Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 14 December 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Executive Functions, Self-Regulation and External-Regulation: Relations and new evidence View all 12 articles

Executive function and effortful control—Similar and different evidence from big data analysis

  • Department of Education, Art and Humanities College, Gangneung–Wonju National University, Gangneung-si, South Korea

Introduction: The current study explored commonalities and similarities between executive function (EF) and effortful control (EC).

Methods: The major empirical studies published between 2013 and 2022 in the World of Science (WoS) was collected. The bibliographic information was systematically analyzed.

Results and discussion: (1) EC is the efficiency of executive attention that incorporates inhibitory control (IC), attentional control, activation mainly related to temperament. On the other hand, EF is the efficiency of self-directed action that encompasses IC, working memory (WM), and shifting/cognitive flexibility in particular focuses on the cognitive aspect. (2) EF research has overwhelmingly outnumbered EC research (2,000 EF studies vs. 50 EC studies per year). (3) According to a co-word analysis with keyword co-occurrences, the subject of preschool students and individual differences co-occurred in EF studies. (4) EC usually occurs with working memory and early childhood. In the more detailed analysis of the articles, the EF and EC studies used younger subject groups than older subject groups. EC studies were especially likely to use subjects in early childhood. (5) The Delis–Kaplan Tests of Executive Functioning System (D-KEFS) was the most commonly used test for EF. In contrast, the EC used self-report surveys such as the Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ). This research illustrates and discusses key findings in the EC and EF data and provides suggestions for future study directions.

Introduction

Self-regulation (SR) has been an important topic in learning and education for the past 130 years since Hall (1891) mentioned a “volitional” reaction as a concept instead of an “unconscious” reaction (Post et al., 2006). SR has traditionally been described in the context of educational and settings, as the ability to comply with a request” (Kopp, 1982), that results in initiating and ceasing activities. More recently, such ideas were expanded and specified to focus on goal-directed activities (Inzlicht et al., 2021). Given the idea, SR can be defined as activities to achieve goals in the context of human learning and socialization. These activities aims to develope both tempermantal and cognitive aspects.

The main constructs of self-regulation are executive function (EF) and effortful control (EC). EF is a self-directed action necessary in selecting and creating a goal, and it refers to implementing the goal and maintaining the behavior toward the goal (Baggetta and Alexander, 2016). Researchers note that EF is a construct composed of the following main components: (1) inhibitory control (IC), (2) working memory (WM), and (3) shifting/cognitive flexibility (Baggetta and Alexander, 2016). On the other hand, EC is “the efficiency of executive attention, including the ability to inhibit a dominant response, to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors”(Rothbart and Bates, 2006, p. 129). Therefore, EC includes underlying constructs of (1) IC, (2) attentional control, and (3) activation. EC pertains more to emotional activities in nature and is a concept particularly focused on temperament. Given the conceptual definition, inhibition is a common notion penetrating EF and EC. In addition to structural similarity, EF and EC share a functional similarity: executive attention (Zhou et al., 2012). Due to this conceptual and functional similarity, one can often see an overlap in the use of EF and EC measurement tools. For instance, Go/No Go and Stroop testing are representative tools commonly used in EF (e.g., Belghali et al., 2022) and EC (e.g., Lengua et al., 2007). However, despite the conceptual similarities, there are differences between EF and EC studies. EF is primarily associated with self-regulating activities governed by a cognitive-psychological approach, the so-called “cool system” (Mischel et al., 2003). On the other hand, researchers have studied EC with the “hot system,” i.e., more emotion-laden regulatory activities. One core construct missing in EF research but not EC research drives this difference: working memory (Zhou et al., 2012). For instance, working memory is the most crucial cerebral activity in reasoning and academic performance (Gilhooly, 2004) and is relevant to attention (Gioia et al., 2002).

More recently, Gagne (2017) used temperament-based and neural systems approaches to distinguish between EC and EF. We can easily understand EC from a temperament-based approach, whereas EF needs a more neural systems approach. When understanding those concepts from self-control perspectives, the EF IC underlies cognitive functions, but the EC IC underlies emotional temperament dimensions (Liew, 2012). Regardless of academic history and trends, educational practices in the field use both concepts interchangeably (Gagne, 2017). Some scholars even argued for synthesizing both perspectives (Liew, 2012).

