manag. 31, 2601–2611. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2011.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojedokun, O., and Balogun, S. K. (2013). Self-monitoring and responsible environmental behavior: the mediating role of attitudes towards littering. Front. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2:31–38.

Google Scholar

Ong, I. B. L., and Sovacool, B. K. (2012). A comparative study of littering and waste in Singapore and Japan. Resour. Conserv. Recycl 61, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2011.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, B., and Kubacki, K. (2015). The four Es of social marketing: ethicality, expensiveness, exaggeration and effectiveness. J. Soc. Mark. 5, 83–99. doi: 10.1108/JSOCM-01-2014-0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, B., Rundle-Thiele, S., and Kubacki, K. (2018). Can the theory of planned behavior explain walking to and from school among Australian children? A social marketing formative research study. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark. 23:e1599. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.1599

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasool, S., Cerchione, R., Centobelli, P., and Orapallo, E. (2022). Smoking kills you, littering butts damages others too: analyzing sustainable consumer behavior in the era of circular economy. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 24, 7115–7137. doi: 10.1007/s10668-021-01741-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimal, R. N. (2008). Modeling the relationship between descriptive norms and behaviors: a test and extension of the theory of normative social behavior (TNSB). Health Commun. 23, 103–116. doi: 10.1080/10410230801967791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rundle-Thiele, S., Russell-Bennett, R., Leo, C., and Dietrich, T. (2013). Moderating teen drinking: combining social marketing and education. Health Educ. 113, 392–406. doi: 10.1108/HE-07-2012-0041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, J. D. (2012). From millennium development goals to sustainable development goals. Lancet 379, 2206–2211. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60685-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarstedt, M., Henseler, J., and Ringle, C. M. (2011). Multi-group analysis in partial least squares (PLS) path modeling: alternative methods and empirical results. Adv. Int. Mark. 22, 195–218. doi: 10.1108/S1474-7979(2011)0000022012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, P., Anh, L. H., Wagner, J., Reichenbach, J., and Hebner, A. (2017). Solid waste management in Ho Chi Minh city, Vietnam: moving towards a circular economy? Sustainability. 9:286. doi: 10.3390/su9020286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W., Bator, R. J., Large, L. B., Bruni, C. M., and Tabanico, J. J. (2013). Littering in context: personal and environmental predictors of littering behavior. Environ. Behav. 45, 35–59. doi: 10.1177/0013916511412179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, K., and Bansal, M. (2013). Environmental consciousness, its antecedents and behavioral outcomes. J. Indian Bus. Res. 5, 198–214. doi: 10.1108/JIBR-10-2012-0080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G., and Liu, J. H. (2003). Differentiating active and passive littering: a two-stage process model of littering behavior in public spaces. Environ. Behav. 35, 415–433. doi: 10.1177/0013916503035003006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, J., and Kaur, R. (2021). Influencing the intention to adopt anti-littering behavior: an approach with modified TPB model. Soc. Mark. Q. 27, 117–132. doi: 10.1177/15245004211013333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. R., Louis, W. R., Terry, D. J., Greenaway, K. H., Clarke, M. R., and Cheng, X. (2012). Congruent or conflicted? The impact of injunctive and descriptive norms on environmental intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 32, 353–361. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stead, M., Gordon, R., Angus, K., and McDermott, L. (2007). A systematic review of social marketing effectiveness. Health Educ. 107, 126–191. doi: 10.1108/09654280710731548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stok, F. M., Ridder, D. T. D., Vet, E., and Wit, J. B. F. (2014). Don’t tell me what I should do, but what others do: the influence of descriptive and injunctive peer norms on fruit consumption in adolescents. Br. J. Health Psychol. 19, 52–64. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thanh, N. P., and Matsui, Y. (2013). Assessment of potential impacts of municipal solid waste treatment alternatives by using life cycle approach: a case study in Vietnam. Environ. Monit. Assess. 185, 7993–8004. doi: 10.1007/s10661-013-3149-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torgler, B., Garcia-Valinas, M. A., and Macintyre, A. (2012). Justifiability of littering: an empirical investigation. Environ. Values 21, 209–231. doi: 10.3197/096327112X13303670567378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuoitrenews (2015). Littering a Chronic Problem in Vietnam. Available online at: https://tuoitrenews.vn/features/25612/littering-a-chronic-problem-in-vietnam (accessed May 21, 2015).

Google Scholar

Vietnam News (2018). Heavy Fines Fail to Stop Littering. Available online at: https://vietnamnews.vn/society/462834/heavy-fines-fail-to-stop-littering.html (accessed June 18, 2018).

Google Scholar

Wever, R., Onselen, L. V., Silvester, S., and Boks, C. (2010). Influence of packaging design on littering and waste behavior. Package Technol. Sci. 23, 239–252. doi: 10.1002/pts.892

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Bank Group (2017). Environmental, Health, and Safety Guidelines for Waste Management Facilities. Available online at: https://www.ifc.org/wps/wcm/connect/5b05bf0e-1726-42b1-b7c933c7b46ddda8/Final%2B%2BWaste%2BManagement%2BFacilities.pdf?MOD=AJPERES&CVID=jqeDbH3 (accessed July 25, 2017).

Google Scholar

Yadav, R., and Pathak, G. S. (2016). Young consumers’ intention towards buying green products in a developing nation: extending the theory of planned behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 135, 732–739. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.06.120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: littering, connectedness to nature, social norms, environmental consciousness, intention to stop littering

Citation: Zhang A, Pang B, Kim J, Nguyen T-M and Nham PT (2022) An explorative study of psychological and social factors impacting littering behavior in Vietnam. Front. Psychol. 13:1025062. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1025062

Received: 25 August 2022; Accepted: 17 November 2022;
Published: 09 December 2022.

Edited by:

Roberto Cerchione, University of Naples Parthenope, Italy

Reviewed by:

Piera Centobelli, University of Naples Federico II, Italy
Eugenio Oropallo, University of Bergamo, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Pang, Kim, Nguyen and Nham. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anran Zhang, zhanganran@xidian.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.