Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 10 January 2022
Sec. Psychopathology

Paranormal Experience Profiles and Their Association With Variations in Executive Functions: A Latent Profile Analysis

  • 1Department of Psychology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, United Kingdom
  • 2Adelphi Values Ltd., Bollington, United Kingdom
  • 3Blanquerna Foundation, Barcelona, Spain

This study investigated relationships between inter-class variations in paranormal experience and executive functions. A sample of 516 adults completed self-report measures assessing personal encounter-based paranormal occurrences (i.e., Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability), executive functions (i.e., General Executive Function, Working and Everyday Memory, and Decision Making) together with Emotion Regulation and Belief in the Paranormal. Paranormal belief served as a measure of convergent validity for experience-based phenomena. Latent profile analysis (LPA) combined experience-based indices into four classes based on sample subpopulation scores. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) then examined interclass differences. Results revealed that breadth of paranormal experience was associated with higher levels of executive functioning difficulties for General Executive Function, Working Memory, Decision Making, and Belief in the Paranormal. On the Everyday Memory Questionnaire, scores differed on Attention Tracking (focus loss) and Factor 3 (visual reconstruction), but not Retrieval (distinct memory failure). In the case of the Emotion Regulation Scale, class scores varied on Expressive Suppression (control), however, no difference was evident on Cognitive Reappraisal (reframing). Overall, inter-class comparisons identified subtle differences in executive functions related to experience. Since the present study was exploratory, sampled only a limited subset of executive functions, and used subjective, self-report measures, further research is necessary to confirm these outcomes. This should employ objective tests and include a broader range of executive functions.

Introduction

Drinkwater et al. (2021b) found that greater personal involvement with the paranormal (i.e., experience, visiting practitioners and self-professed ability) was associated with increased proneness to reality testing deficits, greater emotion-based reasoning, and higher paranormal belief. These results deepened conceptual understanding by demonstrating that nuanced variations in paranormal experience were related to subtle differences in cognitive-perceptual factors allied to subclinical delusion formation and thinking style. These outcomes concurred with those observed for paranormal belief (Irwin et al., 2012a,b; Williams et al., 2021).

Extending this work, the present paper investigated relationships between personal paranormal encounters and executive functions. Executive functions denote interrelated mental activities comprising top-down processes (Burgess and Simons, 2005; Diamond, 2013), specifically interference control (the ability to effectively select stimuli in accordance with set goals for further processing), working memory (short-term storage and manipulation of information), inhibition (self-control and resistance to acting impulsively), and cognitive flexibility (ability to think outside pre-established frameworks). These component processes play an integral role in everyday activities such as planning, recall, dual-tasking, and attentional focus (Diamond, 2013).

To date, only limited research has examined relationships between paranormal factors and executive functions. Moreover, published work has focused largely on beliefs, particularly religion and superstition. Although these domains are core facets of paranormality (Dagnall et al., 2010c), they represent only a narrow range of construct content. Furthermore, the fact these supernatural facets share important features constrains the generalizability of findings to other paranormal domains. Specifically, religious and superstitious beliefs derive from magical ideation, credence in forms of causation that are scientifically/conventionally invalid (Eckblad and Chapman, 1983), and persist in the absence of empirical support (Lindeman and Svedholm, 2012). In terms of executive functions, increased religiosity is linked to activity in frontal brain regions, explicitly the dorsolateral-pre-frontal cortex (Azari et al., 2001), and areas in the medial prefrontal cortex (Muramoto, 2004; Rim et al., 2019). Whereas superstition is concomitant with medial temporal lobe dysfunction, predominantly the hippocampus (Brugger et al., 1994).

Regarding religion, Schjoedt et al. (2013) contend that executive resource depletion plays a crucial role in sacred experiences. Specifically, three features (i.e., demand for the expressive suppression of emotion, exposure to goal demoted and causally opaque actions, and presence of a charismatic authority) limit the capacity for individual processing of religious events, and concurrently increase susceptibility to authoritative narratives. In support of the cognitive resource depletion model (Schjoedt et al., 2013), studies have shown that reductions in available resources (decreased working memory capacity) are related to increased paranormal belief (Richards et al., 2014). Additionally, Richards et al. (2014) found that paranormal belief was positively associated with inattentional blindness. This is the inability to register an unexpected visual stimulus or event when attention is engaged on another task (Mack, 2003). This is an important observation, since inattentional blindness is partially dependent upon reduced top-down processing (executive control) (Hannon and Richards, 2010).