As described above, the distinction between EC and EF seems complicated due to the difficulty distinguishing between cognitive–emotional development and the commonality of measures and instruments. Existing literature does not address these problems sufficiently from a systematic data-based review. Thus, the current study explores these problems from several points. First, we review EF and EC studies to understand people circumvented by drastic technological, social, and pathological changes over the past ten years (2012–2022), such as those confronting online blended learning. Advances in research have led to the development and introduction of new psychometric measurements. In addition, a systematic analysis of the relevant literature is necessary to figure out more scientifically the commonalities and/or similarities between EF and EC. The current study drives these research gaps with the following specific research questions.

In the general educational context and for typically developing human beings, what are the similarities and differences between EF and EC regarding:

1. The number of publications by year?

2. Study characteristics revealed in the keywords?

3. Definitions?

4. Instruments and subjects?

Therefore, this study clarifies the conceptual and psychometric differences between EF and EC through big data-based analysis. However, this effort does not argue against a conceptual distinction between EF and EC. Instead, the present study reveals how to explain EF and EC under the umbrella term of self-regulation. Furthermore, this clarification could function as a base to suggest how to synthesize these two concepts in the field.

Methods

Search process

I used several search parameters and steps to drive an adequate dataset for answering the research questions. First, I collected studies from the Web of Science (WoS) database with the following search parameters: published since 2013 in peer-reviewed academic journals stamped with Social Science Citation Index (SSCI), or Science Citation Index (SCI), or Art and Humanity Citation Index (A&HCI) because the indices already screen quality studies. I only used English, human learning and performance, empirical studies in nature, and behavioral or neuroimaging instruments as search terms to represent the research topics. For instance, I excluded studies using meta-analysis on the effects of EF and EC (Sung et al., 2022) to avoid redundancy in the meta-analysis and empirical studies. Second, because this review’s principal goal was to find commonality and distinction between EF and EC in their concepts and operations, I created two data pools in the keywords: one containing executive function and another containing effortful control. The initial search process resulted in a collection of 17,038 EF studies and 482 EC studies. I downloaded the data on May 4, 2022.

Due to the many retrieved articles, the next step was to narrow the initial data pools to manageable levels. Thus, I filtered the EF studies based on their inclusion in the “HIGH H INDEX” category offered by the WoS database. This second step resulted in 85 EF articles for generating thematic maps. Finally, I further narrowed the datasets for more analyses (keyword co-occurrences, concepts, subjects, and instruments). Figure 1 summarizes the data filtering steps.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Data filtering process and the products using the data pool for executive function and effortful control studies.

Analysis

I obtained the number of publications by year from the initial search data from the WoS, which included 17,038 EF and 482 EC studies. To figure out study characteristics in the keywords, I considered 85 articles with high ranks according to the WoS search index for the EF and EC pools, respectively. First, I analyzed these pools’ keywords and obtained thematic maps. Next, I extracted keyword co-occurrences for the EF and EC pools with 30 top high-ranked articles. Finally, I used the Bibliometrix package in R (Aria and Cuccurullo, 2017) to map the themes and co-occurrences with keywords from the pools.

In addition, to address differences in the concepts/operations, subjects, and instruments, I analyzed 15 highly-referenced articles from each of the EF and EC collections. Then, I extracted conceptual similarities and differences by reviewing the collected papers. Finally, after the physical screening, I examined the EF and EC measurements and population groups.

Results and discussion

Annual publication

Figure 2 illustrates the initial search process, where bar charts represent the number of publications by year, and the line charts are the percentage of publications within that year out of the total published articles over the recent decade. As displayed in the left chart, the number of EF study publications steadily increased from approximately 1,400 to 2,200. On the other hand, the annual EC publications remained similar from 2013 to 2016 (about 40), then almost doubled from about 40 in 2016 to 80 in 2019. The increment was again steady afterward. Regardless of the trend in the annual publication rates, the number of total publications over the decade contrasts between EF and EC. While EC studies are about 50 per year, EF studies are approximately 2,000 per year, i.e., 400 times more than EC studies.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Amount of publications for executive function studies (left) versus effortful control studies (right) by year (2013–2022).

