- 1Department of Psychology, Teachers’ College, Beijing Union University, Beijing, China
- 2Learning and Psychological Development Institution for Children and Adolescents, Beijing Union University, Beijing, China
- 3Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States
- 4School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- 5Department of Psychiatry, The Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
The study investigated the associations between children’s self-reported habitual sleep disturbance and multidimensional executive function (EF). Two hundred and four 7–9-year-old typically developing children completed the Sleep Self-Report and finished the Red-Blue Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and Backward Digit Span Test, indexing different EF components including inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. Results revealed that all the three EF components were significantly correlated with sleep. However, cognitive flexibility was no longer significantly related to sleep when the other EF components – inhibitory control and working memory – were controlled for. Meanwhile, inhibitory control, as well as working memory, was still significantly related to sleep after controlling for the other EF components. Results suggest that children’s self-reported sleep might be associated directly with inhibitory control and working memory, but indirectly with cognitive flexibility.
Introduction
People spend approximately one-third of their lives asleep. Sleep is essential to healthy development across physical, psychosocial, emotional, and cognitive domains (Sadeh, 2007; Ednick et al., 2009; Dewald et al., 2010; Beebe, 2011; Wang and Yip, 2020). A burgeoning body of research has examined how sleep problems affect cognition (Astill et al., 2012; Shochat et al., 2014; Hoyniak et al., 2019). Theoretically, cognition is a multiple, complex omnibus of psychological abilities (Killgore, 2010), and research has examined the association between sleep and diverse cognitive components including attention, memory, processing speed, and complex reasoning (Lim and Dinges, 2010; Astill et al., 2012). Studies delivered mixed results, with parts of studies revealing the link between sleep and some cognitive abilities, while other studies showing no association in some cognitive tasks (Lim and Dinges, 2010; de Bruin et al., 2017). Such inconsistencies might be attributed to heterogeneous measurement of sleep and cognition, as well as diverse samples from infant to adults, from healthy to atypical populations.
One of the basic and core component of cognitive abilities is executive function (EF). EF refers to a set of fundamental, general mechanisms, and processes, often linked to the prefront cortex of brain, that control, direct, or coordinate cognition and action (Miyake et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2013). EF not only is an essential part for cognition but also works importantly for mental health, school performance, and life success (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Diamond, 2013). It is often postulated in the literature that EF has three elementary components: inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility (also called cognitive shifting; Miyake et al., 2000; Garon et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2013). Each component can be measured by separate tests such as the Stroop test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and digit span backwards test (Miyake et al., 2000; Garon et al., 2008). Research has revealed that the three components were moderately associated with each other but clearly separable (Garon et al., 2008).
As an important domain of cognition, EF has received increasing attention by studies linking sleep to cognition. However, inconsistent findings were reported concerning whether and how sleep was associated with EF (Sadeh et al., 2003; Bourke et al., 2011). After reviewing the relevant literature, we summarized some common aspects as below.
First, most studies examined sleep and multiple types of cognitive abilities including EF (Lim and Dinges, 2010; Astill et al., 2012). In these studies, EF was considered as a sub-domain of the grand cognition, and hence usually only one or two EF tasks were used (Bernier et al., 2013; Sadeh et al., 2015; Vermeulen et al., 2016). Although it is often postulated that there are three different components in EF, few studies have systematically investigated the relationship between sleep and all the components of EF (Karpinski et al., 2008).
Second, studies have often employed experimental manipulation on sleep, such as sleep deprivation/restriction (the most frequently used method; Killgore, 2010; Lim and Dinges, 2010), or bedtime/sleep schedule modification (Sadeh et al., 2003). A meta-analysis of the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitions revealed a moderate effect on working memory (Lim and Dinges, 2010). It is worth noting that such experimental disturbance on sleep was quite acute and drastic, leading to significant impairment of basic arousal and attention and consequently resulting in worse performance on EF tests (Killgore, 2010). Nevertheless, sleep deprivation is not as common as mild or moderate sleep interruption in real-life environment. We therefore can justifiably doubt whether natural and modest disturbance on habitual sleep, such as occasional wake-up at night or daytime sleepiness, has any impact on EF.
