Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 November 2022
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic International Mother Language Day: Enhancing Home Language Development from a Young Age View all 10 articles

The unique and compensatory effects of home and classroom learning activities on Migrant and Seasonal Head Start children’s Spanish and English emergent literacy skills

  • 1School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, New College of Interdisciplinary Arts and Sciences, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Rutgers University-Camden, Camden, NJ, United States
  • 3Department of Early Childhood Education/Art Education, Brooklyn College, The City University of New York, New York, NY, United States

Children of migrant and seasonal farmworkers (MSFW) are among the most underprivileged, underserved groups in the United States. The current study examined how home and classroom language and literacy experiences uniquely and interactively contributed to MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish. Participants were 255 Spanish-English dual language learning children (Mage = 49 mon; 98.3% Latino/Hispanic) and their parents and 47 teachers, drawn from the Migrant and Seasonal Head Start (MSHS) Study. Parents reported how often the target children engaged in language and literacy activities (i.e., teaching letters, words, or numbers, book-reading, singing, and storytelling) with their family members. Teachers reported how often the target children engaged in classroom language and literacy activities (e.g., book-reading, learning letters, retelling stories, etc.). Children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish were assessed by standard tests. After controlling for demographic variables, home and classroom language and literacy activities uniquely predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in Spanish, but not in English. Additionally, home and classroom activities compensated one another in supporting children’s English and Spanish emergent literacy development. That is, language and literacy activities in one context showed a stronger effect for children who experienced less frequent activities in the other context. Together, these findings shed light on ways to support MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills and reveal the importance of integrating and connecting home and school learning experiences.

Introduction

In the United States, there are 2.5–3 million migrant and seasonal farmworkers (MSFW; National Center for Farmworker Health (NCFH), 2020), most of whom are foreign born (75%), self-identified as Hispanic/Latino (83%), and used Spanish as their primary language (77%; National Center for Farmworker Health (NCFH), 2020). They have an average education level of 8th grade, and approximately one third of them are living below the poverty line (National Center for Farmworker Health (NCFH), 2020). Young children of MSFW families are among the most underprivileged, underserved groups in the United States (Mathur and Parameswaran, 2012), facing developmental obstacles such as food insecurity, unstable and crowded housing, language and cultural barriers, and limited access to educational and healthcare services (Perreira et al., 2006; Barrueco, 2012; Tavassolie et al., 2018). To date, most studies have focused on MSFW children’s mental and physical health (Kupersmidt and Martin, 1997; Beltran, 2010; Taylor and Ruiz, 2017), with a scarcity of work on their development of early language and literacy skills. As a branch of the Head Start program (a federal program providing free early childhood education to low-income families), the Migrant and Seasonal Head Start (MSHS) program has been designed to offer high-quality, culturally appropriate child development and family support to MSFW families across 38 states in the United States (Early Childhood Learning and Knowledge Center (ECLKC), 2022), providing a valuable context for studying MSFW children’s language and literacy development and experiences.

Children’s emergent literacy skills, such as their knowledge of letters and words, phonological awareness, and print concepts, are important precursors of future reading skills and academic success (Lonigan et al., 2000). Most MSFW children are Spanish-English dual language learners (DLLs), who are exposed to Spanish at home and English at preschool during the months they are enrolled in MSHS programs (Mathur and Parameswaran, 2012). Given their limited English experiences, many MSFW children struggle with emergent literacy in English, which could later become a barrier for school achievement (Tavassolie et al., 2018). A study of MSFW children in Florida showed that, even though children made progress in their English over time, only 43% of the children reached the developmental benchmark in English at the end of preschool; and in kindergarten, 52% and 23% of MSFW children were at high and medium risk in their development of English emergent literacy skills (Tavassolie et al., 2018). Additionally, MSFW parents express concerns regarding their children’s Spanish loss (Smith and Johnson, 2019). Spanish is critical for children to develop their Latino identity, learn about their culture and heritage, and communicate with family members. Yet, MSFW children tend to use less Spanish after being exposed to English in preschool (Smith and Johnson, 2019).

Language and literacy activities in both the home and classroom contexts offer children important opportunities to develop emergent literacy skills (Hammer et al., 2014; Piasta, 2016). However, very few studies have examined MSFW children’s home and classroom experiences simultaneously. The current study asked how home and classroom language and literacy activities uniquely predicted MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish; and whether home and classroom activities interacted with each other in their contributions to children’s emergent literacy skills.

Home language and literacy activities and children’s emergent literacy skills

Ample research has demonstrated the effect of home language and literacy experiences on children’s emergent literacy skills (e.g., Reese et al., 2010; Farver et al., 2013). In particular, the frequency of language and literacy activities (e.g., book-reading, teaching letters and words, storytelling, and singing songs) is found to relate to children’s emergent literacy skills (Reese et al., 2000; Raikes et al., 2006; Rodriguez and Tamis-LeMonda, 2011).

Book-reading exposes children to language input that is diverse, complex, and cognitively demanding (Peterson and McCabe, 1994; Soderstrom and Wittebolle, 2013; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2018). Both correlational and intervention work has revealed the benefits of frequent book-reading for early language and literacy development (Bus et al., 1995; Raikes et al., 2006; Noble et al., 2019). Studies with low-income, Latino families also documented the links between book-reading frequency and children’s emergent literacy skills in both English and Spanish (Farver et al., 2013; Gonzalez et al., 2017; Shen and Del Tufo, 2022). For instance, in a sample of Latino Head Start children, parents’ engagement with children in literacy activities in English and Spanish predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in both languages (Farver et al., 2013). Importantly, parent–child book-reading in one language might benefit children’s emergent literacy skills in the other language. One study showed that the frequency of mother–child book-reading, which was primarily conducted in Spanish only or both English and Spanish, predicted Latino children’s receptive vocabulary in English during preschool years (Gonzalez et al., 2017).

Other than book-reading, parental engagement in code-related activities such as teaching children how to read and write letters and words also support the development of emergent literacy skills (Sénéchal and Lefevre, 2002, 2014; Haney and Hill, 2004; Hood et al., 2008; Inoue et al., 2018). For example, a three-year longitudinal study has suggested that the frequency of parental teaching of literacy skills predicted children’s concurrent emergent literacy skills at preschool and their reading and spelling skills in 1st and 2nd grades (Hood et al., 2008). Similar findings have been observed in monolingual Spanish-speaking samples. For example, a study with low-socioeconomic status Chilean families found that mothers who frequently taught children how to read and write letters had children who showed better letter-word identification skills (Mendive et al., 2020).