Study characteristics revealed in keywords

Thematic maps using keywords

I mapped clusters of keywords on a two-dimensional diagram covering density and centrality to enable an understanding of significant research trends (Figures 3, 4). Centrality refers to the degree of interaction a cluster has with other parts of the network. Density means the degree to which a particular keyword appears in the content several times (Hu et al., 2013). The thematic map is an intuitive plot that locates the themes according to the quadrant: (1) the upper right quadrant refers to the motor theme, (2) the lower right presents the basic theme, (3) the lower left quadrant means emerging and declining themes, and (4) the top left quadrant is the specialized/niche theme.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Thematic map of 85 executive function studies (2013–2022). Each quadrant refers to (1) a motor theme, (2) a basic theme, (3) an emerging/declining theme, and (4) a specialized/niche theme.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Thematic map on 85 effortful control studies (2013–2022). Each quadrant refers to (1) a motor theme, (2) a basic theme, (3) an emerging/declining theme, and (4) a specialized/niche theme.

Figure 3 shows the thematic map for the discourse in executive function studies. The motor themes of the EF studies (quadrant 1) conveyed school-readiness interventions for children. In addition, I observed a prevalence of basic (quadrant 2) and niche themes (quadrant 4). The basic themes covered three chunks: the first chunk regards older adults’ cognitive impairment (e.g., Alzheimer’s, dementia), the second chunk pertains to children’s deficits (e.g., neuropsychological performance and schizophrenia), and the third chunk concerns memory (short-term and long-term). Overall, the basic themes retrieved from the EF studies were relevant to age-specific cognitive malfunctioning. Niche themes (quadrant 4) were pertinent to selective attention, Asperger syndrome, and executive dysfunction.

Figure 4 shows a thematic map highlighting the discourse in effortful control studies. The hot topics of the EC studies, presented in motor themes (quadrant 1), conveyed personality and emotional regulation in early childhood. The “hot” system weighing temperament and emotion seemed closely related to the EC studies, as noted by Mischel et al. (2003). As opposed to older adults as focal research subjects in EF studies, the basic themes for EC studies (quadrant 2) comprised three clusters mainly dealing with young children. The first keyword cluster was young children’s temperament, the second cluster regarded children’s EF and attention, and the third covered petrophysical functioning (e.g., prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex) concerning attention and delay. Developing mechanisms, randomized control, and cognitive deficits were niche themes (quadrant 4) in the EC studies, i.e., themes for specific fields.

In addition, I located three chunks of themes in the center of the chart regarding the relevance degree of EF studies. The first chunk pertained to expertise and decision-making. The second included school readiness for preschool and elementary school students. The last chunk was about adolescents’ inhibitory control and performance, which showed sparse density compared to the first two chunks. Finally, confirmatory factor analysis for the EC behavior rating inventory resulted in emerging or declining themes (quadrant 3).

Co-word analysis with keyword co-occurrences

A program generated a visual word map of co-word networks to uncover links between concepts through term co-occurrences. As one can observe from Figure 5, four major chunks of keywords emerged from the 30 most cited EF studies according to the degree to which the keywords were likely to occur together. Individual differences in preschool children appeared, and performance co-occurred with inhibition, brain, and attention in childhood. Schizophrenia and school readiness also strongly co-occurred with executive function. Finally, the older adult presented together with dementia and memory impairment.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Keyword co-occurrence plots on 30 most cited executive function studies (2013–2022).

Likewise, Figure 6 shows three co-occurring chunks of keywords. Effortful control arose with working memory, early childhood, and preschool children. Self-regulation also comprised a big keyword chunk with achievement and temperament in this study pool. Finally, individual differences, IC, and personality co-occurred and were strongly related to EF. These trends were similar to what I found in the thematic maps (Figures 3, 4).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Keyword co-occurrence plots on 30 most cited effortful control studies (2013–2022).