Lack of executive control can also result in increased susceptibility to proactive interference and the tendency to perceive random coincidences as causally related (Rominger et al., 2011). The inclination to see patterns within meaningless stimuli is a significant prognosticator of higher levels of paranormal belief (Dagnall et al., 2007, 2014, 2016a,b; Escolà-Gascón, 2020). Correspondingly, the ability to overcome interference plays a central role in working memory (Lustig et al., 2001; Rominger et al., 2019).

Other biases related to paranormal belief (i.e., false perceived agency, Riekki et al., 2014; and faces in noise, Riekki et al., 2013) are explicable in terms executive functioning. Particularly, they denote failures to actively inhibit or exercise sufficient control over the influence of top-down signals on the processing of sensory data. Neuroimaging studies support this supposition. For example, Lindeman et al. (2013) found that paranormal believers reported seeing more illusory “signs” (indications or symbolic hints) in neutral pictures (e.g., a brick wall) when imagining personal scenarios. This tendency aligns with lower activity in the inferior frontal gyrus, which is activate during cognitive inhibition (e.g., Aron et al., 2004, 2014).

Further research reports links between variations in dopaminergic activity, paranormal belief, and executive functions. These relationships are potentially attributable to the fact that the pre-frontal region receives dopaminergic inputs from the midbrain and expresses a variety of dopamine receptor subtypes (Logue and Gould, 2014; Ott and Nieder, 2019). Pertinent to the present paper, Krummenacher et al. (2010) reported that alterations in dopaminergic activity altered cognitive processing in skeptics. Subsequently, they performed more like paranormal believers on a perceptual task requiring the detection of meaningful patterns amidst noise. Krummenacher et al. (2010) concluded that this was imputable to dopamine reducing perceptual sensitivity (the signal to noise ratio). This is consistent with the observation that greater internal noise (compared to signal) can produce perceptual aberrations, generate delusions, and enhance superstitious beliefs (Shaner, 1999). These outcomes are commensurate with the view that executive processes play an integral role in error monitoring (van Elk and Aleman, 2017).

Extending previous research on cognitive-perceptual personality factors (Drinkwater et al., 2021a,b), this paper conducted an exploratory investigation to determine whether differences in the breadth and intensity of paranormal experiences were associated with variations in neuropsychological measures related to self-reported executive functions (i.e., memory and decision making). In addition, the ability to control or regulate emotional responses was assessed. Emotion regulation was included as past work has shown associations between executive-control functions and emotions plays a role in anomalous experiences (Schjoedt et al., 2013). That is, similar frontal regions that form the neural basis of executive functioning also influence emotional expressive control and appraisal (e.g., Zelazo and Cunningham, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2009; Schmeichel and Tang, 2015). These regions have additionally been theorized to be a factor underpinning anomalous experience such as religion (Grafman et al., 2020). Accordingly, this study explored how emotional regulation functions in the context of paranormal belief and ability.

Finally, the level of paranormal belief served as a measure of convergent validity for experience-based phenomena. This was necessary because experience-based indices (i.e., Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability) were amalgamated to form “novel” composite measures. Using Cohen (1988) effect size guidelines, consistent with preceding research, moderate-large positive correlations should be observed between belief and experience-based measures. Furthermore, Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability should be positively related to belief but make distinct, unique contributions.

Consistent with Drinkwater et al. (2021b), the present study used latent profile analysis (LPA) to combine personal encounter-based phenomena (Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability). LPA extends research in several ways. Specifically, it identifies important, nuanced variations in individual supernatural histories/perceptions, assesses a broader range of factors than traditional measures (i.e., subjective paranormal experience), and acknowledges that experience of the paranormal is heterogeneous (see Drinkwater et al., 2021b). Furthermore, the application of LPA to belief has provided important conceptual insights (see Denovan et al., 2018).

Methods

Design

Data collection occurred at one point in time. A frequent criticism of this cross-sectional approach is vulnerability to common method variance (CMV) (Spector, 2019). To counter CMV, the present investigation implemented procedural remedies (Krishnaveni and Deepa, 2013). Explicitly, instructions to respondents created psychological distance between scales by emphasizing the unique nature of each construct (Podsakoff et al., 2003). In addition, to lessen possible social desirability and evaluation apprehension effects, directions informed respondents that there were no correct answers.