Additionally, learning activities that do not rely on print materials, such as storytelling and singing, are highly valued in Latino and/or MSFW families as essential ways to support children’s language and literacy development and convey cultural lessons (Luo and Tamis-LeMonda, 2019; van der Pluijim et al., 2019). It is therefore critical to include these non-print activities in the examination of home experiences. Indeed, studies using combined measures of children’s engagement in book-reading, storytelling, and singing have found positive associations between the frequency of these activities and children’s early language and/or emergent literacy skills in infancy and preschool years (Rodriguez et al., 2009; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2019; Song et al., 2022).

Fewer studies have examined children’s language and literacy experiences in MSFW families. Constraints such as long working hours, high illiteracy rates, limited access to learning materials, and unfamiliarity with the U.S. education system present challenges for MSFW parents to support their children’s early literacy development (Mehta et al., 2000; Perreira et al., 2006; Tavassolie et al., 2018). Nonetheless, MSFW parents value education and have high dedication to and expectation for their children (O’Brien et al., 2011; Barrueco, 2012; Smith and Johnson, 2019). Qualitative research has shown that MSFW parents engaged their children in a variety of reading and writing activities, including but not limited to reading books, messages, and letters from their families, reading and writing alphabetic letters and children’s names, reading the Bible, and telling stories (Lynch, 2008; Purcell-Gates, 2013). In a study of 48 MSHS children, researchers examined children’s emergent literacy skills and their home literacy experiences (e.g., book-reading frequency, access to books in English and Spanish), and found that the composite score of home literacy experiences predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in their dominant language (Ezell et al., 2000). Intervention studies aiming to promote language and literacy activities in MSFW or migrant families have also shown positive effects on children’s early language and literacy skills (Boyce et al., 2010; St. Clair et al., 2012).

Classroom language and literacy activities and children’s emergent literacy skills

The quantity/frequency of classroom language and literacy activities has been found to support children’s emergent literacy skills (Xue and Meisels, 2004; Connor et al., 2006; Guarino et al., 2006; Zucker et al., 2013). For example, in a study with a culturally and linguistically diverse sample, pre-k children who spent more time in teacher-directed activities such as book-reading showed greater gains in their emergent literacy skills over the school year (Pianta et al., 2020). Longitudinal studies have also found that the duration or frequency of language and literacy activities in preschool or kindergarten predicted children’s language and emergent literacy growth from pre-k to kindergarten (Christopher and Farran, 2020) and their gains in reading skills from kindergarten to 5th grade (Sonnenschein et al., 2010). These findings highlight the facilitative role of classroom language and literacy activities in children’s early literacy development.

Most children in the MSHS programs are DLLs and speak Spanish as their primary language (Stechuk and Burns, 2005). Yet, research on Spanish-English DLLs’ classroom language and literacy experiences is still limited. Like their monolingual peers, DLLs benefit from frequent, high-quality language and literacy instructions (Gersten and Geva, 2003; Graves et al., 2004; Gersten et al., 2005; Baker et al., 2006; Cirino et al., 2007). At the same time, it is crucial for teachers to provide DLLs with culturally and linguistically responsive instructions and support their home language development (Castro et al., 2011; Sawyer et al., 2016). For example, Head Start teachers’ instructional support in the DLLs’ home language (e.g., quantity of Spanish use, instructional strategies such as questioning and literacy materials in Spanish) has been found to predict DLLs’ home language skills (White et al., 2020). Other studies comparing bilingual and English-only programs have also suggested that bilingual programs support children’s Spanish development without slowing down their English acquisition (Collier and Thomas, 2004; Rolstad et al., 2005; Barnett et al., 2007; Figueras-Daniel and Li, 2021). Nonetheless, observations of preschool teachers’ classroom practices with DLLs have suggested that teachers tend to use few linguistically responsive practices (e.g., providing key words in children’s home language, giving children opportunities to use both English and the home languages) and more basic, low-quality language and literacy instructions (e.g., not using many open-ended questions or advanced vocabularies) with DLLs (Justice et al., 2008; Sawyer et al., 2016).

To date, few studies have examined teachers’ language and literacy practices in MSHS classrooms in relation to children’s developmental outcomes. One intervention study found that training teachers to use high-quality instructions (e.g., building children’s vocabulary, engaging children in book-reading, and implementing classroom activities in a playful and effective way) during classroom language and literacy activities promoted MSHS children’s emergent literacy growth in English and Spanish (Solari et al., 2016).

The unique and interactive effects of home and classroom experiences

Most research has examined the effects of children’s home and classroom language and literacy experiences separately, without asking how these two components of children’s learning experiences uniquely contribute to the development of emergent literacy skills and whether they interact with one another. The bioecological model posits that children’s immediate contexts (e.g., home and classroom settings) are not independent. Rather, various developmental contexts interact with each other in their contribution to child outcomes (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2006). Similarly, the multisystemic approach proposes that child development unfolds within an interconnected system of individual, family, and extra-familial (e.g., school, community) factors (Barrueco, 2012). It is therefore necessary to consider children’s home and school experiences simultaneously in minority, marginalized populations.

Studies examining the unique effect of home and classroom language and literacy experiences have yielded mixed findings. One study examined the effect of home and classroom literacy experiences (e.g., book-reading frequency and the availability of literacy materials in the home and classroom settings) on the emergent literacy skills of children enrolled in MSHS programs and found that home literacy experiences was a stronger predictor than classroom literacy experiences (Ezell et al., 2000). However, a study with Turkish 5-year-olds found that classroom literacy experiences (e.g., the availability of books, book-reading and early writing activities, etc.), but not home literacy experiences (e.g., number of books, book-reading frequency, etc.), predicted children’s emergent literacy skills 4 months later (Altun et al., 2018). Other studies have documented the unique roles of both home and classroom experiences. For example, a reading intervention study compared three conditions, a classroom only condition in which teachers were trained to read to children using the dialogic reading approach, a classroom plus home condition in which both teachers and parents were trained to read to children using the same approach, and a control condition (Whitehurst et al., 1994). While both treatment conditions improved children’s early language skills, the classroom plus home condition had a stronger effect than the classroom only condition. Another study found that 3-year-old children who had better home literacy experiences and whose preschool center had higher levels of average child ability (a potential indicator of center quality) showed more advanced literacy skills at 1st and 3rd grades (Melhuish et al., 2008).