Concepts of executive function and effortful control

In addition to the above structural analysis for recent EF and EC studies, I performed a semantic analysis to comprehend academic definitions of these two constructs. I retrieved 15 top-cited articles from each study pool. The explicit descriptions in the articles are as follows (Tables 1, 2). According to these references, the most common use of adjectives defining EF included “goal-directed” (e.g., Benson et al., 2013), “domain-general” (e.g., Lucas et al., 2013), and “task-related” (Gijselaers et al., 2017). The components or processes for defining EF were “self-regulation,” “control,” “working memory,” “inhibition,” “planning,” “attention,” and “shifting” (e.g., Rhodes et al., 2016). EF is a multifaceted construct comprising higher-order and lower-order functions. For instance, Gijselaers et al. (2017) viewed EF as a hierarchical construct of common EF and EF-specific variation. In addition, “cognitive” processes (e.g., Niermeyer et al., 2019) were salient for attributes. This overall trend is consistent with Zhou, Chen, and Main’s study Zhou et al. (2012). However, other studies also mentioned “emotional” and “social” processes (e.g., Lima et al., 2014). The most cited articles defined EF as a cognitive process underlying goal-directed and task-related behavior and a multifaceted construct, including self-regulation, working memory, inhibition, planning, attention, and shifting. The EF can also encompass emotional and social regulatory processes.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Explicit definitions of the executive function retrieved from 15 most cited articles.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Explicit definitions of effortful control retrieved from 15 most cited articles.

While EF regarded more “what to do,” EC highlighted “what not to do.” The most cited articles often mentioned “inhibit a dominant response,” “suppress impulsive or premature responses,” and “self-regulation” in their definition. In addition to these highlights on IC over premature and unnecessary responses, studies included “activation of a subdominant response” and “reactivity” as core components of EC. Following Zhou et al.’s (2012)’ study, definitions and operations indicated that EF and EC’s commonality often included inhibition as a core construct. In addition, researchers discriminated EC from EF because EC is more of a “temperament” (Lipsey et al., 2017). I also found this trend in the current analysis.

Instruments and subjects

The common test for EF is the Delis–Kaplan Tests of Executive Functioning System (D-KEFS), which includes Wisconsin Card Sorting (to measure shifting), Trail Making (to measure IC), and the verbal fluency test (to measure working memory) (see Table 3). Otherwise, researchers used similar tasks to measure the underlying constructs of shifting, inhibitory control, and working memory. For instance, Benson et al. (2013) examined children’s shifting ability with the “Bear/Dragon” game, similar to the “Simon Says” game. Other studies often measured shifting ability with a card sorting test (e.g., Lucas et al., 2013).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Instruments and subject of executive function retrieved from 15 most cited articles (Supplementary Appendix).

When it comes to EC, the major research instrument is the self-report survey. For instance, six out of 15 EC studies used the Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ) (e.g., Lin et al., 2013; Zeytinoglu et al., 2017) or the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (EATQ). Evans and Rothbart (2007) developed the original ATQ in 35 items capturing (1) attention control (12 items), (2) activation control (12 items), and (3) IC (11 items). Each item asks the respondent to indicate their agreement with a statement (e.g., “Although the assignment is hard, I can finish it on time”). Later, researchers revised and published a shorter version with 17 items for adolescents. The next instrument researchers frequently used was the Delay-of-Gratification, applied in three studies (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2019).

In terms of subject groups, EC studies (Table 4) involved very young subjects such as infants (Kim et al., 2013) or toddlers (Sulik et al., 2015; Lin et al., 2019). In contrast, EF studies (seven out of 15) used children (Benson et al., 2013) as a subject group. This phenomenon seems to pertain to the cognitive development process of humans. In childhood, corresponding to the early stage of development, the brain is less myelinated and thus shows very distracted brain activity (Brydges et al., 2013). As a result, children’s IC for minimizing and simplifying unnecessary tasks to achieve goals is weaker than adolescents’ (Atherton et al., 2020). In addition, effortful control develops around two years of age and rapidly in infancy (Kim et al., 2013).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Instruments and subject of effortful control retrieved from 15 most cited articles (Supplementary Appendix).