Third, another important piece of evidence linking sleep to EF comes from atypically developmental samples. Among these studies, sleep-disordered breathing (SDB), a spectrum of breathing disorders during sleep, from primary snoring to obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) syndrome, was the most frequently assessed sleep index (Beebe et al., 2003; Karpinski et al., 2008; Bourke et al., 2011; Joyce and Dimitriou, 2017). For example, in a study of 3–5-year-old children, the higher parent-reported risk for SDB was associated with impaired EF across the dimensions of inhibitory control, working memory, and planning (Karpinski et al., 2008). In another study of 8–12-year-old children, working memory, a dimension of EF, was found poorer in the OSA group than the control healthy group; poorer working memory was also found associated with sleep-related respiratory index (Lau et al., 2015). Moreover, the findings linking sleep to EF was more likely to be found significant in clinic group such as children with Down syndrome (Joyce and Dimitriou, 2017), mild traumatic brain injury (Landry-Roy et al., 2018), or ADHD (Sciberras et al., 2015). Besides evidence in child samples, a meta-analysis in adult samples also revealed a significant difference in EF between the OSA and control groups (Beebe et al., 2003).
In short of the aforementioned literature, there is evidence showing that drastic sleep disturbance, including sleep deprivation and sleep-disordered breathing syndrome, is linked with worse EF (Beebe et al., 2003; Lim and Dinges, 2010). Yet, scant evidence is available to support the association between mild sleep disturbances and EF in healthy people. Hence, the current study aims to investigate whether individual variation in everyday sleep quality is related to EF using cross-sectional data. In addition, we utilize the multidimensional conceptualization of EF including all the core components of inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory (Diamond, 2013). This is what previous research tends to neglect (Karpinski et al., 2008). Thus, the current study will look into all the three EF components and investigate their respective links with habitual sleep disturbance.
Moreover, the previous research on the relation between sleep and EF has primarily examined populations across a broad age range, including samples from children (Astill et al., 2012) to adults (Lim and Dinges, 2010) and even elders (Sforza et al., 2010; Sabeti et al., 2018; Sattari et al., 2019). As previous studies revealed a moderating effect of age (it was found that the relation between sleep and cognition appeared to be more significant in younger people; Bonnet and Rosa, 1987; Sadeh et al., 2002; Dewald et al., 2010), the current study will focus on children rather than adults, seeking to establish the relation between sleep and EF in the early developmental stage.
Furthermore, children’s sleep can be measured by a variety of standardized instruments (Orgilés et al., 2013), including both objective methods (such as polysomnographic technique and actigraphy) and subjective methods (such as self-reported and parent-reported questionnaires). Usually, the objective methods examine the domains of sleep duration and onset time, whereas the subjective surveys can examine more domains, such as bedtime resistance, sleep anxiety, and day-time sleepiness (Owens et al., 2000a). In order to obtain more information on sleep behavior and disturbance, we used questionnaire to measure children’s sleep in the current study. Moreover, given the fact that research shows the self-reported sleep is more closely related to objective assessment than parent-reported sleep (Orgilés et al., 2013; Holzhausen et al., 2021), and that children tend to identify more sleep disturbances by self-report (e.g., sleep onset delay, night waking-up, and daytime drowsiness) than do their parents (Aronen et al., 2000; Owens et al., 2000b), the current study chose a self-reported scale, Sleep Self-Report (SSR; Owens et al., 2000a,b), which has been employed in different countries around the world (van Litsenburg et al., 2010; Schwerdtle et al., 2012; Orgilés et al., 2013; Steur et al., 2019). Considering the fact that the youngest age adapted for SSR is 7 or 8 years old (Owens et al., 2000b; Orgilés et al., 2013), and that the link of sleep and cognition tends to be more significant at younger ages (Dewald et al., 2010), we chose the early primary school children in the current study.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Children were mainly recruited from primary schools in the urban area of Beijing, China. Consent from teachers and parents of the participants were obtained before our survey officially started. Participants were all Chinese citizens and native Mandarin speakers, and most of them were from middle-class families. Two hundred and four children (103 boys), aged from 7.6 to 9.2 years (M = 8.2 years, SD = 0.3 years), were included in the final analysis. Another 28 children were excluded because they had missing or invalid data on either the EF or sleep test.
Measures
Executive Functions
The EF battery was adapted from tests commonly used in developmental psychology. One classic task was chosen for each of the three EF core elements.
