Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 23 December 2021
Sec. Consciousness Research

Executive Functioning: Assessing the Role of Perceived Paranormal Ability

  • 1Department of Psychology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, United Kingdom
  • 2Adelphi Values Ltd, Bollington, United Kingdom
  • 3Blanquerna Foundation, Barcelona, Spain

This study examined whether scores on self-report measures of executive functions varied in accordance with level of self-professed paranormal ability. The investigators compared three groups varying in attribution of paranormal facilities: practitioners (Mediums, Psychics, Spiritualists and Fortune-Tellers), self-professed ability and no ability. Consistent with recent research on cognitive-perceptual factors allied to delusional formation and thinking style, the researchers anticipated that practitioners would score higher on paranormal belief and self-reported executive function disruption. Correspondingly, the investigators also hypothesised that the self-professed ability group would demonstrate greater belief in the paranormal and higher levels of executive function disruption than the no ability group. A sample of 499 (219 males, 279 females) respondents completed the measures online. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) found a large effect size, alongside significant differences on all variables apart from Cognitive Reappraisal. Pairwise comparisons indicated that Paranormal Belief increased as a function of level of ability; practitioners scored higher than self-professed, who in turn scored higher than the no ability group. For executive functioning, significant differences emerged only for the no ability vs. self-professed ability and no ability vs. practising groups. Collectively, outcomes indicated that perception of ability, regardless of intensity of paranormal conviction, influenced subjective appraisal of executive functions. Failure to find consistent differences between practitioner and self-professed ability groups suggested that discernment of ability was sufficient to heighten awareness of executive functioning disruptions.

Introduction

Irwin and Watt (2007, p. 1) define paranormal experiences as ‘apparent anomalies of behaviour and experience that exist apart from currently known explanatory mechanisms that account for organism-environment and organism–organism information and influence flow’. The study of personal supernatural encounters is important because they represent a relatively common feature of existence (e.g., Tenhaeff, 1972; Schouten, 1994; Dagnall et al., 2016). Furthermore, they can have a profound impact on experiments (Schmied-Knittel and Schetsche, 2005; Laythe et al., 2021). However, despite their importance, paranormal experiences in comparison to beliefs remain relatively under researched. Moreover, investigators have historically focused on only narrow operationalisations, such as subjective paranormal experiences (SPEs), which denote an individual’s readiness to ascribe supernatural causation to an event or occurrence (Glicksohn, 1990).

Nonetheless, SPEs are a useful index because that they recognise the phenomenological significance of explicit individual encounters (Drinkwater et al., 2013) and attributional processes (Irwin et al., 2013; Lange et al., 2019; Laythe et al., 2021). However, SPEs are limited as they focus on singular incidents, whereas analysis of interviews indicates that many supernatural occurrences involve multiple, sustained events and are dispositional in nature. Furthermore, accounts are often vague and uncertain; being defined as strange and unusual and only possibly, paranormal in origin (Drinkwater et al., 2017). In this context, SPEs predominantly index spontaneous, important instances that are externally generated, beyond individual control (i.e., receptive rather than productive) and self-labelled. Thus, SPEs as a unit of measurement, often conflate person-centred (internal) and situational (external) factors. Consequently, they are largely insensitive to perceived possession of paranormal abilities, which belong to the percipient, manifest in a variety of ways and endure over time. Consistent with this delineation, ability-based accounts are often vague, ill-defined and draw on a range of unsubstantiated, anecdotal evidence. For these reasons, ability-based experiences differ qualitatively and quantitatively to regularly reported SPEs (i.e., ghosts/hauntings and extrasensory perception).

Noting these factors, Drinkwater et al. (2021a,b) used self-professed paranormal ability as an index of experience. Although researchers have under investigated the role of facility, measurement instruments have historically recognised the importance and independence of ability, belief and experience. Notable examples being the Anomalous Experiences Inventory (AEI; Gallagher et al., 1994), the Multivariable Multiaxial Suggestibility Inventory-2 (MMSI-2; Escolà-Gascón, 2020a; Escolà-Gascón et al., 2021) and the Australian Sheep Goat Scale (ASGS; Thalbourne and Delin, 1993). The AEI measures Anomalous/Paranormal Experiences alongside Beliefs, Fear and Ability. The MMSI-2 examines Anomalous Perceived Phenomena (APP) in conjunction with 12 cognitive and personality scales, which include belief in ‘psychic abilities’. The ASGS, which remains a widely used tool for assessing paranormal credence, includes items assessing ability (e.g., ‘I believe I have marked psychokinetic ability’), experience (e.g., ‘I believe I have had at least one experience of telepathy between myself and another person’) and belief (e.g., ‘I believe in the existence of psychokinesis’).