To date, no studies to our knowledge have examined the interaction between home and school language and literacy activities in relation to MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills. However, studies with other populations have suggested that home and classroom experiences may shape child development in a compensatory manner, such that rich, high-quality language and literacy experiences in one context may compensate the poor experiences in the other context (Magnuson et al., 2004; McCartney et al., 2007; Crosnoe et al., 2010; Vernon-Feagans et al., 2013). For example, Vernon-Feagans et al. (2013) found children who received less complex maternal language input at home to benefit more from positive caregiver–child verbal interactions. Other studies have also shown that high-quality classroom experiences matter more for children with poorer home learning experiences due to factors such as low-income, low maternal education, and single parenthood (Magnuson et al., 2004; McCartney et al., 2007; Crosnoe et al., 2010). Home literacy experiences can also compensate the lack of language and literacy support in the classroom context. For example, one study found that DLL children’s engagement in home literacy activities in their heritage language predicted their vocabulary growth in the societal language, but only for those who received low levels of classroom language stimulation (Willard et al., 2021).

In contrast to the compensatory hypothesis, some studies found little evidence that children from more disadvantaged families would benefit more from high-quality classroom experiences (Burchinal et al., 2000). There is even some evidence suggesting that children with stimulating home experiences might be better prepared for learning in the classroom, indicating a complementary relationship of home and school experiences. A study with a low-income sample found that high-quality childcare positively predicted children’s emergent literacy skills only for those children exposed to high cognitive stimulation at home (Votruba-Drzal et al., 2004). Similarly, a study of Head Start children suggested a greater effect of classroom quality on children’s problem solving and reasoning for those children with better home learning experiences (e.g., frequent language and literacy activities, abundant learning materials and toys, and warm, non-punitive parenting behaviors; Bryant et al., 1994). Perhaps, children need to reach a certain skill level before taking advantage of their classroom experiences (Vygotsky, 1978), and linguistically and cognitively stimulating home experiences play a key role in helping children achieve the threshold.

The current study

To better understand the unique and interactive effects of home and classroom literacy activities on MSHS children’s emergent literacy skills, we asked two research questions:

1. How do home and classroom language and literacy activities uniquely contribute to MSHS children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish? We hypothesized that frequencies of language and literacy activities in both the home and classroom settings would account for unique variances in MSHS children’s emergent literacy skills.

2. Do home and classroom language and literacy activities interact with each other in their contributions to children’s English and Spanish literacy skills? If home and classroom activities benefit child development in a compensatory way, we would expect classroom language and literacy activities to show a stronger positive effect for children who experienced less frequent language and literacy activities at home, and vice versa. Alternatively, if home and classroom activities work in a complementary way, we would expect the effect of classroom activities to be stronger for those children who more frequently engaged in language and literacy activities at home.

Methods

Participants

Participants were drawn from the Migrant and Seasonal Head Start (MSHS) Study (Caswell et al., 2020), which aimed to understand the characteristics of MSHS programs, children, and families, the quality of MSHS services and practices, and the relation between MSHS characteristics and the outcomes of children and families (Caswell et al., 2019). The study involved a nationally representative sample of 122 MSHS classrooms, 234 lead and assistant teachers, 873 children, and 778 parents (Caswell et al., 2019). Data was collected between January 2017 and January 2018, via MSHS staff surveys, parent interviews, classroom observations, and direct child assessments.

Of the original 873 children, 255 had valid data on emergent literacy skills in English and/or Spanish, parental report of home language and literacy activities, and teachers’ report of classroom language and literacy activities. Given that children were only assessed in English and Spanish, we further excluded 20 preschoolers exposed to a home language other than English or Spanish. Thus, the final analytic sample included 235 children and their parents (one parent of each child) and 47 lead teachers. Table 1 presents the demographic information of the sample. On average, children (51.49% males) were 49 months of age (SD = 9.17) at the time of assessment. Almost all of them (98.3%) were identified by their parents as Latino/Hispanic. Most participating parents were mothers (88.9%) and had elementary to high school education levels (62.4%). About 82% of the parents reported using all Spanish or more Spanish than English with their children. The teachers’ education levels ranged from high school to graduate education (see Table 1 for more details).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for demographic variables, home and classroom language and literacy activities, and children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish.

Measures

Home language and literacy activities

Parents were interviewed about how many days in the past week they themselves or someone in the family engaged in each of the four types of activities in any language with the target child: teaching the child letters, words, or numbers, reading or looking at books, singing songs, and telling stories (1– zero days, 2 – 1 to 2 days a week, 3 – 3 to 4 days a week, and 4 – 5 to 7 days a week). An average score across these 4 activities was calculated, with higher scores indicating more frequent language and literacy activities at home (M = 2.87, SD = 0.71, Range = 1–4, Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.69).

Classroom language and literacy activities

Teachers were asked about how often children in their class engaged in each of the eight types of language and literacy activities in any language in a survey: learning the names of letters, writing children’s own names, learning about the conventions of print, retelling stories, listening to stories read by teachers, working on phonics, discussing new words, and practicing writing alphabets (1 – never, 2 – about once a month or less, 3 – two to three times a month, 4 – once or twice a week, 5 – three to four times a week, 6 – every day). An average score was calculated to indicate how frequently children engaged in these eight activities (M = 5.46, SD = 0.57, Range = 2.5–6, Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.80).

Emergent literacy skills

Children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish were assessed using the Letter-Word Identification (English) and Identificación de letras y palabras (Spanish) scales on the Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey–Revised Normative Update (WMLS-R NU; Woodcock et al., 2005). These assessments examine children’s knowledge of the alphabet and their ability to read single words (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.87 for English and 0.85 for Spanish; Caswell et al., 2019). All children were assessed in both English and Spanish, starting with the child’s dominant language as reported by their parents. The English and Spanish standard scores were used for analyses. It is important to note that the norms were based on English-speaking and Spanish-speaking monolingual samples. Although the standard scores captured individual differences among children, they likely underestimated DLLs’ skill levels and must be interpreted with caution.

Covariates

A group of demographic variables were controlled for in the analyses. At the child/family level, we controlled for children’s age, gender, the total number of children living in the household, parental education, and parents’ relative language use with the child (1-All English, 3-Same amount of Spanish and English, and 5-All Spanish). According to the MSHS dataset, parental education was coded based on a scale, ranging from 1 (no school) to 25 (professional degree; see more details in Table 1 Notes). At the classroom level, we included teachers’ education level (1-less than high school diploma, 5-higher than Bachelor’s degree) and instructional language. Teachers reported on the language(s) they used when teaching children, reading to children, and presenting information, on a scale from 1 (English completely) to 5 (Spanish completely). An average score was calculated to indicate teachers’ relative use of English and Spanish (M = 2.57, SD = 1.25, Range = 1–5). Across the sample, teachers used slightly more English than Spanish.