Inhibitory Control
The Red-Blue Test (RBT) was performed to test children’s inhibitory control. This task was adapted from the classic Stroop (Stroop, 1935) and the Day-Night task (Gerstadt et al., 1994; Simpson and Riggs, 2005). In the RBT task, the children were presented with a red or blue geometric figure on computer screen in each trial. They were told that the red color was named as “blue” while the blue color was named as “red” on an alien planet. The children were required to speak out the color of the figure according to the rule on that alien planet. If a correct answer was given within 2 s, the child would get one score for each trial. There were 36 trials in total. Of note, the original version has 16 trials, which is appropriate for preschoolers. Nevertheless, the current study on early primary school children finally conducted 36 instead of 16 trials, for the following justifications: (1) the previous research (Gerstadt et al., 1994) and our pilot study have found that children tend to make more errors in later trials than earlier trials, and (2) the reliability increases with the number of trials in a cognitive ability test (Steinborn et al., 2018). With the increase of trial number, the current test has gained sufficient reliability (split-half r = 0.82, Cronbach α = 0.71).
Cognitive Flexibility
A simplified version of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Nelson, 1976; Anderson et al., 1991) was conducted on computer to measure children’s cognitive flexibility abilities. In the test of the current study, the children were presented with a screen containing figures with three dimensions (color, shape, and quantity; e.g., two red stars). In each trial, there were one response figure on the top and four stimulus figures on the bottom. The child was asked to match the response figure to one of the stimulus figures according to a particular dimension. Immediately after each trial, feedback on whether the choice was correct or not was given to the respondent. Dimension rule changed every six trials (10 in the original version). When the dimension rule changed, the experimenter would verbally give instruction “Now, let us find out the figure with the same COLOR/SHAPE/QUANTITY of the upper one (target).” (No instruction was specified at the moment in original version). There were 42 trials in total. After the first change of dimension, children would gain one score for each correct trial. Thus, the full mark was 36.
Working Memory
The Backward Digit Span Test (Alloway and Alloway, 2010) was performed to test children’s working memory. In the test, the experimenter announced a sequence of numbers two times and the child was required to repeat the numbers in reverse order. The digit length started from two-digit numbers and ended at six-digit numbers. There were three trials for each length. If a child failed in all the three trials of the same length, his/her test terminated. A child would get one score if the correct answer was given within 30 s in each trial. The full score was 15.
Sleep
The Sleep Self-Report (SSR; Owens et al., 2000b) was used to measure children’s sleep disturbance in everyday life. The scale was initially designed to assess sleep habits and problems in children aged 7–12 years old. Twenty-three items were asked to rate on a 3-point scale, ranging from “rarely or never (0–1 time per week),” “sometimes (2–4 times per week),” to “usually (5–7 times per week).” Example items include “Do you wake up at night when your parents think you are asleep?,” “Do you feel rested after a night’s sleep? (Reverse scoring).” The scores of these 23 items were then summed up to reflect sleep problem severity: the higher score, the more disturbed sleep.
The scale scores have been demonstrated with high internal consistencies, with a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.88 in a United States sample (n = 334, 6–11 years old; Owens et al., 2000b) and a McDonald’s omega coefficient of 0.85 in a Spanish sample (n = 1,228, 8–12 years old; Orgilés et al., 2013).
We developed a Chinese version of the SSR by translating the English source SSR template into Chinese and verifying the translation accuracy through back-translation. Another 433 Chinese children (7–12 years old) were recruited to test the psychometric properties of the Chinese version, revealing that the Cronbach’s α was 0.82 and retest-reliability (ICC) was 0.87.
Procedure
The study was approved by the ethics board of the first author’s university. The children who were enrolled into the current research completed the study in their school. For the three EF tasks, tests were administered on a one-to-one basis between the child participant and a research assistant in a quiet room. The test order of three tasks was counterbalanced. For each of the EF tasks, before formal test, the research assistant introduced the rule and performed two checking trials to ensure the child had fully understood the rule. This session took about 20 min for each child.
For the sleep questionnaire, children were tested collectively in groups of 20–40 children. A research assistant read the instructions and questionnaires aloud (paused about 10 s for each item) and answered any questions arising during the test, so as to ensure that the children had understood and were able to respond to all items. This session took about 15 min.
Analyses
First, zero-order correlation analysis and partial correlation analysis with the demographic variables controlled for were conducted to examine whether sleep was associated with each of the three EF components (inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory). Second, further correlation analyses were employed to investigate the associations between each EF component and sleep after controlling for the other two EF components, respectively. Finally, based on the findings of the second step, path analysis modeling was conducted to examine the complex relationship among sleep and the three EF components.