The inclusion of ability alongside experience and belief within the AEI and the ASGS reflects that facility is a core component of paranormal ideation that merits consideration. In support of this supposition, examination of the constructs reveals that while they overlap, they are discrete and discernible. Conceptually, ability denotes perceived possession and enactment of paranormal powers, experience refers to the ascription of paranormality to an event or occurrence, and belief is faith in the existence of supernatural forces and/or abilities. Thus, although individuals often predicate their alleged ability on possible experiences, ability and experience are different constructs. For instance, correlations between AEI subscales reveal only medium-large associations (Drinkwater et al., 2021a). Moreover, ability (vs. experience and belief) is differentially associated with other variables. For example, ability does not correlate with neuroticism and general sensation seeking, whereas experience and belief are positively related (Gallagher et al., 1994).

The advantage of an ability-based measure is that it recognises that some individuals believe they possess enduring supernatural faculties. Moreover, studies have also reported associations between self-professed paranormal powers and cognitive-perceptual characteristics. For instance, Parra and Carlos Argibay (2012) found that alleged psychics (vs. controls) scored higher on dissociation, absorption and fantasy proneness. Building on this work, Drinkwater et al. (2021a) investigated whether scores on cognitive-perceptual factors related to subclinical delusion formation and thinking style differed as a function of self-professed paranormal ability. They found that, compared with no and self-professed ability conditions, paranormal practitioners (i.e., Mediums, Psychics, Spiritualists and Fortune-Tellers) scored higher on proneness to reality testing deficits, emotion-based reasoning and paranormal belief. Analysis revealed similar differences between the self-professed and no ability conditions. These outcomes indicated that variations in self-perceived supernatural powers were associated with differences in cognitive-perceptual style.

This notion is commensurate with research that reports individuals scoring high on cognitive-perceptual factors allied to belief in the paranormal demonstrate subtle impairments in executive function, working memory and attention (Noguchi et al., 2008). Particularly, studies using non-clinical student populations, note inverse relationships between schizotypy and neurocognitive functioning (e.g., Jahshan and Sergi, 2007). Functions include executive functioning as assessed by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (i.e., abstract reasoning ability and capacity to shift cognitive strategies in response to changing external demands/rule changes; Daneluzzo et al., 1998), spatial working memory (Park and McTigue, 1997) and sustained attention (Gooding et al., 2006).

Advancing this work, the present paper examined relationships between self-professed paranormal ability and executive functions. Executive functions broadly denote cognitive processes that comprise top-down control (Burgess and Simons, 2005; Diamond, 2013). These include short-term storage and active manipulation of information within current attentional focus (working memory), selection of specific information amidst other data for subsequent processing (interference control), self-control and resistance to acting on impulse (inhibition) and processing that provides a basis for mentation and behaviour outside pre-established frameworks (Diamond, 2013).

Although previous work has explored paranormal belief and executive functions (e.g., Wain and Spinella, 2007), few studies have considered experience, especially self-professed ability. Delorme et al. (2013) conducted a relevant study with mediums. In one experiment, the investigators measured physiological responses during a psychic reading. Analyses revealed one medium displayed a decrease in prefrontal midline theta waves when making accurate responses (i.e., corresponding to the individual; Delorme et al., 2013). Functionally, this may have resulted from a decrease in executive or working memory processing, which was concomitant with the medium entering a ‘receptive mental state’, where the transfer of anomalous information could occur. This elucidation is speculative because it derived from one medium. An alternative explanation is that executive control processes facilitate the voluntary capacity to enter trance-like states that requires intentional action that manifests as prefrontal activations (e.g., Mainieri et al., 2017). In support of the supposition that activation of executive functions is associated with sense of control, Escolà-Gascón (2020b) conducted an experiment with purported mediums. In relation to success on experimental tests of anomalous information reception, the investigation reported a negative correlation between altered states of consciousness and suggestion.

Additionally, work by Delorme et al. (2018) tested mediums on a task requiring dead or alive decisions for unknown faces. Some mediums performed above chance, which was associated with right hemisphere parieto-occipital activations. However, this activity was more likely associated with attentional modulation than executive functioning. Other research using fMRI has discovered activations in sensory cortical regions during mediumistic trance-like states (Mainieri et al., 2017). These related to mediums reporting vivid visual and auditory spirits. However, the researchers also observed neural changes during imagined (non-mediumistic) trance states. Thus, alterations did not necessarily distinguish anomalous from non-anomalous states. Mainieri et al. (2017) also found frontal activations in mediumistic trance-like states. These were primarily in orbitofrontal regions rather than those more directly allied to executive functioning and likely reflected processes promoting sensory integration and evaluation.