Analytic plan

As shown in Table 1, the proportions of missing values were low, ranging from 0 to 7.7%. The Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test suggested that data were missing completely at random (Chi2 = 18.63, p = 0.91). Multiple imputation was conducted in STATA 17.0 to handle missing values among the control variables (Stata Corp, 2021). The multiple imputation model included all covariates, with and without missing values, such that the missing values were predicted based on existing data. Twenty imputed datasets were generated, and findings were based on pooled estimates.

Because children and families were nested within classrooms, multilevel modeling was conducted using the mixed command of STATA 17.0 to examine the effects of child-level (i.e., home language and literacy activities) and classroom-level (i.e., classroom language and literacy activities) factors, accounting for variation between classrooms. First, unconditional models were conducted to estimate the amount of variance explained by differences among individual children (Level 1) and classrooms (Level 2). Second, to examine the contributions of home and classroom language and literacy activities to children’s emergent literacy skills (Research Question 1), a set of two-level random intercept models were estimated, using home language and literacy activities as the key predictor at Level 1, and classroom language and literacy activities as the key predictor at Level 2. Models were estimated separately for English and Spanish literacy skills. Finally, to examine whether home and classroom activities interact with each other in their contributions to emergent literacy skills (Research Question 2), a cross-level interaction term between the home and classroom language and literacy activities was added to the models described above. When the interaction term was significant, we further estimated the conditional effects of classroom language and literacy activities, when the frequency of home language and literacy activities was set to be either low (i.e., 15th percentile of the analytic sample) or high (i.e., 85th percentile). The nlcom command of STATA was used after model estimation to compute point estimates, standard errors, p values, and confidence intervals for different combinations of parameter estimates (e.g., the effect of one predictor given a specific value of another predictor; Stata Corp, 2021). Likewise, we also estimated the effects of home language and literacy activities, when the frequency of classroom activities was low or high (i.e., 15th and 85th percentiles). In each model, covariates of the home and classroom contexts were included at Level 1 (i.e., child age, gender, parental education, parental Spanish use, total number of children in the household) and Level 2 (i.e., teachers’ educational level, teachers’ instructional language) respectively.

Results

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the key predictors and outcome variables. Children showed enormous variation in their English and Spanish emergent literacy skills (range = 32–134 for English and 45–137 for Spanish), with an average standard score of 83.64 (SD = 12.84) for English emergent literacy and 94.79 (SD = 12.42) for Spanish emergent literacy, suggesting that overall children had better literacy skills in Spanish than in English (p < 0.001). Notably, the standard scores must be interpreted with caution, because the norm used was based on English-speaking and Spanish-speaking samples.

Unconditional models

Unconditional models suggested that 73.23% and 82.84% of the variances in English and Spanish emergent literacy skills could be attributed to the differences among individual children, respectively; whereas 26.77% and 17.16% of the variances in English and Spanish emergent literacy skills could be explained by the differences among classrooms, respectively. In the models below, we included a random intercept to account for between-classroom differences.

Unique contributions of home and classroom language and literacy activities

As shown in Model A (Table 2), neither home nor classroom language and literacy activities uniquely predicted MSHS children’s emergent literacy skills in English, after controlling for demographic covariates and parents’ and teachers’ relative English and Spanish use. However, both the home (b = 3.82, SE = 1.12, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [1.63, 6.00]) and classroom (b = 3.31, SE = 1.43, p = 0.021, 95% CI = [0.50, 6.12]; see Model C, Table 2) activities uniquely predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in Spanish. Children who experienced more frequent language and literacy activities at home and in the classroom showed better Spanish literacy skills.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Home and classroom language and literacy activities predicting emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish.

Interaction between home and classroom language and literacy activities

In both the English and Spanish models, the interaction term between home and classroom language and literacy activities was significant, indicating that the home and classroom activities moderated the effect of one another on children’s emergent literacy skills (see Models B and D, Table 2).

Specifically, the effect of classroom language and literacy activities was stronger for children who experienced less frequent language and literacy activities at home. For children who engaged in home language and literacy activities almost every day (i.e., 85th percentile or frequency of home activities = 3.6), classroom language and literacy activities did not predict children’s English (b = −2.50, SE = 2.08, p = 0.228, 95% CI = [−6.58, 1.57]; upper half of Table 3) or Spanish (b = 1.09, SE = 1.76, p = 0.535, 95% CI = [−2.36, 4.55]; lower half of Table 3) literacy skills. However, for children who engaged in home language and literacy activities only 1 to 2 days a week (i.e., 15th percentile or frequency of home activities = 2), classroom activities significantly contributed to children’s Spanish literacy skills (b = 6.73, SE = 2.15, p = 0.002, 95% CI = [2.52, 10.93]). Although the conditional effect was non-significant for children’s English literacy skills, the coefficient changed from negative to positive (b = 3.81, SE = 2.43, p = 0.117, 95% CI = [−0.95, 8.56]; see Table 3) as the frequency of home literacy activities decreased, revealing a similar trend.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Conditional effects of home and classroom language and literacy activities on children’s emergent literacy skills in English and Spanish.

Similarly, home language and literacy activities had a greater effect on children’s emergent literacy skills, when classroom language and literacy activities were less frequent. For children who engaged in all 8 types of language and literacy activities in the classroom everyday (i.e., 85th percentile or frequency of classroom activities = 6), the effect of home language and literacy activities was non-significant for English (b = −0.94, SE = 1.61, p = 0.559, 95% CI = [−4.09, 2.21]) and Spanish (b = 1.71, SE = 1.49, p = 0.252, 95% CI = [−1.21, 4.62]; see Table 3) literacy skills. However, for children who engaged in different types of language and literacy activities in the classroom only 3 to 4 times a week (i.e., 15th percentile or frequency of classroom activities = 5.05), home language and literacy activities significantly predicted their English (b = 2.81, SE = 2.37, p = 0.038, 95% CI = [0.15, 5.47]) and Spanish (b = 5.05, SE = 1.25, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [2.60, 7.50]; see Table 3) literacy skills. Together, these findings suggested that language and literacy activities in the home and classroom settings compensated one another in supporting children’s emergent literacy development.

Discussion

Children of MSFW families face many challenges in early development and are largely “invisible” in educational research. The current study examined the unique and interactive effects of home and classroom language and literacy activities on MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills. Results showed that the frequencies of home and classroom language and literacy activities uniquely contributed to children’s emergent literacy skills in Spanish, but not in English. Additionally, children’s home and classroom experiences compensated one another. Language and literacy activities in one context were more beneficial for those children who experienced less frequent language and literacy activities in the other context.