Results
Correlations Between the Sleep and EF
The zero-order correlations between the sleep scores (SSR) and the EF scores (inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory) are presented in Table 1. All the three kinds of EF were significantly related to sleep (for inhibitory control, r = −0.411, p < 0.001; for cognitive flexibility, r = −0.325, p < 0.001; for working memory, r = −0.374, p < 0.001), indicating that the more disturbed sleep was associated with worse EF abilities. The partial correlations after controlling for age and sex, as presented in Table 1, showed that the association between sleep and EF also held.
Results also revealed that the three components of executive functions were moderately correlated with each other, suggesting that they were constructed on a common element.
Correlations Between the Sleep and EF After Controlling for Other EF Components
Given the fact that the different components of EF are associated with each other, sleep might be statistically associated with one EF component (e.g., shifting) in an indirect way via taking effect on another one or two EF components (e.g., inhibitory control or working memory) which are naturally related to the former EF component (e.g., shifting).
Based on the justification above, we conducted further partial correlations between sleep and EF controlling for the other two EF subcomponents as well as sex and age (Table 1). For inhibitory control, its correlation with sleep held after adding into cognitive flexibility and working memory as controlled variables (r = −0.272, p < 0.001). For working memory, after additionally controlling for the inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, its link with sleep was also significant (r = −0.213, p = 0.004). For cognitive flexibility, however, there was no significant association with sleep after additionally controlling for the other two EF subcomponents (r = −0.086, p = 0.227).
Path Analysis Examining the Complex Relationship Between Sleep and Different Components of EF
As we found that the association of sleep with cognitive flexibility turned non-significant after controlling for inhibitory control and working memory, we further conducted a path analysis model to investigate the mediation effect of inhibitory control and working memory between sleep and cognitive flexibility. The model was computed by Mplus 7.0 and the method of robust maximum likelihood estimation was chosen. The impacting paths and coefficients are presented in Figure 1. The model testing showed a good model fit: χ2(1) = 1.513, p = 0.219, CFI = 0.996, TLI = 0.979, SRMR = 0.019, RMSEA = 0.050. The indirect effects from sleep to cognitive flexibility via inhibitory control (coefficient = 0.130, SE = 0.032, t = 4.023, p < 0.001) and via working memory (coefficient = 0.125, SE = 0.035, t = 3.607, p < 0.001) were both significant.
Figure 1. Path analysis modeling examining the indirect association between sleep and cognitive flexibility via inhibitory control and working memory; *** p < 0.001.
We also conducted another comparative model by freeing the path parameter from sleep to cognitive flexibility. Results showed that the direct path from sleep to cognitive flexibility was not significant (b = 0.090, SE = 0.171, t = 1.272, p = 0.203) and the indirect effects from sleep disturbance to cognitive flexibility via inhibitory control and via working memory remained significant (ps < 0.001).
Discussion
The current study examined whether and how sleep disturbance in everyday life was related to the different components of executive function in typically developing children. Previous research mainly focused on the effect on EF from sharply disrupted sleep, such as sleep deprivation and chronic sleep breathing disorder (Jones and Harrison, 2001; Beebe et al., 2003; Killgore, 2010; Lim and Dinges, 2010). Among these studies, EF, which actually is a multidimensional concept including three core components of inhibitory control, flexibility, and working memory (Diamond, 2013), was usually regarded as just one of the facets of cognition, and only one or two EF tasks were measured. In our study, the self-reported sleep score focusing on several aspects of habitual sleep, such as inadequacy of sleep, nocturnal disturbance, and day-time sleepiness, was found to be correlated to scores of all the three EF components. We further found that, while the other two EF components – inhibitory control and working memory – were controlled for, cognitive flexibility was no longer significantly related to sleep. Meanwhile, the partial correlation between sleep and inhibitory control after controlling for working memory and cognitive flexibility, as well as the one between sleep and working memory after controlling for inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, was still significant. Results suggest that self-reported everyday sleep disturbance in children might be directly associated with working memory and inhibitory control, but only indirectly linked to cognitive flexibility.