Respondents

The sample comprised 516 respondents, mean age (M) = 40.17 years, SD = 16.85, range 18–87. There were 225 males (44%), M = 40.96 years, SD = 17.89, range 18–87; and 290 females (56%), M = 39.62 years, SD = 16.00, range 18–75. One participant failed to specify gender. Data screening revealed that skewness and kurtosis values were acceptable; within −2.0 to + 2.0 (Byrne, 2013). Respondent recruitment was via Bilendi, an established online provider of quality, representative samples (Salak et al., 2021). Generally, panel data is diverse, far reaching, and comparable with traditional methods (Kees et al., 2017). The researchers requested United Kingdom-based respondents aged 18 years and over.

Measures

Experiential Paranormal Factors

Personal encounter-based phenomena comprised Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability (see Dagnall et al., 2016c; Drinkwater et al., 2020, 2021a,b).

Experience

Items established whether respondents had directly encountered commonly reported, core receptive paranormal phenomena (see Drinkwater et al., 2013; Dagnall et al., 2016c; Drinkwater et al., 2017a,2018). Explicitly, psi (i.e., mental abilities/powers) and life after death (i.e., communication with the deceased). Experiences included psychic occurrence, mediumship, spiritualism, telepathy, precognition, premonition, and remote viewing. The provision of precise conceptual based definitions ensured that respondents were aware of the nature of each phenomenon. For example, “Spiritualists provide information regarding the transition of the human spirit from the physical body to the afterlife. In the context of this definition, have you ever personally experienced spiritualism?” Totaling experiences produced a score ranging from 0 (no experience) to 8 (experienced all phenomena). This is a well-established, reliable method for assessing self-reported paranormal experiences (see Dagnall et al., 2016c; Drinkwater et al., 2020, 2021a,b).

Practitioner Visiting

Questions enquired whether respondents had visited paranormal practitioners in areas allied to psi and life after death. Classifications reflected the foremost industries (i.e., Mediums, Psychics, Spiritualists, and Fortune-Tellers). Respondents indicated whether they had visited each practitioner type using a dichotomous format (yes vs. no). This scale has demonstrated satisfactory internal reliability (Drinkwater et al., 2021b). Scores thus ranged from 0 to 4, with higher scores indicating greater interaction with paranormal practitioners.

Self-Perceived Ability

Items asked respondents whether they believed that they possessed paranormal ability in each of the core phenomena (i.e., mediumship, psychic, spiritualism, and fortune telling). For instance, “To what extent (in percentage terms, 0–100) do you believe that you possess psychic abilities?” This method has excellent internal consistency (Drinkwater et al., 2021b). A further item asked whether respondents were paranormal practitioners (yes vs. no). These measures allowed the researchers to categorize respondents as no ability, self-professed ability (moderate), and practitioners (high). Previous research has established the validity of these groupings and that they differ in intensity of personal ascription of level of ability (see Drinkwater et al., 2021a).

Measures of Executive Function

Self-report measures assessed executive functioning including General Executive Functions, Memory, and Decision Making.

General Executive Functions (Webexec)

The Webexec (Buchanan et al., 2010) is a measure of executive functioning problems designed for internet-mediated research. The scale contains six items assessing difficulties in maintaining focus, concentration, multitasking, sustaining a train of thought, completing tasks, and acting impulsively. Items appear as questions (e.g., “Do you find it difficult to keep your attention on a particular task?”) and participants respond via a four-point Likert scale (1 = no and 4 = many problems). Summation of item scores produces a total ranging from 6 to 24, with higher scores indicating greater executive functioning problems. The Webexec has demonstrated construct and convergent validity, and satisfactory internal reliability (Buchanan et al., 2010). In this study, good alpha reliability existed, α = 0.89.

The Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised

The 13-items Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised (EMQ-R) (Royle and Lincoln, 2008) assesses everyday memory-related behaviors (e.g., “Completely forgetting to do things you said you would do, and things you planned to do.”). Participants respond by providing an estimate of how many times item content has occurred over the previous month (0 = once or less, 1 = more than once a month but less than once a week, 2 = about once a week, 3 = more than once a week but less than once a day, 4 = once or more in a day). The scale encompasses three subscales measuring Retrieval (i.e., memory failure), Attention Tracking (focus loss), and Factor 3 (visual reconstruction). Previous studies indicate that the EMQ-R has sound psychometric properties (Royle and Lincoln, 2008). These subscales demonstrated good to satisfactory reliability; Retrieval (α = 0.92), Attention Tracking (α = 0.87), and Factor 3 (α = 0.73).

