Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 13 September 2022
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Culture in Human Thinking and Reasoning View all 10 articles

Editorial: The role of culture in human thinking and reasoning

  • 1School of Literature and Human Sciences, Osaka Metropolitan University, Osaka, Japan
  • 2Faculty of Education Health & Wellbeing, School of Psychology, University of Wolverhampton, Wolverhampton, United Kingdom
  • 3Laboratory Cognition Humaine et Artificielle, Université Paris 8, Paris, France

Editorial on the Research Topic
The role of culture in human thinking and reasoning

There have been many studies describing cultural differences in thinking and reasoning. This scientific development is mostly based upon the contrasts between Westerners' analytic cognition and Easterners' holistic cognition (e.g., Nisbett et al., 2001) and/or Westerners' linear thinking and Easterners' dialectical thinking (e.g., Peng and Nisbett, 1999). These studies have come from both social psychologists and cognitive psychologists. Although the former have tried to explain the differences in the frame of social and/or cultural systems, the latter have tried to focus on the cognitive process, which is likely to be influenced by cultural practice. Current studies on the relationship between human thinking and culture from both sides do not necessarily conduct cross-cultural comparisons, but focus on how a culture shapes people's thinking style and how people's thinking and reasoning can be adaptive in each culture.

There have been many explanations for cultural differences in cognition. For example, Miyamoto (2013) identified three levels of cultural differences: distal-level situational factors, proximal-level situational factors, and the psychological level. Cultural differences in thinking and reasoning are said to be at the psychological level. According to her, socio-ecological factors and cultural traditional factors at the distal-level may influence people's thinking and reasoning via proximal-level factors. This idea gives us a basic frame of explanation for cultural differences.

Bentahila et al. reviews the literature on moral systems and human moral judgment which are influenced by history, religious beliefs, social ecology, and institutional regulations. Each factor can be either at the distal-level or at the proximal-level. Zhou and Li reports on the influence of the Chinese traditional thought of Zhongyong on resilience. Chun-ling reports an ecological cognitive analysis of Chinese harmonious discourse. Baratgin et al. report on how Kanak's social norms influence people's responses using Knetsch's exchange paradigm. Shao et al. tested the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis considering the difference between French and Chinese languages—they did not observe the influence of language difference and hence they rejected the hypothesis and argue for cultural universality.

Secondly, it is noteworthy that four papers based on dual-process approaches (e.g., Evans, 2010) are published in this special topic. This approach supposes two kinds of process: The intuitive process and the reflective process. Among the cognitive theories of human reasoning, Yama (2018) argued that the dual-process approach is the most promising to be applied to explanations for cultural differences in thinking and reasoning. Dual-process theories make it possible to discuss the influence of explicit/implicit distinctions in cultural practices pertaining to two kinds of rationality: evolutionary adaptation of the intuitive process and normative rationality of the reflective process. Cultural effects have been regarded as implicit (intuitive) hence it is assumed that people's thinking is influenced by cultural products implicitly.

The paper of Suzuki et al. reports the power of implicit process. In spite of people's unconsciousness of cultural context, it still, in effect, influences people's thinking. This proposal is added to argue that intuitive processes can be rational in a sense. Hashimoto et al. test a dual-process model for cultural content: a moral dilemma. They discuss this in the frame of human adaptation. Meada et al. adopt a dual-process approach to the case of punishment and reward. The paper of Majima et al. shows cultural differences in the use of analytical thinking between Westerners and Easterners.

In what direction are studies on the relation between culture and human thinking headed? As categorized into the explanations for cultural differences and the adoption of dual-process theories in this editorial, we propose two directions. One is to pursue the explanations for contemporary cultural diversity and locate these in the frame of “big human history.” In this case, it is necessary for psychologists not only to conduct empirical studies but to access the big data used by historians. The other is to adopt the dual-process approach. This not only gives us the implicit/explicit distinction of cultural influences but introduces the view of human cultural adaptation into research in this field.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant-in-aid from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (No. 18K03010).

Acknowledgments

We deeply acknowledge the help of the reviewers for the publication of the articles.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Evans, J. B. T. (2010). Thinking Twice: Two Minds in One Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.5040/9780755621422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, Y. (2013). Culture and analytic versus holistic cognition: toward multilevel analyses of cultural influences. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 131–188. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00003-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psycho. Rev. 108, 291–310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, K., and Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am. Psychol. 54, 741–754. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yama, H. (2018). “Thinking and reasoning across cultures,” in The Routledge International Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning, eds L. J. Bell, and V. A. Thompson (London: Routledge), 624–638.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: culture, cultural difference, thinking, reasoning, dual-process theory

Citation: Yama H, Galbraith N, Baratgin J and Hashimoto H (2022) Editorial: The role of culture in human thinking and reasoning. Front. Psychol. 13:1018392. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1018392

Received: 13 August 2022; Accepted: 16 August 2022;
Published: 13 September 2022.

Edited and reviewed by: Bernhard Hommel, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Yama, Galbraith, Baratgin and Hashimoto. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hiroshi Yama, eWFtYS5oaXJvc2hpMTIwNCYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.