Previous studies have yielded inconsistent results on whether sleep disturbance is associated with compromised EF in children (Killgore, 2010). After comparing these studies, we found that the different aspects of sleep measured by different methods might account for the discrepancy. Specifically, positive results were mostly provided by studies using questionnaire to measure multiple aspects of sleep, while negative results were mainly from studies using actigraphy or sleep dairy to measure sleep duration or efficiency (the ratio of minutes asleep to minutes in bed). For example, a significant association was found between scores of Sleep Self-Report questionnaire and working memory of 5–13-year-old children (Sciberras et al., 2015), but not found between those of sleep duration (or sleep efficiency) and working memory of 7–13-year-old children (Sadeh et al., 2002). The heterogeneous result patterns of subjective multiple sleep questionnaire and objective sleep duration can also be identified in the same study. For example, in a study of 8–9-year-old children, working memory was found associated with sleep scores from questionnaire (School Sleep Habits Survey) but not to sleep duration from actigraphy (Buckhalt et al., 2007). Similarly in an adolescent study, the correlation between sleep scores computed from Epworth Sleepiness Scale and EF scores was found significant, while sleep duration provided by actigraphy was not significantly associated with EF outcomes (Anderson et al., 2009). It was argued that sleep questionnaire and actigraphy-derived sleep duration might tap into different aspects of sleep (Landry-Roy et al., 2018). In the current study, we used a questionnaire concerning multiple aspects of sleep including habit and disturbance; therefore we could find the association between sleep and EF. Results from the current study and the studies mentioned above suggest that the multiple aspects of sleep, rather than the sleep duration, could be significantly associated with EF development of children. Of note, it does not mean that adequate sleep duration is not important for children’s cognitive development. A meta-analysis showed that the effect of inadequate sleep on simple attention (the more basic cognitive component) was “large,” while its effect on EF including working memory and short-term memory was just “small” (Lim and Dinges, 2010). We therefore speculate that different aspects of sleep are associated with different parts of cognition.
The complex relationship in which sleep disturbance is directly associated with inhibitory control and working memory while indirectly linked with cognitive flexibility might be attributed to the comprehensive inter-structure of EF itself. It is suggested and has been evidenced that the three components of EF share some common bedrock but have their own separate parts (Garon et al., 2008). Each EF component is supposed to have a non-executive common part to which the other two components also share, and an executive specific part which it holds exclusively. Research has proven that sleep deprivation can affect the executive and non-executive processes in different patterns (Tucker et al., 2010). In order to interpret the results in the current study, it may be safe to conclude that habitual sleep disturbance in children may be associated with the common part as well as the specific parts of working memory and inhibitory control but may not be linked to the specific part of flexibility. There was converging evidence yielded in other studies: it was found that the performance on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, which indexed cognitive flexibility, was not significantly associated with one-night sleep deprivation (Steinhoff and Eckardt, 1999) or consecutive days of partial sleep deprivation (Khazaie et al., 2010). These results suggest that cognitive flexibility might be not sensitive to sleep problems.
The authors were also concerned that the correlation between sleep and EF in the present study might be resulted from some confounding variables. The first considered factor in developmental research was age, so we controlled for children’s age in the subsequent partial correlation analysis, and the results revealed that the association between sleep and EF remained significant. Besides, other possible noise variables (such as socio-economic status and parenting styles) that were not measured in the present study might explain the association between sleep and EF in children. However, our further examination suggested that confounding variables could not account for the whole correlation between sleep and EF. If the common correlated part among sleep and the three EF components could be completely explained by the confounding variables, then all the three correlations between sleep and each of the EF components would completely disappear after controlling for the other two EF components, since that had completely encompassed the common correlated part shared by all the EF components and sleep. However, the analysis of partial correlation between inhibitory control and sleep after controlling for the other EF components, as well as that between working memory and sleep after controlling for the other EF components, denied the case. It suggested there might be, at least, some extra or specific link between sleep and inhibitory control, and between sleep and working memory, even when the common covariate factors were ruled out.
The current study was just a correlational study and could not reveal the direction of the relationship. Hence, the finding of the link between sleep and EF could be resulted from either the effect of sleep on EF or the effect of EF on sleep. Both of the two directions are possible to work in everyday life. Nevertheless, we tended to interpret the current result as that sleep might have an impact on EF development: on one hand, there were numerous studies employing sleep deprivation evidenced that sleep did impact several cognitive abilities including some EF elements (Lim and Dinges, 2010); on the other hand, there was also evidence from neuroscience research, which demonstrated that several important neural processes benefit from sleep. For example, sleep would facilitate the process of synaptic downscaling (or homeostasis), which makes the excessive activity of neurons during the performance of specific information-processing in the day-time into an energetically suitable level (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006; Makela et al., 2020). Sleep at night would also help the upregulation of protein associated with brain and synaptic plasticity (Basheer et al., 2005; Cirelli, 2005). These molecular biological processes benefitted from sleep are the neural basis for the development of cognitive abilities including EF. Further, the particular relationship between sleep disturbance and the development of EF ability can be interpreted through the maturation of the prefrontal region of the brain. Evidence from cognitive neuroscience studies suggested that the development of prefrontal cortex is particularly vulnerable to the effect of sleep disturbance (Harrison et al., 2000). Meanwhile, the EF ability was proved to be strongly associated with prefrontal cortex (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Garon et al., 2008). Hence, it is speculated that the disruption of sleep leads to subsequent prefrontal cortical dysfunction, resulting in the poor performance in the EF test.