An alternative approach to considering the role of executive functioning in anomalous states has used individuals who claim to have experienced spirit possession. Although the existence of different types of spirit possession (Bourguignon, 1976) complicates evaluation of cognitive-neural substrates, the distinction between intermittent and ‘transitory dissociative’ is useful in this context (Al-Adawi et al., 2001). The former denotes instances where a spirit has taken over an individual, whereas the latter refers to a brief state attendant with stressful events. Al-Adawi et al. (2001) found that the intermittent group (vs. transitory) scored lower on executive functioning (i.e., Wisconsin card sorting test, verbal fluency, trail making and the tower of London test). These finding contradict previously outlined work with mediums, which concluded that changes in neural activity signified increased executive control. This discrepancy may arise from the involuntary nature of spirit possession compared to the control mediums exercise over their trance-like states. Other work has shown that non-psychotic individuals, who claim anomalistic experiences, demonstrate lower performance on tests of executive function compared to controls (Powers et al., 2017).

Present Study

This study extended preceding research by exploring whether scores on self-report measures of executive functioning varied as a function of self-professed paranormal ability. To allow comparisons with prior investigations on cognitive-perceptual factors, the present study used the same range of paranormal practitioners (i.e., Mediums, Psychics, Spiritualists and Fortune-Tellers) and categories (i.e., no ability vs. self-professed ability vs. practitioners) as Drinkwater et al. (2021a). Consistent with the outcomes of Drinkwater et al. (2021a), the researchers hypothesised that paranormal practitioners would score higher on paranormal belief and report greater levels of executive functioning disruption than the ability group, who correspondingly would score higher on measures than the no ability condition.

Materials and Methods

Respondents

Four hundred and ninety-nine respondents participated (Mean age, M) = 40.33 years, SD = 16.94, range 22–87. In terms of gender, 219 respondents were males (44%), M = 41.06 years, SD = 18.02, range 18–87; and 279 were females (56%), M = 39.83 years, SD = 16.05, range 18–75. To ensure there was a range of ability within sample data the investigators recruited the sample in two phases. The first targeted contacts acquired through the Paranormal Society and research projects. Since this convenience sample yielded only 152 respondents. The second used Bilendi, a social research company that provides participant panels for research and marketing. Bilendi samples have previously featured in published studies and reports (e.g., Lippke et al., 2021; van Schalkwyk et al., 2021). Accordingly, academics acknowledge the organisation as a source of quality samples. Moreover, panel data generally is comparable to that collected via traditional means in terms of reach and diversity (Kees et al., 2017). The researchers requested a Unite Kingdom-based representative sample comprising respondents aged 18 years and over.

Measures

Self-Professed Paranormal Ability

Items assessed whether respondents believed that they possessed paranormal abilities (i.e., Spiritualist, Psychic, Medium and Fortune-Teller) and determined if they were practicing practitioners. The selected domains characterised primary, receptive components of paranormality (Dagnall et al., 2010c, 2011). To ensure that answers represented conceptual classifications, a clear explanation prefaced each ability (e.g., ‘Psychics perceive energy left behind from people who have died’). Respondents indicated the degree to which they believed they possessed facilities on a scale ranging 0 (no) to 100 (certain) and whether they were paranormal practitioners using a dichotomous ‘Yes/No’ option.

Executive Functions

Self-report instruments assessed a range of executive functions (i.e., general, memory and decision-making; see Drinkwater et al., Under Review).

Webexec

The Webexec is a 6-item instrument that measures general executive functioning problems. The scale was designed for Internet-mediated research and has featured in published work (e.g., Friedman-Krauss et al., 2014). Items index a range of executive functions including sustaining focus, concentration, multitasking, maintaining a train of thought, task completion and impulsivity. Within the Webexec, items appear as questions (‘Do you find yourself acting on “impulse?” ’). Respondents indicate answers on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (no problems) to 4 (a great many problems). Summation of scores produces a total between 6 and 24; higher scores signify greater experience of executive functioning problems. The Webexec has established psychometric properties (i.e., satisfactory content validity and internal reliability; Buchanan et al., 2010).

Everyday Memory Questionnaire-Revised

The Everyday Memory Questionnaire-Revised (EMQ-R) consists of 13 items that index subjective memory failure in everyday life. Items present as behaviours (e.g., ‘Getting the details of what someone was told you mixed up and confused’) and respondents specify how frequently content has happened over the past month on a 5-point Likert scale. Scores range from 0 (once or less) to 4 (once or more in a day). The scale includes three subscales measuring Retrieval (memory failure), Attention Shifting (focus loss) and Factor 3 (visual reconstruction). The EMQ-R has good psychometric integrity (Royle and Lincoln, 2008).