In addition, there is a shared belief in establishing EC early as possible for satisfactory human socialization and schooling (Eisenberg et al., 2003). For instance, psychologists have chosen infant EC as their research topic following the EC’s critical period and its ripple effect on infants’ lives (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2013; Lipsey et al., 2017). In contrast to the research gap between EC and EF in using infants as study subjects, researchers used adolescents with a similar frequency (5 out of 15) between EF (e.g., Rhodes et al., 2016) and EC studies (e.g., Bao et al., 2015). Researchers were less likely to use adult subject groups for EC and EF studies; however, I found one more article in the EF study pool than in the EC study pool. In sum, the EF and EC studies used younger subjects more often than older subjects. In addition, EC studies were especially likely to use subjects in early childhood.

Key findings

This study explored the common attributes and differences between EF and EC based on the results of major empirical studies published between 2013 and 2022. As a result of big data analysis using bibliographic information published in the World of Science (WoS), major published papers found a slight difference between EC and EF in terms of concepts, measures, instruments, and subjects of use.

Hot effortful control and cool executive function

As per the definition, the efficiency of executive attention that incorporates inhibitory control (IC), attentional control, activation mainly related to temperament. On the other hand, most EF studies focused on the cognitive rather than the affective aspect. The keyword analysis also showed a slightly more pronounced difference between the two research streams. According to the keyword thematic topic analysis, in the EF studies, cognitive keywords such as “working memory” and “short-term memory” appeared as base themes. On the other hand, the EC studies include temperament as the base theme leading the basic flow of the study.

Metcalfe and Mischel’s (1999) hot versus cool framework explains the given conceptual differences well through a hot/cool system; humans have a two-fold interactive processing system. The hot system is the “go” system because it follows an emotional process and responds immediately and simply. It decreases under stress and is necessary for the control of external stimuli. On the other hand, the cool system follows a cognitive process, develops slowly and late, and has the nickname “know” system. When stressed, the cool system becomes a stimulus rather than an activation and is necessary for voluntary control. The EF functions based on a cool system, whereas the EC is based on a hot system.

Regarding measures and instruments, the EF–EC distinction needs further discussion. Indeed, the present analysis of the measures showed overlaps between the two concepts. For example, major EF studies used such comprehensive batteries as D-KEFS, which highly rely on cognitive interaction activity time, such as the Sorting Test and Tower Test. At the same time, there was considerable use of performance tests (e.g., Go/No Go, Trail-Making) that measure immediate response in EF studies. EC researchers also switched between instruments based on hot and cool systems. For instance, Kim et al. (2013) used a representative hot system-based measure called “Delay of Gratification” to measure EC and a cool system-based measure such as “Go/No Go.” The Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ) (Evans and Rothbart, 2007), which frequently appears in EC research, is based on effortful control, consisting of three sub-constructs: activation control, attention control, and IC. Attention control is close to cerebral activity, and IC is an item measuring temperamental activity. It is challenging to differentiate between these two constructs due to the ambiguity of the hot–cool systems in the EC and EF measurement tools and their use. Nevertheless, we can understand this commonality in the same context as what was argued by the existent literature (e.g., Liew, 2012; Gagne, 2017).

Younger subjects used in effortful control studies than in executive function studies

A more noticeable difference was captured between the EC and EF studies concerning the study subjects. Statistically, participants’ ages in EC studies were lower than in EF studies. Researchers argue that EC of self-regulation abilities critically develops at 22–33 months of age (Bernier et al., 2010); some even claim 12–18 months as a critical period in EC development (Kochanska and Knaack, 2003). Thus, there seems to be an age difference between EC and EF development. Moreover, EC researchers predominantly used infants or toddlers in their studies. In contrast, EF studies used children older than the EC’s major study participants but still young. The EF and EC studies with this interest in children support existing studies (Montroy et al., 2016) that early stages of human development result in differentiated self-regulation.

There is a link between the age difference of study subjects and the main topics covered in EF and EC studies. For example, research topics that form a significant trend regarding EF were school readiness and interventions related to school adjustment. This finding is of interest to researchers considering that the subjects of EF studies are mainly children. In addition, the main keywords such as “emotion regulation,” “personality,” and “event-related” confirm the flow of EC research. One can infer emotion regulation and personality to accompany EC studies, considering the operational definition of EC frequently includes temperament. However, more direct measures such as the event-related instrument would be useful when researchers pay attention to babies before language development because the subjects’ self-report is unavailable, and their behaviors are not easy to interpret.