Home language and literacy activities and children’s emergent literacy skills

Consistent with previous work (Farver et al., 2013), the frequency of home language and literacy activities uniquely predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in Spanish, while controlling for demographic variables and children’s classroom experiences. This finding shows the important role of MSFW parents in their children’s emergent literacy development. Even though MSFW parents in our sample had relatively low levels of education (65% of the parents did not have a high school diploma), their engagement in language and literacy activities with the children showed a positive effect on children’s emergent literacy skills in the home language. Indeed, almost 40% of the MSFW parents in our sample reported engaging their children in language and literacy activities 3 to 4 days a week or more frequently, indicating their high motivation and investment in promoting early language and literacy development (Purcell-Gates, 2013).

The frequency of home language and literacy activities did not uniquely predict children’s emergent literacy skills in English. Most MSFW parents reported only or primarily using Spanish with their children. However, this finding does not necessarily mean that the effect of home language and literacy activities is language specific. On one hand, it might take children time to transfer their emergent literacy skills from one language to another, thus showing a delayed cross-language transfer effect. For example, a study found that home literacy activities at kindergarten, which primarily occurred in Spanish, predicted Latino children’s concurrent emergent literacy skills in Spanish, which further predicted their reading skills in both Spanish and English in the 7th grade (Reese et al., 2000). On the other hand, the effect of home language and literacy activities on children’s English emergent literacy skills seems to vary by children’s experiences in the classroom, which will be discussed later.

Classroom language and literacy activities and children’s emergent literacy skills

Overall, MSHS teachers reported frequently engaging children in language and literacy activities in the classroom. Classroom language and literacy activities uniquely predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in Spanish, highlighting the supportive role of MSHS classroom in children’s home language development. Most MSHS teachers used some Spanish during teaching and learning activities to accommodate the linguistic and cultural needs of MSFW children. Indeed, teachers’ use of the home language of DLLs has been found to benefit children’s home language growth (Collier and Thomas, 2004; Rolstad et al., 2005; Barnett et al., 2007; Figueras-Daniel and Li, 2021).

However, classroom activities did not uniquely predict children’s English emergent literacy skills. This could not be simply explained by teachers’ instructional language(s), as most teachers reported using both English and Spanish during classroom activities. Supplementary analyses examining the interaction between classroom language and literacy activities and teachers’ instructional language(s) suggested that the effect of classroom activities did not vary by the language(s) teachers used (p’s > 0.05). One possible explanation is that MSHS teachers might be primarily focusing on supporting children’s English language skills rather than their English literacy skills. Many MSHS children came to the program with very limited English proficiency (Stechuk and Burns, 2005) and might need to acquire adequate English language skills before they could develop emergent literacy skills in English. Additionally, children might be more engaged and interested in Spanish language and literacy activities, which are more relevant to their cultural and linguistic experiences at home and in the MSFW community (Purcell-Gates, 2013). Children’s high level of engagement and interests in learning might further enhance their Spanish learning outcomes (Baroody and Diamond, 2016).

The compensatory roles of home and classroom language and literacy activities

The effects of home and classroom language and literacy activities compensated one another. Specifically, frequent classroom language and literacy activities predicted children’s Spanish emergent skills for those children who experienced infrequent language and literacy activities at home, but not for those who engaged in home activities 5–7 days a week. These findings were consistent with previous evidence that high-quality early education buffers against the negative effects of impoverished home experiences on children’s language and cognitive outcomes (Magnuson et al., 2004; McCartney et al., 2007; Crosnoe et al., 2010; Vernon-Feagans et al., 2013, 2019). Theoretically, the unique and compensatory roles of home and classroom language and literacy activities support the notion of Mesosystem in the Bioecological Model (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2006), which maintains that children’s immediate developmental contexts (e.g., family, school) can interact with one another and jointly impact developmental outcomes.

Likewise, home language and literacy activities benefited children’s emergent literacy skills more when children had less frequent language and literacy activities in the classroom, highlighting the protective role of home language and literacy activities. When children experienced relatively low frequency of classroom activities, home language and literacy activities predicted children’s emergent literacy skills in both Spanish and English. These findings suggested that language and literacy activities in the family context are crucial for MSFW children to develop their home language and may compensate the lack of language and literacy support in the classroom. More interestingly, even though most MSFW parents reported that they predominantly used Spanish with their children, the impact of home language and literacy activities could go beyond the home language to children’s learning of English. Previous work with DLLs also showed that the frequency of family literacy activities in the heritage language predicted children’s vocabulary growth in the societal language when children experienced low to average quality of language stimulation in the classroom (Willard et al., 2021).

Practical implications

Findings of the study have practical implications for parents and teachers. First, enhancing language and literacy practices in MSFW households may be a valuable strategy to promote children’s development of emergent literacy skills. MSFW parents often view school as the primary context for children to acquire English skills and consider themselves incapable of supporting children’s English learning when cultural and language barriers prevent them from participating in school-related activities (Smith and Johnson, 2019). However, our study showed that home language and literacy activities were associated with children’s emergent literacy skills in both English and Spanish, when the frequency of classroom activities was relatively low. Early intervention and prevention programs should encourage MSFW parents to frequently engage in language and literacy practices that are valuable and appropriate in their own cultural and linguistic contexts (Boyce et al., 2010; St. Clair et al., 2012), as well as empower them to recognize their critical role in supporting children’s dual language development.

Additionally, our findings highlight the compensatory role of language and literacy rich MSHS classrooms for children with limited language and literacy resources at home. It is important to promote teachers’ engagement in classroom language and literacy activities via professional training (Solari et al., 2016). Notably, even though classroom is a primary context for English exposure, classroom language and literacy activities did not show a significant effect on children’s English emergent literacy skills. More attention and resources are needed to help MSHS teachers develop effective teaching strategies to support DLLs’ English acquisition (Zepeda et al., 2011). The multisystemic approach recognizes the importance of bridging the gap between the language and literacy practices in the home and school contexts (Barrueco, 2012). Teachers should understand and build upon the literacy knowledge MSFW children gain from their home and community and integrate culturally and linguistically relevant practices into classroom activities (Purcell-Gates, 2013). For instance, in an ethnographic study, compared to book-reading activities, MSFW children were more interested and engaged in culturally responsive activities such as making birthday cards, which they frequently experienced in the migrant farmworker camps (Purcell-Gates, 2013). Interventions should also consider facilitating children’s home and classroom experiences simultaneously (Whitehurst et al., 1994; Grøver et al., 2020). For example, a study in Norway found that providing DLLs with the same set of books to read both in the classroom and at home improved children’s early language and literacy skills in the societal language (Grøver et al., 2020).