The Working Memory Questionnaire

The Working Memory Questionnaire (WMQ) is a 30-item instrument that assesses working memory functioning. Scale items reference three facets: short-term storage (retention over a brief period), att43wention (mental slowness/fatigue, dual tasking and distractibility) and executive function (decision-making, planning and shifting). The WMQ presents items in the form of questions (e.g., ‘Do you feel that you tire quickly during the day?’) and respondents answer using a five-point Likert scale, which ranges from 0 (no problem at all) to 4 (very severe problem). Totalling of items produces a score between 0 and 120; higher scores reflect greater levels of working memory difficulties. The WMQ is an established, validated, robust psychometric instrument (Vallat-Azouvi et al., 2012).

Decision Making Questionnaire

The Control (e.g., ‘Do you enjoy making decisions?’) and Instinctiveness (e.g., ‘Do you rely on “gut feelings” when making decisions?’) subscales of the Decision Making Questionnaire (DMQ) evaluated decision-making efficacy (French et al., 1993). In combination, these dimensions assess impulsiveness (impetuosity), an important aspect of executive functioning. DMQ items appear as questions, and respondents answer via a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Very infrequently or never) to 6 (Very frequently or always). French et al. (1993) validated the DMQ, and the instrument has featured in academic research (Douse and McManus, 1993; Kumar and Gupta, 2017).

Other Measures

Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; 10-items) assesses trait emotion regulation strategies. It comprises two subscales: cognitive reappraisal (e.g., ‘When I want to feel more positive emotion, I change the way I’m thinking about the situation’) and expressive suppression (e.g., ‘I keep my emotions to myself’). Respondents indicate the extent to which they agree with statements using a seven-point Likert scale 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The ERQ possesses robust psychometric properties (Gross and John, 2003).

Belief in the Paranormal

Revised Paranormal Belief Scale

The Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS) is a 26-item instrument that assesses the extent to which individuals endorse the existence of paranormal phenomena (Tobacyk, 2004). Since its development, the scale has become the predominate measure of paranormal belief. Item content appears as statements. Participants respond via a seven-point Likert scale by selecting a choice ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree; Lange et al., 2000). Summation of items produces a score ranging from 0 to 156. Higher scores denote greater levels of paranormal belief. The RPBS possesses well-established measurement properties (i.e., validity and reliability; Drinkwater et al., 2017).

Procedure

Respondents accessed the Participant Information Sheet (PIS) via a web link. The PIS outlined the study background then asked respondents to provide informed consent. Accepting participants then progressed to the instructions. These requested that respondents carefully read and attempt all items, advance through sections in their own time and answer openly and honestly. The study materials comprised sections on demographics (i.e., age and preferred gender), paranormal ability, executive functioning and belief in the paranormal. At the conclusion of materials, respondents were debriefed.

To address potential data contaminating factors the researchers employed procedural devices. Firstly, to prevent order effects, section and scale presentation rotated across participants. Secondly, to negate the influence of social desirability the instructions stated that there were no correct responses. Finally, to counter common method variance (CMV; Spector, 2019), scale instructions differentiated between constructs by emphasising scale uniqueness (Podsakoff et al., 2003). This was important as the study used a cross-sectional design. Data collected at one point in time is susceptible to CMV because measure proximity can inflate perceived relationships between constructs under observation.

Ethics Statement

The Faculty of Health, Psychology and Social Care Ethics Committee at Manchester Metropolitan University (October 2018; Project ID, 954) provided approval for a programme of study investigating factors associated with self-professed psychic ability/mediumship.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Respondents were categorised into three groups based on level of self-professed ability (no ability vs. ability vs. practising). Practitioner numbers were relatively similar across specialism (Mediumship, Psychic, Spiritualist and Fortune-Tellers; Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Frequencies (and percentages in brackets) of professed ability and practitioner groups.

Practitioners (vs. non-practising) expressed greater confidence about their self-professed paranormal abilities. However, since practitioners frequently reported multiple abilities/services it was not possible to compare differences. In terms of practitioner services, n = 34, 35% offered 1; n = 28, 29% offered 2; n = 22, 23% offered 3; and n = 12, 13% offered 4. Due to this overlap, practitioner services were combined to produce an overall group (n = 96, 19%). Analysis then explored difference between practitioners (n = 96, 19%), self-professed abilities (n = 197, 40%) and respondents declaring no abilities (n = 206, 41%). Descriptive information for paranormal belief and measures of executive functioning appears in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reliability, means and standard deviations for paranormal belief and neuropsychological measures as a function of ability.