The Working Memory Questionnaire

The Working Memory Questionnaire (WMQ) (Vallat-Azouvi et al., 2012) comprises 30 items investigating three domains of working memory: short-term storage (the ability to retain information for a brief period), attention (i.e., distractibility, mental slowness, mental fatigue, and dual-task processing), and executive function (i.e., decision making, planning, and shifting). Items appear as questions (e.g., “Do you have difficulty remembering what you have read?”) and participants respond via a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (“no problem at all”) to 4 (“very severe problem in everyday life”). Higher scores represent greater working memory difficulties. The WMQ is a robust, psychometrically validated scale (Vallat-Azouvi et al., 2012). Excellent reliability existed in this study, α = 0.97.

Decision Making Questionnaire

The present study used the Control and Instinctiveness subscales of the Decision Making Questionnaire (DMQ) (French et al., 1993) to assess decision making efficacy. These dimensions evaluate impulsiveness (hastiness), which is a key component of executive functioning. Items appear as questions and participants respond, using a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“Very infrequently or never”) to 6 (“Very frequently or always”). The scale has been psychometrically validated (French et al., 1993) and used in published studies (Douse and McManus, 1993; Kumar and Gupta, 2017). For the current study, the composite measure demonstrated satisfactory reliability, α = 0.73.

Other Measures

Additional measures assessed self-reported ability to control emotional responses (Emotional Regulation) and Paranormal Belief.

Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross and John, 2003) includes 10 items that evaluate dispositional emotion regulation strategies. Specifically, expressive suppression (i.e., “I control my emotions by not expressing them”) and cognitive reappraisal (i.e., “When I want to feel more positive emotions, I change the way I’m thinking about the situation”). The scale presents items as statements and participants respond by completing a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The ERQ has established psychometric integrity (Gross and John, 2003). In the present study, internal reliability was adequate (expressive suppression, α = 0.60; cognitive reappraisal, α = 0.60).

Belief in the Paranormal (Revised Paranormal Belief Scale)

The Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS) (Tobacyk, 2004) is a 26-item measure of belief in the paranormal. Items appear as statements (e.g., “I believe in God”) and participants provide responses using a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) (see Lange et al., 2000). Total scores range from 0 to 156, with higher scores indicating belief in the paranormal. The RPBS is a widely used, psychometrically satisfactory measure (Drinkwater et al., 2017b). Internal reliability in this study was excellent, α = 0.96.

Procedure

Prospective respondents accessed information by clicking on a web link. After reading the participant information sheet, only respondents providing informed consent progressed. Subsequent instructions then directed respondents to read and complete all items, provide truthful answers, and progress through sections at their own pace. The survey contained subsections comprising demographic characteristics (i.e., age and preferred gender), paranormal experiences, measures of executive functioning, and paranormal belief. To prevent potential order effects, sections and scales rotated across respondents. On completion of the study, respondents were debriefed.

Ethics Statement

Ethical approval was provided by the Manchester Metropolitan University, Faculty of Health, Psychology and Social Care Ethics Committee (October 2018; Project ID, 954).

Results

Analytical Strategy

Initially analysis examined descriptive relationships. Then, using Mplus version 7 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012), group membership was established by combining personal encounter-based paranormal measures (Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability). Model fit evaluated a 1-class model, followed by iterations with increasing latent profiles. A range of indices determined the number of latent profiles (classes): Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), sample-size adjusted BIC (ssaBIC; Sclove, 1987), Lo-Mendell-Rubin-adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-A-LRT; Lo et al., 2001), and a measure of entropy (Ramaswamy et al., 1993). Lower values for AIC, BIC, and ssaBIC indicate greater fit in addition to consideration of LMR-A-LRT and entropy. LMR-A-LRT tested significance of fit (via a p value). For entropy, values above 0.8 specify sound separation of profiles relative to data (Ramaswamy et al., 1993). Finally, analysis using SPSS26 explored class differences on study measures.

Descriptive Statistics

Data screening identified seven z-scores marginally greater than 3.25. Prior to analysis, the researchers transformed these to the next highest score (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001). Correlations (Table 1) revealed personal encounter-based indices (Experience, Practitioner Visiting, and Ability) correlated positively. Paranormal Belief, General Executive Functioning, Working Memory, Decision Making, Emotion Regulation correlated positively, as did Emotion Regulation and Everyday Memory subscales. Several associations were small (r = 0.10–0.30) (Cohen, 1992). However, using Gignac and Szodorai’s (2016) guidelines these signify meaningful relationships (i.e., small, r = 0.11; medium, r = 0.19, and large, r = 0.29).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.