Limitations and future directions

The study has several limitations. First, we only focused on the frequency of language and literacy activities reported by parents and teachers. The adult self-report approach might result in an overestimation of children’s engagement. Observational studies are needed to replicate our findings. Additionally, the study did not examine other types of activities frequently experienced by MSFW, such as reading letters from their extended families, writing a grocery shopping list (Purcell-Gates, 2013), as well as children’s media exposure such as television watching and access to computers and electronic devices (e.g., smartphones). It is also possible that different learning activities contribute to early literacy development in unique ways, an area worth exploring in future research. Furthermore, other important aspects of children’s experiences, such as the quality of language and literacy activities (Justice et al., 2018), the levels of children’s engagement and interest during these activities (Baroody and Diamond, 2016), and the language(s) used during these activities, also play an important role and are worth examining in combination with the frequency of activities. Therefore, more observational work is needed in future research.

In addition to their family members and teachers, MSFW and/or Latino children constantly interact with and gain language and literacy skills from their extended families, peers, and members from the community (Gonzalez and Uhing, 2008; Purcell-Gates, 2013). Future studies should consider these culturally relevant experiences.

Finally, the current study was based on cross-sectional data, which requires us to consider various potential causal scenarios. Children not only learn from language and literacy activities but can also actively elicit language and literacy learning opportunities from their home and classroom environments. Children’s language and literacy experiences might also change over time, as they gain more exposure to the U.S. school system, calling for a longitudinal approach. To date, there is a dearth of longitudinal studies with MSFW children, partially due to the high mobility of the MSFW families (Mathur and Parameswaran, 2012).

Conclusion

Language and literacy activities in both the home and classroom contexts play an imperative role in MSFW children’s emergent literacy skills. More importantly, frequent language and literacy activities in one context would be more beneficial when children engaged in these activities less frequently in the other context, showing a compensatory relation of the two contexts. Together, these findings can help parents, teachers, and education policy makers find ways to enhance MSFW children’s emergent literacy development, better prepare them for school learning, and ultimately increase equity and equality in early childhood and long-term development.

Data availability statement

The datasets (Migrant and Seasonal Head Start Study, United States, 2017–2018) for this study can be found here: https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/studies/37348. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to the ICPSR online request system.

Ethics statement

This is a secondary data analysis of an existing dataset. In the original study, written informed consent was obtained from the participating parents, teachers, and children’s legal guardians. The current study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Rutgers University.

Author contributions

RL contributed to conceptual framework, data analysis, and manuscript writing. LS contributed to conceptual framework and manuscript editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Altun, D., Tantekin Erden, F., and Snow, C. E. (2018). A multilevel analysis of home and classroom literacy environments in relation to preschoolers’ early literacy development. Psychol. Sch. 55, 1098–1120. doi: 10.1002/pits.22153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. K., Gersten, R., Haager, D., and Dingle, M. (2006). Teaching practice and the reading growth of first-grade English learners: validation of an observation instrument. Elem. Sch. J. 107, 199–220. doi: 10.1086/510655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett, W. S., Yarosz, D. J., Thomas, J., Jung, K. H., and Blanco, D. (2007). Two-way and monolingual English immersion in preschool education: an experimental comparison. Early Child. Res. Q. 22, 277–293. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baroody, A. E., and Diamond, K. E. (2016). Associations among preschool children’s classroom literacy environment, interest and engagement in literacy activities, and early reading skills. J. Early Child. Res. 14, 146–162. doi: 10.1177/1476718X14529280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrueco, S. (2012). Linguistic, literacy, and socioemotional interventions for young children in migrant and seasonal farm worker families. In B. Wasik and B. HornVan (Eds.), Handbook of Family Literacy. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Beltran, M. A. (2010). Exploring program quality factors that impact child services in migrant and seasonal head start programs. Diss. Abstr. Int. 71:2742.

Google Scholar

Boyce, L. K., Innocenti, M. S., Roggman, L. A., Norman, V. K. J., and Ortiz, E. (2010). Telling stories and making books: evidence for an intervention to help parents in migrant head start families support their children's language and literacy. Early Educ. Dev. 21, 343–371. doi: 10.1080/10409281003631142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U., and Morris, P. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon and R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & R. M. Lerner (Eds) Handbook of Child Psychology : 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed. New York: John Wiley.

Google Scholar

Bryant, D. M., Burchinal, M., Lau, L. B., and Sparling, J. J. (1994). Family and classroom correlates of head start children's developmental outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 9, 289–309. doi: 10.1016/0885-2006(94)90011-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchinal, M. R., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Bryant, D. M., and Clifford, R. (2000). Children's social and cognitive development and child-care quality: testing for differential associations related to poverty, gender, or ethnicity. Appl. Dev. Sci. 4, 149–165. doi: 10.1207/S1532480XADS0403_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bus, A. G., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Rev. Educ. Res. 65, 1–21. doi: 10.3102/00346543065001001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro, D. C., Páez, M. M., Dickinson, D. K., and Frede, E. (2011). Promoting language and literacy in young dual language learners: research, practice, and policy. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 15–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00142.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caswell, L., Bumgarner, E., Barrueco, S., López, M., Wolf, A., and Layzer, C. (2019). Migrant and Seasonal Head Start Study 2017: Data Tables, OPRE Report #2019–66. Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Google Scholar

Caswell, L., Bumgarner, E., Barrueco, S., López, M., Wolf, A., Layzer, C., et al. (2020). Migrant and Seasonal Head Start Study, United States, 2017-2018. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor].

Google Scholar

Christopher, C., and Farran, D. (2020). Academic gains in kindergarten related to eight classroom practices. Early Child. Res. Q. 53, 638–649. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirino, P. T., Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Foorman, B. R., Carlson, C. D., and Francis, D. J. (2007). Teacher characteristics, classroom instruction, and student literacy and language outcomes in bilingual kindergarteners. Elem. Sch. J. 107, 341–364. doi: 10.1086/516668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, V. P., and Thomas, W. P. (2004). The astounding effectiveness of dual language education for all. NABE J. Res. Practice 2, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., and Slominski, L. (2006). Preschool instruction and children's emergent literacy growth. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 665–689. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crosnoe, R., Leventhal, T., Wirth, R. J., Pierce, K. M., and Pianta, R. (2010). NICHD early child care research network (NICHD ECCRN): family socioeconomic status and consistent environmental stimulation in early childhood. Child Dev. 81, 972–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01446.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Early Childhood Learning and Knowledge Center (ECLKC). (2022). Migrant and seasonal head start collaboration office. Available at: https://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/state-collaboration/article/migrant-seasonal-head-start-collaboration-office (Accessed August 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ezell, H. K., Gonzales, M. D., and Randolph, E. (2000). Emergent literacy skills of migrant Mexican American preschoolers. Commun. Disord. Q. 21, 147–153. doi: 10.1177/152574010002100303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farver, J. A. M., Xu, Y., Lonigan, C. J., and Eppe, S. (2013). The home literacy environment and Latino head start children's emergent literacy skills. Dev. Psychol. 49, 775–791. doi: 10.1037/a0028766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueras-Daniel, A., and Li, Z. (2021). Evidence of support for dual language learners in a study of bilingual staffing patterns using the classroom assessment of supports for emergent bilingual acquisition (CASEBA). Early Child. Res. Q. 54, 271–285. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gersten, R., Baker, S. K., Haager, D., and Graves, A. W. (2005). Exploring the role of teacher quality in predicting reading outcomes for first-grade English learners: an observational study. Remedial Spec. Educ. 26, 197–206. doi: 10.1177/07419325050260040201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gersten, R., and Geva, E. (2003). Teaching reading to early language learners. Educ. Leadersh. 60, 44–49.