To our best knowledge, the current study is the first to investigate the complex relationship between self-reported sleep disturbance in everyday life and all the three basic EF subcomponents in typically developing children, revealing that the correlations between each of the EF components and sleep were all significant but the positive association between sleep and cognitive flexibility did not hold after controlling for the other two EF (inhibitory control and working memory). These findings advance current knowledge about the relationship between sleep and cognitive development. Yet, there are limitations to be considered in future research. In the current study, all the EF performances were measured by accuracy, not by reaction time (RT). Since some researchers suggested that RT or speed is more reliable in certain cognitive tests (Steinborn et al., 2018), such as in the current inhibitory control test (Lagattuta et al., 2011), more indexes or tasks are needed in further investigation. Moreover, as the current study only tested early primary school children in China, it remains to be evaluated whether the same result pattern will emerge in individuals from other countries, or from other age-stages (such as preschoolers, adolescents, or adults). In the context of cognitive development, some evidence shows that the structure of EF does not remain stable but develops with age. For example, by using factor analysis of different EF tests, the component of shifting was found to be mixed with inhibition when the children are young (Lee et al., 2013). For this reason, we might expect there are possibly different correlation patterns among EF tasks and sleep in different age groups. In addition, as we have discussed above, sleep scales and sleep duration derived from actigraphy might tap into different aspects of sleep that might impact EF differently. Given the fact the current study only used the Sleep Self-Report scale, future research should utilize both objective and subjective methods to measure sleep.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Psychology Department, Beijing Union University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
Author Contributions
YC, MZ, and NW conceived the study. YC, YW, and SW analyzed the data. YC and YW drafted the manuscript. MZ and NW revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the Beijing Social Science Foundation (No.16JYC023), Scientific Research Project of Beijing Education Committee, Premium Funding Project for Academic Human Resources Development in Beijing Union University to YC, Science and Technology Planning Project of Guangdong Province (No.2017A070713010) to MZ, Academic Research Projects of Beijing Union University (No.JS10202002) to NW.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.793000/full#supplementary-material
References
Alloway, T. P., and Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 106, 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003
Alvarez, J., and Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 16, 17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Jones, R. D., and Tranel, D. (1991). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 13, 909–922. doi: 10.1080/01688639108405107
Anderson, B., Storfer-Isser, A., Taylor, H. G., Rosen, C. L., and Redline, S. (2009). Associations of executive function with sleepiness and sleep duration in adolescents. Pediatrics 123, e701–e707. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1182
Aronen, E. T., Paavonen, E. J., Fjallberg, M., Soininen, M., and Torronen, J. (2000). Sleep and psychiatric symptoms in school-age children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 502–508. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00020
Astill, R. G., Van der Heijden, K. B., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Van Someren, E. J. (2012). Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: a century of research meta-analyzed. Psychol. Bull. 138, 1109–1138. doi: 10.1037/a0028204
Basheer, R., Brown, R., Ramesh, V., Begum, S., and McCarley, R. W. (2005). Sleep deprivation-induced protein changes in basal forebrain: implications for synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. Res. 82, 650–658. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20675
Beebe, D. W. (2011). Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 58, 649–665. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.002
Beebe, D. W., Groesz, L., Wells, C., Nichols, A., and McGee, K. (2003). The neuropsychological effects of obstructive sleep apnea: a meta-analysis of norm-referenced and case-controlled data. Sleep 26, 298–307. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.298
Bernier, A., Beauchamp, M. H., Bouvette-Turcot, A. A., Carlson, S. M., and Carrier, J. (2013). Sleep and cognition in preschool years: specific links to executive functioning. Child Dev. 84, 1542–1553. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12063
Bonnet, M., and Rosa, R. R. (1987). Sleep and performance in young adults and older normals and insomniacs during acute sleep loss and recovery. Biol. Psychol. 25, 153–172. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(87)90035-4