Ability Group Comparisons

Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) assessed whether ability scores (no ability, self-professed ability and practitioners) differed significantly on study measures (i.e., paranormal belief, general executive function, working and everyday memory, decision-making and emotion regulation). Since Drinkwater et al. (Under review) observed experience-based differences on EMQ-R (Retrieval, Attention Tracking and Factor 3) and ERQ (Expressive Suppression and Cognitive Reappraisal) subscales were included within the analysis.

Prior to MANOVA, data screening occurred. This indicated that all values were acceptable; fell within the range of −2 to +2 (Byrne, 2010). Data points represent outliers if they possess a studentised residual >4 and a Cook’s distance >4/(n − k − 1), where n represents the sample size and k denotes the number of independent variables. Using these criteria, no outliers arose. However, Box’s test (i.e., assessment of homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices) was significant (244.54, p < 0.001). Accordingly, interpretation used Pillai’s criterion instead of Wilk’s lambda because it is a more robust index.

MANOVA produced a significant overall effect, Pillai’s criterion = 0.54, F (18, 978) = 20.13, p < 0.001. A large effect size occurred (η2 = 0.27) indicating differences in measures across ability groups. Univariate analyses indicated significant differences on all variables apart from Cognitive Reappraisal (Table 3). Small to medium effect sizes existed for all outcomes except paranormal belief (large effect).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and pairwise comparison summary for outcome variables.

Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni) revealed that Paranormal Belief increased as a function of level of ability; practitioners scored higher than self-professed, who scored higher than no ability (Table 3). For General Executive Functioning, Working Memory and Everyday Memory significant differences emerged for the no ability vs. ability and the no ability vs. practising comparisons. There were no differences between the ability conditions (ability vs. practising). For Decision Making and Expressive Suppression, analysis revealed significant differences between no ability vs. practising and ability vs. practising groups. Further contrast analysis (Table 4) indicated that for all measures, except for Decision Making and Cognitive Reappraisal, ability groups (self-professed ability and practitioners) reported greater executive functioning problems.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Contrasts between no ability, ability and practising groups in relation to executive functioning.

Discussion

Analysis revealed ability-related differences on several of the measures used in this study. Generally, perceived ability was associated with greater reported levels of Paranormal Belief and executive function disruption. However, practitioners (vs. self-professed) only scored higher on Paranormal Belief, Decision Making and Expressive Suppression. Collectively, these outcomes indicated that perception of ability, regardless of intensity of paranormal conviction, influenced subjective appraisal of executive functions. The failure to find consistent differences between the declared ability groups on executive function contrasted with Drinkwater et al. (2021a), who observed ability-related variations on cognitive-perceptual measures (proneness to reality testing deficits and emotion-based reasoning).

In the present study, the failure to find consistent differences between practitioners and those with self-professed ability is difficult to explain. The practitioners scored higher on Paranormal Belief and reported greater confidence in their alleged abilities. Commensurate with Drinkwater et al. (2021a), this suggests that the two groups differ in their perceptions of the paranormal. One possibility, based on the observed outcomes, is that discernment of ability was sufficient to heighten awareness of executive functioning disruptions. Tentatively, this could be an artefact of the scales used, which focus on degree of perceived disruption. In this context, the important distinction is between presence/absence of disruption rather than extent. The latter classification is likely lacking in discriminatory power because it is sensitive to subjective variations in ratings. Additionally, although no significant difference in executive functioning was found between the practitioners and self-professed group, the numerical difference was in the expected direction. Specifically, it was higher for the practitioner group. Given that the magnitude of the disparity in Paranormal Belief scores was smaller between these groups and the corresponding no ability group, then it is possible that the variation in Paranormal Belief was insufficient to detect any real underlying differences.

While the current paper was exploratory in nature, it is important to recognise limitations. One issue was the use of self-report measures. These rely upon the introspection of executive functioning and accordingly are susceptible to interpretative bias. A related concern with metacognitive assessments of processes is that higher-order functions may not be fully accessible to consciousness (Dagnall et al., 2008; Chan et al., 2015).

Concomitantly, heightened awareness of executive function disturbance may reflect disparities in cognitive focus rather than actual impairment. There is substantial evidence to support this notion. Specifically, both experience and belief in the paranormal correlate positively with variables associated with unusual perceptions and ideations (i.e., proneness to reality testing deficits, Dagnall et al., 2018; and schizotypy, Dagnall et al., 2010b). This coincides with a preference for intuitive, intra-psychic data (Dagnall et al., 2010a; Drinkwater et al., 2012). Hence, perceived executive function disruption in respondents professing paranormal abilities could merely reflect an internalised focus.