Google Scholar

Gonzalez, J. E., Acosta, S., Davis, H., Pollard-Durodola, S., Saenz, L., Soares, D., et al. (2017). Latino maternal literacy beliefs and practices mediating socioeconomic status and maternal education effects in predicting child receptive vocabulary. Early Educ. Dev. 28, 78–95. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1185885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, J. E., and Uhing, B. M. (2008). Home literacy environments and young Hispanic children's English and Spanish oral language: a communality analysis. J. Early Interv. 30, 116–139. doi: 10.1177/1053815107313858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graves, A. W., Gersten, R., and Haager, D. (2004). Literacy instruction in multiple-language first-grade classrooms: linking student outcomes to observed instructional practice. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 19, 262–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5826.2004.00111.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grøver, V., Rydland, V., Gustafsson, J. E., and Snow, C. E. (2020). Shared book reading in preschool supports bilingual children’s second-language learning: a cluster-randomized trial. Child Dev. 91, 2192–2210. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarino, C. M., Hamilton, L. S., Lockwood, J. R., and Rathbun, A. H. (2006). Teacher Qualifications, Instructional Practices, and Reading and Mathematics Gains of Kindergartners (NCES 2006–021). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Educational Statistics.

Google Scholar

Hammer, C. S., Hoff, E., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D. C., and Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: a critical review. Early Child. Res. Q. 29, 715–733. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haney, M., and Hill, J. (2004). Relationships between parent-teaching activities and emergent literacy in preschool children. Early Child Dev. Care 174, 215–228. doi: 10.1080/0300443032000153543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hood, M., Conlon, E., and Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy practices and children's literacy development: a longitudinal analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 252–271. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue, T., Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., and Kirby, J. R. (2018). Examining an extended home literacy model: the mediating roles of emergent literacy skills and reading fluency. Sci. Stud. Read. 22, 273–288. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1435663

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justice, L., Jiang, H., and Strasser, K. (2018). Linguistic environment of preschool classrooms: what dimensions support children’s language growth? Early Child. Res. Q. 42, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A. J., Hamre, B. K., and Pianta, R. C. (2008). Quality of language and literacy instruction in preschool classrooms serving at-risk pupils. Early Child. Res. Q. 23, 51–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupersmidt, J. B., and Martin, S. L. (1997). Mental health problems of children of migrant and seasonal farm workers: a pilot study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 224–232. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., and Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 36, 596–613. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, R., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2019). Preschool book-sharing and oral storytelling experiences in ethnically diverse, low-income families. Early Child Dev. Care 189, 1602–1619. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1400542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, J. (2008). Engagement with print: low-income families and head start children. J. Early Child. Lit. 8, 151–175. doi: 10.1177/1468798408091853

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnuson, K. A., Meyers, M. K., Ruhm, C. J., and Waldfogel, J. (2004). Inequality inpreschool education and school readiness. Am. Educ. Res. J. 41, 115–157. doi: 10.3102/00028312041001115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, S., and Parameswaran, G. (2012). School readiness for young migrant children: the challenge and the outlook. Int. Scholar. Res. Notice. 2012, 1–9. doi: 10.5402/2012/847502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCartney, K., Dearing, E., Taylor, B. A., and Bub, K. L. (2007). Quality child care sup-ports the achievement of low-income children: direct and indirect pathways through caregiving and the home environment. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 28, 411–426. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2007.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehta, K., Gabbard, S. M., Barrat, V., Lewis, M., Carroll, D., and Mines, R. (2000). Findings from the National Agricultural Workers Survey (NAWS), 1997-1998: a demographic and employment profile of United States farm workers. Available at: http://www.dol.gov/asp/programs/agworker/report_8.pdf (Accessed August 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., and Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. J. Soc. Issues 64, 95–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendive, S., Mascareño Lara, M., Aldoney, D., Pérez, J. C., and Pezoa, J. P. (2020). Home language and literacy environments and early literacy trajectories of low-socioeconomic status Chilean children. Child Dev. 91, 2042–2062. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Center for Farmworker Health (NCFH) (2020). Facts about agricultural workers. Available at: http://www.ncfh.org/facts-about-agricultural-workers-fact-sheet.html

Google Scholar

Noble, C., Sala, G., Peter, M., Lingwood, J., Rowland, C., Gobet, F., et al. (2019). The impact of shared book reading on children's language skills: a meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 28:100290. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, R., Barrueco, S., López, M. L., and D’Elio, M. A. (2011). Design of the National Migrant and Seasonal Head Start Survey. Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services.

Google Scholar

Perreira, K. M., Chapman, M. V., and Stein, G. L. (2006). Becoming an American parent: overcoming challenges and finding strength in a new immigrant Latino community. J. Fam. Issues 27, 1383–1414. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06290041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Dev. Psychol. 30, 937–948. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pianta, R. C., Whittaker, J. E., Vitiello, V., Ruzek, E., Ansari, A., and Hofkens, T. (et al., 2020). Children's school readiness skills across the pre-K year: associations with teacher-student interactions, teacher practices, and exposure to academic content. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 66:101084. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2019.101084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piasta, S. B. (2016). Current understandings of what works to support the development of emergent literacy in early childhood classrooms. Child Dev. Perspect. 10, 234–239. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purcell-Gates, V. (2013). Literacy worlds of children of migrant farmworker communities participating in a migrant head start program. Res. Teach. Engl. 20, 68–97.