Bourke, R., Anderson, V., Yang, J. S. C., Jackman, A. R., Killedar, A., Nixon, G. M., et al. (2011). Cognitive and academic functions are impaired in children with all severities of sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med. 12, 489–496. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.010
Buckhalt, J. A., El-Sheikh, M., and Keller, P. (2007). Children's sleep and cognitive functioning: race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects. Child Dev. 78, 213–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00993.x
Cirelli, C. (2005). A molecular window on sleep: changes in gene expression between sleep and wakefulness. Neuroscientist 11, 63–74. doi: 10.1177/1073858404270900
de Bruin, E. J., van Run, C., Staaks, J., and Meijer, A. M. (2017). Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 32, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.006
Dewald, J. F., Meijer, A. M., Oort, F. J., Kerkhof, G. A., and Bögels, S. M. (2010). The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Ednick, M., Cohen, A. P., McPhail, G. L., Beebe, D., Simakajornboon, N., and Amin, R. S. (2009). A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep 32, 1449–1458. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1449
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., and Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 134, 31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., and Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition 53, 129–153. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
Harrison, Y., Horne, J. A., and Rothwell, A. (2000). Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults—a model for healthy aging? Sleep 23, 1067–1073. doi: 10.1093/sleep/23.8.1f
Holzhausen, E. A., Hagen, E. W., LeCaire, T., Cadmus-Bertram, L., Malecki, K. C., and Peppard, P. E. (2021). A comparison of self- and proxy-reported subjective sleep durations with objective actigraphy measurements in a survey of Wisconsin children 6-17 years of age. Am. J. Epidemiol. 190, 755–765. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwaa254
Hoyniak, C. P., Bates, J. E., Staples, A. D., Rudasill, K. M., Molfese, D. L., and Molfese, V. J. (2019). Child sleep and socioeconomic context in the development of cognitive abilities in early childhood. Child Dev. 90, 1718–1737. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13042
Jones, K., and Harrison, Y. (2001). Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 5, 463–475. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0203
Joyce, A., and Dimitriou, D. (2017). Sleep-disordered breathing and cognitive functioning in preschool children with and without Down syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 61, 778–791. doi: 10.1111/jir.12387
Karpinski, A. C., Scullin, M. H., and Montgomery-Downs, H. E. (2008). Risk for sleep-disordered breathing and executive function in preschoolers. Sleep Med. 9, 418–424. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.06.004
Khazaie, H., Tahmasian, M., Ghadami, M. R., Safaei, H., and Russo, M. B. (2010). The effects of chronic partial sleep deprivation on cognitive functions of medical residents. Iran. J. Psychiatry 5, 74–77.
Killgore, W. D. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 185, 105–129. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5
Lagattuta, K. H., Sayfan, L., and Monsour, M. (2011). A new measure for assessing executive function across a wide age range: children and adults find happy-sad more difficult than day-night. Dev. Sci. 14, 481–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00994.x
Landry-Roy, C., Bernier, A., Gravel, J., and Beauchamp, M. H. (2018). Executive functions and their relation to sleep following mild traumatic brain injury in preschoolers. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 769–780. doi: 10.1017/s1355617718000401
Lau, E. Y., Choi, E. W., Lai, E. S., Lau, K. N., Au, C. T., Yung, W. H., et al. (2015). Working memory impairment and its associated sleep-related respiratory parameters in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Med. 16, 1109–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.04.025
Lee, K., Bull, R., and Ho, R. M. H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Dev. 84, 1933–1953. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12096
Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol. Bull. 136, 375–389. doi: 10.1037/a0018883
Makela, T. E., Peltola, M. J., Saarenpaa-Heikkila, O., Himanen, S. L., Paunio, T., Paavonen, E. J., et al. (2020). Night awakening and its association with executive functioning across the first two years of life. Child Dev. 91, e937–e951. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13326
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12, 313–324. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(76)80035-4
Orgilés, M., Owens, J. A., Espada, J. P., Piqueras, J. A., and Carballo, J. L. (2013). Spanish version of the Sleep Self-Report (SSR): factorial structure and psychometric properties. Child Care Health Dev. 39, 288–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2012.01389.x
Owens, J. A., Maxim, R., Nobile, C., McGuinn, M., and Msall, M. (2000a). Parental and self-report of sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 154, 549–555. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.154.6.549