Additionally, collecting data at one point in time is potentially problematic because evaluations are recall based and susceptible to deviation as a function of accessibility, availability and context (e.g., Schwarz, 2007). Hence, subsequent studies should establish the veracity of intra-respondent judgments by taking multiple assessments at different time points. Although these may still not accord with performance on objective measures, compound ratings will provide a more robust index of individual subjective ratings. Moreover, researchers could compare these with proxy ratings. In the related area of prospective memory, proxy (vs. self-ratings) have proved invaluable since they often correlate more strongly with objective assessments (Arnold and Bayen, 2019). Nonetheless, to ensure that present outcomes accord with actual performance it is essential that subsequent work cross-tabulates rating-based work with objective measures (Buchanan et al., 2010). This is essential to collaborate the tentative findings of this study. Moreover, testing should also include a broader range of executive functions. In addition, although this study included explicit efforts to encourage response accuracy among the sample, the implementation of attention check items may have limited potential response error to a greater degree.

Finally, the present study used a narrow range of abilities and practitioners (Drinkwater et al., 2021a for a detailed discussion). While professions were commensurate with core paranormal domains, psychic occurrences and communication with the dead, these represented only a subsample of practitioners in areas allied to scientifically unsubstantiated beliefs. Additionally, asking practitioners to label their abilities is problematic because individuals often provide a range of overlapping but distinct services. In this context, future studies could use wider inclusive categories, based on alleged ability rather than service provided (i.e., productive vs. receptive), and assess a broader range of characteristics. The present study used confidence, which represents only one element that contributes to self-perception of capacity. Consideration of the general literature on perceived ability identifies other important factors, such as control, efficacy, motivation, success and commitment, that merit consideration. This is necessary since developing a fuller understanding of the psychological factors associated with self-perceived ability will inform subsequent studies and help to identify potential variables that influence high-order cognitive processes.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Manchester Metropolitan University Faculty of Health, Psychology and Social Care Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KD and ND provided the theoretical focus, developed content, and produced the initial article. KD was responsible for data collection and measurement selection. AD performed data analysis and wrote up the results. ÁE-G contributed further analytical advice and support. AP offered additional input on executive functions. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the BIAL Foundation (Project Number 082/2018/G-07745). The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

AD was employed by Adelphi Values Ltd.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the BIAL Foundation for their support with this research.

References

Al-Adawi, S. H., Martin, R. G., Al-Salmi, A., and Ghassani, H. (2001). Zar: group distress and healing. Ment. Health Relig. Cult. 4, 47–61. doi: 10.1080/13674670124303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, N. R., and Bayen, U. J. (2019). Prospective memory: comparing self-and proxy-reports with cognitive modeling of task performance. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 8, 244–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourguignon, E. (1976). Possession. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.

Google Scholar

Buchanan, T., Heffernan, T. M., Parrott, A. C., Ling, J., Rodgers, J., and Scholey, A. B. (2010). A short self-report measure of problems with executive function suitable for administration via the internet. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 709–714. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, P. W., and Simons, J. S. (2005). “Theories of frontal lobe executive function: clinical applications,” in Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits. eds. P. W. Halligan and D. T. Wade (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 211–231.

Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural Equation Modeling With AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Chan, C. C., Spencer, C. C., West, C., Viegas, C., and Bedwell, J. S. (2015). Metacognitive processes in psychometrically defined schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. 230, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Parker, A., Drinkwater, K., and Walsh, R. S. (2018). Confirmatory factor analysis of the inventory of personality organization-reality testing subscale. Front. Psychol. 9:1116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., and Parker, A. (2011). Alien visitation, extra-terrestrial life, and paranormal beliefs. J. Sci. Explor. 25, 699–720.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Clough, P. (2016). Paranormal experience, belief in the paranormal and anomalous beliefs. Front. Psychol. 7:1045. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Munley, G. (2010a). Reality testing, belief in the paranormal, and urban legends. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 25, 25–55.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Munley, G., and Parker, A. (2008). Memory aberrations, transliminality, and delusional Ideation. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 67–75. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.1.67-75

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Munley, G., Parker, A., and Drinkwater, K. (2010b). Paranormal belief, schizotypy, and transliminality. J. Parapsychol. 74, 117–143.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Parker, A., Munley, G., and Drinkwater, K. (2010c). Common paranormal belief dimensions. J. Sci. Explor. 24, 477–494.