Google Scholar

Raikes, H., Alexander Pan, B., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., et al. (2006). Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Dev. 77, 924–953. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reese, L., Garnier, H., Gallimore, R., and Goldenberg, C. (2000). Longitudinal analysis of the antecedents of emergent Spanish literacy and middle-school English reading achievement of Spanish-speaking students. Am. Educ. Res. J. 37, 633–662. doi: 10.3102/00028312037003633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reese, E., Sparks, A., and Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children’s language and emergent literacy. J. Early Child. Lit. 10, 97–117. doi: 10.1177/1468798409356987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, E. T., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Dev. 82, 1058–1075. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01614.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, E. T., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Spellmann, M. E., Pan, B. A., Raikes, H., Lugo-Gil, J., et al. (2009). The formative role of home literacy experiences across the first three years of life in children from low-income families. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 30, 677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2009.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolstad, K., Mahoney, K., and Glass, G. V. (2005). The big picture: a meta-analysis of program effectiveness research on English language learners. Educ. Policy 19, 572–594. doi: 10.1177/0895904805278067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawyer, B. E., Hammer, C. S., Cycyk, L. M., López, L., Blair, C., Sandilos, L., et al. (2016). Preschool teachers’ language and literacy practices with dual language learners. Biling. Res. J. 39, 35–49. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2016.1138904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sénéchal, M., and LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 73, 445–460. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sénéchal, M., and LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Dev. 85, 1552–1568. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., and Del Tufo, S. N. (2022). Parent-child shared book reading mediates the impact of socioeconomic status on heritage language learners’ emergent literacy. Early Child. Res. Q. 59, 254–264. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J., and Johnson, D. J. (2019). Parental perspectives on education: Mexican and Mexican-American farmworker families with young children enrolled in migrant and seasonal head start. J. Latinos Educ 21, 388–403. doi: 10.1080/15348431.2019.1666010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soderstrom, M., and Wittebolle, K. (2013). When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PLoS One 8, 1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solari, E. J., Zucker, T. A., Landry, S. H., and Williams, J. M. (2016). Relative effects of a comprehensive versus reduced training for head start teachers who serve Spanish-speaking English learners. Early Educ. Dev. 27, 1060–1076. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1158610

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, L., Luo, R., and Liang, E. (2022). Dual language development of Chinese 3-and 4-year-olds: associations with the family context and teachers’ language use. Early Educ. Dev. 33, 219–242. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2020.1865746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnenschein, S., Stapleton, L. M., and Benson, A. (2010). The relation between the type and amount of instruction and growth in children’s reading competencies. Am. Educ. Res. J. 47, 358–389. doi: 10.3102/0002831209349215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. Clair, L., Jackson, B., and Zweiback, R. (2012). Six years later: effect of family involvement training on the language skills of children from migrant families. Sch. Community J. 22, 9–19.

Google Scholar

Stata Corp. (2021). Stata: Release 17. Statistical Software. College Station, TX: Stata Corp LLC.

Google Scholar

Stechuk, R. A., and Burns, M. S. (2005). Making a Difference: A Framework for Supporting First and Second Language Development in Preschool Children of Migrant Farm Workers. Washington, DC: Academy for Educational Development.

Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Custode, S., Kuchirko, Y., Escobar, K., and Lo, T. (2018). Routine language: speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Dev. 90, 2135–2152. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Luo, R., McFadden, K. E., Bandel, E. T., and Vallotton, C. (2019). Early home learning environment predicts children’s 5th grade academic skills. Appl. Dev. Sci. 23, 153–169. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2017.1345634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavassolie, T., López, C., De Feyter, J., Hartman, S. C., and Winsler, A. (2018). Migrant preschool children's school readiness and early elementary school performance. J. Educ. Res. 111, 331–344. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2016.1261074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, Z. E., and Ruiz, Y. (2017). Contextual stressors and the mental health outcomes of Latino children in rural migrant-farmworker families in the Midwest. J. Rural Ment. Health 41, 284–298. doi: 10.1037/rmh0000082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Pluijim, M., van Gelderen, A., and Kessels, J. (2019). Activities and strategies for parents with less education to promote the oral language development of their children: a review of empirical interventions. Sch. Community J. 29, 317–362.

Google Scholar

Vernon-Feagans, L., and Bratsch-Hines, M. E., Family Life Project Key Investigators (2013). Caregiver–child verbal interactions in child care: a buffer against poor language outcomes when maternal language input is less. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 858–873. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vernon-Feagans, L., Mokrova, I. L., Carr, R. C., Garrett-Peters, P. T., and Burchinal, M. R., Family Life Project Key Investigators (2019). Cumulative years of classroom quality from kindergarten to third grade: prediction to children’s third grade literacy skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 47, 531–540. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Votruba-Drzal, E., Coley, R. L., and Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2004). Child care quality and low-income children's development: for who is child care most important? Child Dev. 75, 296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00670.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Read. Dev. Child. 23, 34–41.

Google Scholar

White, L. J., Fernandez, V. A., and Greenfield, D. B. (2020). Assessing classroom quality for Latino dual language learners in head start: DLL-specific and general teacher-child interaction perspectives. Early Educ. Dev. 31, 599–627. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2019.1680785

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., and Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Dev. Psychol. 30, 679–689. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willard, J. A., Kohl, K., Bihler, L. M., Agache, A., and Leyendecker, B. (2021). Family literacy activities and their interplay with family and preschool language environments: links to gains in dual language learners’ German vocabulary. Early Educ. Dev. 32, 189–208. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2020.1744404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodcock, R. W., Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F., Ruef, M., and Alvarado, C. G. (2005). Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey, Revised Normative Update. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside.

Google Scholar

Xue, Y., and Meisels, S. J. (2004). Early literacy instruction and learning in kindergarten: evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study—kindergarten class of 1998–1999. Am. Educ. Res. J. 41, 191–229. doi: 10.3102/00028312041001191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zepeda, M., Castro, D. C., and Cronin, S. (2011). Preparing early childhood teachers to work with young dual language learners. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 10–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00141.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zucker, T. A., Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Pentimonti, J. M., and Kaderavek, J. N. (2013). The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1425–1439. doi: 10.1037/a0030347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dual language learners, Migrant and Seasonal Head Start, emergent literacy, home language and literacy activities, classroom language and literacy activities

Citation: Luo R and Song L (2022) The unique and compensatory effects of home and classroom learning activities on Migrant and Seasonal Head Start children’s Spanish and English emergent literacy skills. Front. Psychol. 13:1016492. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1016492

Received: 11 August 2022; Accepted: 31 October 2022;
Published: 25 November 2022.

Edited by:

He Sun, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

Reviewed by:

Rica Ramirez, University of Texas at San Antonio, United States
Vicky Chondrogianni, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Luo and Song. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rufan Luo, rufan.luo@asu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.