Owens, J. A., Spirito, A., McGuinn, M., and Nobile, C. (2000b). Sleep habits and sleep disturbance in elementary school-aged children. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 21, 27–36. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200002000-00005
Sabeti, S., Al-Darsani, Z., Mander, B. A., Corrada, M. M., and Kawas, C. H. (2018). Sleep, hippocampal volume, and cognition in adults over 90 years old. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 30, 1307–1318. doi: 10.1007/s40520-018-1030-x
Sadeh, A. (2007). Consequences of sleep loss or sleep disruption in children. Sleep Med. Clin. 2, 513–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2007.05.012
Sadeh, A., De Marcas, G., Guri, Y., Berger, A., Tikotzky, L., and Bar-Haim, Y. (2015). Infant sleep predicts attention regulation and behavior problems at 3-4 years of age. Dev. Neuropsychol. 40, 122–137. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.973498
Sadeh, A., Gruber, R., and Raviv, A. (2002). Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 73, 405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414
Sadeh, A., Gruber, R., and Raviv, A. (2003). The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 74, 444–455. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402008
Sattari, N., Whitehurst, L. N., Ahmadi, M., and Mednick, S. C. (2019). Does working memory improvement benefit from sleep in older adults? Neurobiol. Sleep Circadian Rhythms 6, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.nbscr.2019.01.001
Schwerdtle, B., Kanis, J., Kahl, L., Kübler, A., and Schlarb, A. A. (2012). Children's sleep comic: development of a new diagnostic tool for children with sleep disorders. Nat. Sci. Sleep 4, 97–102. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S33127
Sciberras, E., DePetro, A., Mensah, F., and Hiscock, H. (2015). Association between sleep and working memory in children with ADHD: a cross-sectional study. Sleep Med. 16, 1192–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.06.006
Sforza, E., Roche, F., Thomas-Anterion, C., Kerleroux, J., Beauchet, O., Celle, S., et al. (2010). Cognitive function and sleep related breathing disorders in a healthy elderly population: the SYNAPSE study. Sleep 33, 515–521. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.4.515
Shochat, T., Cohen-Zion, M., and Tzischinsky, O. (2014). Functional consequences of inadequate sleep in adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 18, 75–87. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2013.03.005
Simpson, A., and Riggs, K. J. (2005). Factors responsible for performance on the day–night task: response set or semantics? Dev. Sci. 8, 360–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00424.x
Steinborn, M. B., Langner, R., Flehmig, H. C., and Huestegge, L. (2018). Methodology of performance scoring in the d2 sustained-attention test: cumulative-reliability functions and practical guidelines. Psychol. Assess. 30, 339–357. doi: 10.1037/pas0000482
Steinhoff, B. J., and Eckardt, K. M. (1999). Clinical and EEG findings in complex partial status epilepticus with tiagabine. Seizure 8:197. doi: 10.1053/seiz.1999.0280
Steur, L. M. H., Grootenhuis, M. A., Terwee, C. B., Pillen, S., Wolters, N. G. J., Kaspers, G. J. L., et al. (2019). Psychometric properties and norm scores of the sleep self report in Dutch children. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 17, 15–19. doi: 10.1186/s12955-018-1073-x
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 49–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002
Tucker, A. M., Whitney, P., Belenky, G., Hinson, J. M., and Van Dongen, H. P. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep 33, 47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47
van Litsenburg, R. R., Waumans, R. C., van den Berg, G., and Gemke, R. J. (2010). Sleep habits and sleep disturbances in Dutch children: a population-based study. Eur. J. Pediatr. 169, 1009–1015. doi: 10.1007/s00431-010-1169-8
Vermeulen, M. C. M., Astill, R. G., Benjamins, J. S., Swaab, H., Van Someren, E. J. W., and van der Heijden, K. B. (2016). Temperament moderates the association between sleep duration and cognitive performance in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 144, 184–198. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.014
Keywords: executive function, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, working memory, sleep disturbance, habitual sleep, Sleep Self-Report, cognitive development
Citation: Chen Y, Wang Y, Wang S, Zhang M and Wu N (2021) Self-Reported Sleep and Executive Function in Early Primary School Children. Front. Psychol. 12:793000. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.793000
Edited by:
Yoshifumi Ikeda, Joetsu University of Education, JapanReviewed by:
Michael B. Steinborn, Julius Maximilian University of Würzburg, GermanyAndrew Simpson, University of Essex, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2021 Chen, Wang, Wang, Zhang and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ming Zhang, emhhbmdtNTVAbWFpbC5zeXN1LmVkdS5jbg==; Nan Wu, d3VuYW50b21AMTI2LmNvbQ==
†These authors have contributed equally to this work