Google Scholar

Daneluzzo, E., Bustini, M., Stratta, P., Casacchia, M., and Rossi, A. (1998). Schizotypal personality questionnaire and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a population of DSM-III-R schizophrenic patients and control subjects. Compr. Psychiatry 39, 143–148. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(98)90073-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., Beischel, J., Michel, L., Boccuzzi, M., Radin, D., and Mills, P. (2013). Electrocortical activity associated with subjective communication with the deceased. Front. Psychol. 4:834. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., Pierce, A., Michel, L., and Radin, D. (2018). Intuitive assessment of mortality based on facial characteristics: behavioral, electrocortical, and machine learning analyses. Explore 14, 262–267. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2017.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douse, N. A., and McManus, I. C. (1993). The personality of fantasy game players. Br. J. Psychol. 84, 505–509. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1993.tb02498.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Dagnall, N., and Bate, L. (2013). Into the unknown: using interpretative phenomenological analysis to explore personal accounts of paranormal experiences. J. Parapsychol. 77, 281–294.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., and Williams, C. (2021a). Differences in cognitive-perceptual factors arising from variations in self-professed paranormal ability. Front. Psychol. 12:2258. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., and Williams, C. (2021b). Paranormal belief, thinking style and delusion formation: a latent profile analysis of within-individual variations in experience-based paranormal facets. Front. Psychol. 12:670959. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.670959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., Grogan, S., and Riley, V. (2017). Understanding the unknown: A thematic analysis of subjective paranormal experiences. Aust. J. Parapsychol. 17, 23–46.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., and Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theories, and paranormal beliefs. J. Parapsychol. 76, 57–77.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., and Parker, A. (2017). An assessment of the dimensionality and factorial structure of the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. Front. Psychol. 8:1693. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolà-Gascón, Á. (2020b). Forced-choice experiment on anomalous information reception and correlations with states of consciousness using the Multivariable Multiaxial Suggestibility Inventory-2 (MMSI-2). Explore. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2020.11.009 [Epub ahead of print]

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolà-Gascón, Á. (2020a). Researching unexplained phenomena II: new evidences for anomalous experiences supported by the multivariable multiaxial suggestibility Inventory-2 (MMSI-2). Curr. Res. Behav. Sci. 1:100005. doi: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2020.100005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolà-Gascón, Á., Dagnall, N., and Gallifa, J. (2021). The Multivariable Multiaxial Suggestibility Inventory-2 (MMSI-2): a psychometric alternative to measure and explain supernatural experiences. Front. Psychol. 12:692194. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.692194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French, D. J., West, R. J., Elander, J., and Wilding, J. M. (1993). Decision-making style, driving style, and self-reported involvement in road traffic accidents. Ergonomics 36, 627–644. doi: 10.1080/00140139308967925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman-Krauss, A. H., Raver, C. C., Neuspiel, J. M., and Kinsel, J. (2014). Child behavior problems, teacher executive functions, and teacher stress in head start classrooms. Early Educ. Dev. 25, 681–702. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.825190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, C., Kumar, V. K., and Pekala, R. J. (1994). The anomalous experiences inventory: reliability and validity. J. Parapsychol. 58, 402–428.

Google Scholar

Glicksohn, J. (1990). Belief in the paranormal and subjective paranormal experience. Personal. Individ. Differ. 11, 675–683. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90252-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooding, D. C., Matts, C. W., and Rollmann, E. A. (2006). Sustained attention deficits in relation to psychometrically identified schizotypy: evaluating a potential endophenotypic marker. Schizophr. Res. 82, 27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, H. J., Dagnall, N., and Drinkwater, K. (2013). Parapsychological experience as anomalous experience plus paranormal attribution: a questionnaire based on a new approach to measurement. J. Parapsychol. 77, 39–53. doi: 10.1037/t31377-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, H. J., and Watt, C. A. (2007). An Introduction to Parapsychology. 5th Edn. Jefferson, NC: McFarland.

Google Scholar

Jahshan, C. S., and Sergi, M. J. (2007). Theory of mind, neurocognition, and functional status in schizotypy. Schizophr. Res. 89, 278–286. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kees, J., Berry, C., Burton, S., and Sheehan, K. (2017). An analysis of data quality: professional panels, student subject pools, and Amazon’s mechanical Turk. J. Advert. 46, 141–155. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2016.1269304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, K., and Gupta, P. (2017). Impact of age and decision making on false memory. J. Psychosoc. Res. 12, 381–389.