Future directions

In this study, I explored the similarities and differences between EC and EF through big data analysis of major studies over the past decade. Still, undoubtedly, we need more work. Therefore, I derived several important future research topics in summarizing this study’s key findings.

In terms of publication numbers over the past decade, EF research has overwhelmingly outnumbered EC research (2,000 EF studies vs. 50 EC studies per year). Few researchers are studying self-regulation or IC from an emotional perspective, as few invoke EC. Most come from the EF perspective. The difference in publication number relates to the analysis results in which the academic and operational definition of EF often already includes the academic and operational definition of EC. Researchers know less about self-regulation in the hot system (EC). Furthermore, researchers have usually paid attention to EC as a way to solve emotional problems such as violence and delinquency in children and adolescents (Eisenberg et al., 2003). However, we must advance studies on the EC development of older subjects such as adults and the elderly.

It seems necessary to make EC and EF typography a broad spectrum. In other words, when and how we differentiate the EC and EF sub-constructs, it is essential to map them according to the stage of human development. One can start the discussion with the example of studies on inhibition, a key and basic construct of EF and EC. IC appeared to show individual differences around the age of one to two at the onset of toddlerhood (e.g., Montroy et al., 2016). If so, when will the remaining sub-constructs (working memory, shifting, planning, organization, and attentional control) become noticeably differentiated? The answer to this question will provide the basic idea needed to grow and develop EF and EC, a psychological construct that directly impacts academic performance. The answer depends on devising a program for children’s cognitive development or providing an educational environment.

Furthermore, it is necessary to broaden the understanding of determinants and outcome variables related to the development of EC and EF. For instance, one can ask how a person’s EC and EF develop or change before and after school age. How can EC and EF change when the person is situated in public education or home-schooling becuase these two environments involve different levels of temperament and cognitive engagement. This elaboration of the research questions may expand the existing EF and EC studies.

In addition, research on constructs of the agents also seems to need specification. For instance, the IC appeared to be a common core construct across EF and EC. At the same time, research has shown that the IC develops drastically during childhood. Thus, the systematic analysis of the IC studies targeting childhood would elaborate on the EF and EC differences and commonalities.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

The Gangneung-Wonju National University will be in charge of payment of the process fee.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1004403/full#supplementary-material

References

Ahadi, S. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (1994). “Temperament, development, and the big five,” in The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood, eds G. A. Kohnstamm and C. F. Halverson (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 189–207.

Google Scholar

Alvarez, J. A., and Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 16, 17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. J. (2008). “Towards a developmental model of executive function,” in Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective, eds V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, and P. J. Anderson (Hove: Psychology Press), 3–21.

Google Scholar

Aria, M., and Cuccurullo, C. (2017). Bibliometrix: An R-tool for comprehensive science mapping analysis. J. Inform. 11, 959–975. doi: 10.1016/j.joi.2017.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atherton, O. E., Lawson, K. M., and Robins, R. W. (2020). The development of effortful control from late childhood to young adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 119, 417–456. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, Z., Li, D., Zhang, W., and Wang, Y. (2015). School climate and delinquency among Chinese adolescents: Analyses of effortful control as a moderator and deviant peer affiliation as a mediator. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 81–93. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9903-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they work, and why they evolved. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Belghali, M., Statsenko, Y., and Laver, V. (2022). Stroop switching card test: Brief screening of executive functions across the lifespan. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 29, 14–33. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2020.1844865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., and Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1615–1627. doi: 10.1037/a0031056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in Kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brydges, C. R., Anderson, M., Reid, C. L., and Fox, A. M. (2013). Maturation of cognitive control: Delineating response inhibition and interference suppression. PLoS One 8:e69826. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capaldi, D. M., and Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Development and validation of an early adolescent temperament measure. J. Early Adolesc. 12, 153–173. doi: 10.1177/0272431692012002002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., and Faja, S. (2013). “Executive function,” in The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 1): Body and mind, ed. P. D. Zelazo (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 706–743. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., and Shiner, R. L. (2006). “Personality development,” in Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development, 6th Edn, Vol. 3, eds W. Damon, R. Lerner, and N. Eisenberg (New York, NY: Wiley), 300–365.

Google Scholar

Cummings, J. L., and Miller, B. L. (2007). “Conceptual and clinical aspects of the frontal lobes,” in The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders, eds B. L. Miller and J. L. Cummings (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 12–21.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive functioning system: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. doi: 10.1037/t15082-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derryberry, D., and Rothbart, M. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 633–652. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 335–341. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Tsukayama, E., and Kirby, T. A. (2013). Is it really self-control? Examining the predictive power of the delay of gratification task. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 843–855. doi: 10.1177/0146167213482589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baggetta, P., and Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 10, 10–33. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2003). The relations of effortful control and ego control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Dev. Psychol. 39, 761–776. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, L. K., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). “Revision of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire,” in Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the society for research in child development, Minneapolis, MN. doi: 10.1037/t07624-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, D. E., and Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. J. Res. Pers. 41, 868–888. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., and Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J. Exp. Psychol. Genet. 137, 201–225. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagne, J. R. (2017). Self-control in childhood: A synthesis of perspectives and focus on early development. Child Dev. Perspect. 11, 127–132. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., and Rothbart, M. K. (2012). Etiology of preschool behavior problems: Contributions of temperament attributes in early childhood. Infant Ment. Health J. 33, 197–211. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gijselaers, H. J. M., Meijs, C., Neroni, J., Kirschner, P. A., and de Groot, R. H. (2017). Updating and not shifting predicts learning performance in young and middle-aged adults. Mind Brain Educ. 11, 190–200. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilhooly, K. J. (2004). “Working memory and reasoning,” in The nature of reasoning, eds J. P. Leighton and R. J. Sternberg (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 49–77. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511818714.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Kenworthy, L., and Barton, R. M. (2002). Profiles of everyday executive function in acquired and developmental disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 8, 121–137. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.121.8727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guy, S. C., Isquith, P. K., and Gioia, G. A. (2004). BRIEF-SR: Behavior rating inventory of executive function-self-report version: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Hall, G. S. (1891). Notes on the study of infants. Pedagog. Semin. 1, 127–138. doi: 10.1080/08919402.1891.10533929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, C. P., Hu, J. M., Deng, S. L., and Liu, Y. (2013). A co-word analysis of library and information science in China. Scientometrics 97, 369–382. doi: 10.1007/s11192-013-1076-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., Werner, K. M., Briskin, J. L., and Roberts, B. W. (2021). Integrating models of self-regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 319–345. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-061020-105721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., and Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children’s behavior problems and academic performance. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 43–56. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochanska, G., and Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. J. Pers. 71, 1087–1112. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.7106008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochanska, G., Murray, K., and Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Dev. Psychol. 36, 220–232. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Dev. Psychol. 18, 199–214. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., and Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 21, 59–80. doi: 10.1348/026151003321164627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., and Bush, N. R. (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 28, 40–55. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (ed.). (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY: Oxford university press.

Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., and Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment, 5th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., and Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive functions, and education: Bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 105–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, A. B., Moreira, F., Gomes, M. D. M., and Maia-Filho, H. (2014). Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of executive function in a sample of children and adolescents with idiopathic epilepsy. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 72, 954–959. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20140191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, B., Liew, J., and Perez, M. (2019). Measurement of self-regulation in early childhood: Relations between laboratory and performance-based measures of effortful control and executive functioning. Early Child. Res. Q. 47, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, W. L., Hsu, K. Y., Chen, H. C., and Chang, W. Y. (2013). Different attentional traits, different creativities. Think. Skills Creat. 9, 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2012.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsey, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., Dong, N., Fuhs, M. W., and Wilson, S. J. (2017). Learning-related cognitive self-regulation measures for prekindergarten children: A comparative evaluation of the educational relevance of selected measures. J. Educ. Psychol. 109, 1084–1102. doi: 10.1037/edu0000203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logie, R. H., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., and Baddeley, A. D. (2004). Is there a specific executive capacity for dual-task coordination? Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 18, 504–513. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, A. J., Lewis, C., Pala, F. C., Wong, K., and Berridge, D. (2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers’ selective trust: Three cultures compared. Dev. Psychol. 49, 579–590. doi: 10.1037/a0029864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J., and Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev. 106, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., and Mendoza-Denton, R. (2003). “Sustaining delay of gratification over time: A hot-cool systems perspective,” in Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice, eds G. Loewenstein, D. Read, and R. Baumeister (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 175–200.

Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., and Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1744–1762. doi: 10.1037/dev0000159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R. G., Evendon, J. L., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (1987). “Computer-aided assessment of dementia: Comparative studies of neuropsychological deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s disease,” in Cognitive neurochemistry, eds S. M. Stahl, S. D. Iverson, and E. C. Goodman (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 21–36.

Google Scholar

Niermeyer, M. A., Ziemnik, R. E., Franchow, E. I., Barron, C. A., and Suchy, Y. (2019). Greater naturally occurring expressive suppression is associated with poorer executive functioning and motor-sequence learning among older adults. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 41, 118–132. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1502257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., and Rappley, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 59–66. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200201000-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, Y., Boyer, W., and Brett, L. (2006). A historical examination of self-regulation: Helping children now and in the future. Early Child. Educ. J. 34, 5–14. doi: 10.1007/s10643-006-0107-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 386–401. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, S. M., Booth, J. N., Palmer, L. E., Blythe, R. A., Delibegovic, M., and Wheate, N. J. (2016). Executive functions predict conceptual learning of science. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 34, 261–275. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. J. Pers. 71, 1113–1144. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.7106009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., and Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 122–135. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., and Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Dev. 72, 1394–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (1998). “Temperament,” in Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development, 5th Edn, Vol. 3, eds W. Damon and N. Eisenberg (New York, NY: John Wiley), 105–176.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (2006). “Temperament,” in Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development, eds N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, and R. M. Lerner (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 99–166.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., and Posner, M. I. (1994). “A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament,” in Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior, eds J. E. Bates and T. D. Wachs (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 83–116. doi: 10.1037/10149-003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, M. R. (2012). “Effortful control,” in Handbook of temperament, eds M. Zentner and R. L. Shiner (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 145–167.

Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1991). Symbol digit modalities test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T., Picton, T. W., and Alexander, M. P. (2001). “Consciousness, self-awareness, and the frontal lobes,” in The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness, eds S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, and J. D. Duffy (Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc), 101–109.

Google Scholar

Suchy, Y. (2015). Executive functioning: A comprehensive guide for clinical practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Sulik, M. J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., and Silva, K. M. (2015). Associations between respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) reactivity and effortful control in preschool-age children. Dev. Psychobiol. 57, 596–606. doi: 10.1002/dev.21315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sung, M. C., Ku, B., Leung, W., and MacDonald, M. (2022). The effect of physical activity interventions on executive function among people with neurodevelopmental disorders: A meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 52, 1030–1050. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05009-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, B. A., Evans, J. J., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., and Emslie, H. (1996). Behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Müller, U. (2011). “Executive function in typical and atypical development,” in The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, ed. U. Goswami (Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons Ltda), 574–603. doi: 10.1002/9781444325485.ch22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeytinoglu, S., Calkins, S. D., Swingler, M. M., and Leerkes, E. M. (2017). Pathways from maternal effortful control to child self-regulation: The role of maternal emotional support. J. Fam. Psychol. 31, 170–180. doi: 10.1037/fam0000271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., and Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 112–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive function (EF), effortful control (EC), big data analysis, bibliographic information, Delis–Kaplan Tests of Executive Functioning System, Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire

Citation: Chae SE (2022) Executive function and effortful control—Similar and different evidence from big data analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:1004403. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1004403

Received: 27 July 2022; Accepted: 07 November 2022;
Published: 14 December 2022.

Edited by:

Unai Diaz-Orueta, Maynooth University, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Shameem Fatima, COMSATS University Islamabad, Pakistan
Jeffrey R. Gagne, Texas A&M University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Chae. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Soo Eun Chae, c2NoYWVAZ3dudS5hYy5rcg==; orcid.org/0000-0001-6413-7469

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.