Google Scholar

Lange, R., Irwin, H. J., and Houran, J. (2000). Top-down purification of Tobacyk’s Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. Personal. Individ. Differ. 29, 131–156. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00183-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, R., Ross, R. M., Dagnall, N., Irwin, H. J., Houran, J., and Drinkwater, K. (2019). Anomalous experiences and paranormal attributions: psychometric challenges in studying their measurement and relationship. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 6, 346–358. doi: 10.1037/cns0000187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laythe, B., Houran, J., Dagnall, N., and Drinkwater, K. (2021). Conceptual and clinical implications of a “haunted people syndrome”. Spiritual. Clin. Pract. 8, 195–214. doi: 10.1037/scp0000251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippke, S., Fischer, M. A., and Ratz, T. (2021). Physical activity, loneliness, and meaning of friendship in young individuals–a mixed-methods investigation prior to and during the COVID-19 pandemic with three cross-sectional studies. Front. Psychol. 12:146. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.617267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mainieri, A. G., Peres, J. F. P., Moreira-Almeida, A., Mathiak, K., Habel, U., and Kohn, N. (2017). Neural correlates of psychotic-like experiences during spiritual-trance state. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 266, 101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noguchi, H., Hori, H., and Kunugi, H. (2008). Schizotypal traits and cognitive function in healthy adults. Psychiatry Res. 161, 162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., and McTigue, K. (1997). Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophr. Res. 26, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00051-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parra, A., and Carlos Argibay, J. (2012). Dissociation, absorption, fantasy proneness and sensation-seeking in psychic claimants. J. Soc. Psych. Res. 76, 193–203.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, A. R. III, Kelley, M. S., and Corlett, P. R. (2017). Varieties of voice-hearing: psychics and the psychosis continuum. Schizophr. Bull. 43, 84–98. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royle, J., and Lincoln, N. B. (2008). The Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised: development of a 13-item scale. Disabil. Rehabil. 30, 114–121. doi: 10.1080/09638280701223876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmied-Knittel, I., and Schetsche, M. (2005). Everyday miracles: results of a representative survey in Germany. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 20, 3–21.

Google Scholar

Schouten, S. A. (1994). An overview of quantitatively evaluated studies with mediums and psychics. J. Am. Soc. Psych. Res. 88, 221–254.

Google Scholar

Schwarz, N. (2007). “Retrospective and concurrent self-reports: The rationale for real-time data capture,” in The Science of Real-Time Data Capture: Self-Reports in Health Research. eds. A. A. Stone, S. S. Shiffman, A. Atienza, and L. Nebeling (New York: Oxford University Press), 11–26.

Google Scholar

Spector, P. E. (2019). Do not cross me: optimizing the use of cross-sectional designs. J. Bus. Psychol. 34, 125–137. doi: 10.1007/s10869-018-09613-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenhaeff, W. H. C. (1972). Telepathy and Clairvoyance: Views of some Little Investigated Capabilities of Man. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Pub Limited.

Google Scholar

Thalbourne, M., and Delin, P. (1993). A new instrument for measuring sheep-goat variable: its psychometric properties and factor structure. J. Soc. Psych. Res. 59, 172–186.

Google Scholar

Tobacyk, J. J. (2004). A Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. Int. J. Transpers. Stud. 23, 94–98. doi: 10.24972/ijts.2004.23.1.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallat-Azouvi, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., and Azouvi, P. (2012). The Working Memory Questionnaire: a scale to assess everyday life problems related to deficits of working memory in brain injured patients. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 22, 634–649. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2012.681110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Schalkwyk, M. C., Maani, N., McKee, M., Thomas, S., Knai, C., and Petticrew, M. (2021). “When the fun stops, stop”: an analysis of the provenance, framing and evidence of a ‘responsible gambling’ campaign. PLoS One 16:e0255145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wain, O., and Spinella, M. (2007). Executive functions in morality, religion, and paranormal beliefs. Int. J. Neurosci. 117, 135–146. doi: 10.1080/00207450500534068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: paranormal ability, paranormal belief, executive functions, self-report measures, multivariate analysis of variance

Citation: Drinkwater KG, Dagnall N, Denovan A, Parker A and Escolà-Gascón & (2021) Executive Functioning: Assessing the Role of Perceived Paranormal Ability. Front. Psychol. 12:798283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.798283

Received: 19 October 2021; Accepted: 30 November 2021;
Published: 23 December 2021.

Edited by:

Luca Simione, Italian National Research Council, Italy

Reviewed by:

Alejandro Parra, Universidad Abierta Interamericana, Argentina
Ahmad Sohrabi, University of Kurdistan, Iran

Copyright © 2021 Drinkwater, Dagnall, Denovan, Parker and Escolà-Gascón. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Neil Dagnall, n.dagnall@mmu.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.