Latent Profile Analysis

Assessment of 1-class and 2-class models revealed superior fit for the 2-class model (Table 2), with lower AIC, BIC, ssaBIC, and a significant LMR-A-LRT. Comparison of 2-class and 3-class models found the 3-class solution superior.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fit of latent profile models.

The 4-class solution was selected as the final model. Specifically, although greater AIC, BIC and ssaBIC existed, it possessed a considerably lower (and significant) LMR-A-LRT value vs. the 3-class model alongside higher entropy (0.94 vs. 0.82). The three- and four-class models were, nonetheless, examined closely, as they both could have legitimate arguments for being selected. Empirical factors, such as greater separation among profiles, indicated a more defensible basis for selecting the 4-class solution. Finally, the 5-class model failed to improve the model (see Figure 1). Average latent class probabilities for latent profile membership were 0.92 for Class 1 (Low Experience, Moderate Visiting, and High Ability), 0.86 for Class 2 (Moderate Experience, Low Visiting, and Ability), 0.85 for Class 3 (Low Experience, Moderate Visiting, and Low Ability), and 0.89 for Class 4 (Low Experience, Visiting, and Ability), indicating good discrimination.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Latent profiles in relation to Paranormal Experience, Paranormal Practitioner Visiting, and Paranormal Ability (scores range from 0 to 1 since they are depicted as probabilities/percentages).

Association of Latent Profiles With Neuropsychological Measures

Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) assessed latent profiles on Paranormal Belief, General Executive Function, Working Memory, and Decision Making (Table 3). Analysis revealed a significant main effect of group, Pillai’s trace = 0.57, F(12, 1,533) = 30.40, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.19 (large effect size). All outcomes differed significantly.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Latent profile effects on paranormal belief, executive function, working memory, and decision making.

Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons (Table 3) revealed that Class 1 demonstrated highest Belief in the Paranormal and Decision Making. Class 3 reported highest Executive Function and Working Memory disruption. Class 4 had significantly lower scores on all outcomes, with the exceptions of Class 2 and Class 3 on Decision Making.

Emotion Regulation and Everyday Memory Subscale Analysis

Further MANOVA analysis examined differences on Emotion Regulation (Cognitive Reappraisal and Expressive Suppression) and Everyday Memory (Retrieval, Attention Tracking, and Factor 3) subscales (Table 4). A significant main effect occurred, Pillai’s trace = 0.16, F(15, 1,530) = 5.88, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.06 (medium effect size). Significant group effects existed for all variables but Cognitive Reappraisal. Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons (Table 4) revealed that Class 4 scored significantly lower than Class 1 on Attention Tracking Factor 3, and Expressive Suppression, and lower than Class 3 on Retrieval, Attention Tracking, and Factor 3. A less consistent pattern existed for remaining profiles. Specifically, Class 1 scored highest on Expressive Suppression, and Class 3 scored highest on Retrieval, Attention Tracking, and Factor 3.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Latent profile effects on emotion regulation and everyday memory subscales.

Discussion

Based on differences in Experience, Visiting, and Ability, latent profile analysis (LPA) identified four subpopulations. These represented variations in individual supernatural histories and reflected discrete and complex interactions between experiential indices. In this context, consistent with Drinkwater et al. (2021b), LPA advanced experience-based research by acknowledging the broad and heterogeneous empirical basis of supernatural phenomena. Additionally, LPA recognized importance intra-class communalities and disparities.

From a methodological perspective, creation of classes based on multiple experience factors is an important development because preceding work has typically relied upon single measures. For instance, theorists have placed an overreliance on “subjective paranormal experiences” (SPEs) (e.g., Palmer and Neppe, 2004; Dagnall et al., 2016c). Although consideration of SPEs has provided valuable insights into individual perceptions of paranormality, the construct is limited as it references only one encounter type. Precisely, willingness to attribute supernatural causation to a specific event or occurrence. Despite this, since SPE inclusion facilitates direct comparisons with preceding findings, ensuing work should continue to use SPEs as a component within multiple measures.

The addition of Visiting and Ability alongside Experience ensured that the present study sampled a range of personal paranormal occurrences. In this context, the application of LPA enabled amalgamation of discrete but related factors to form conceptually meaningful classes. Consideration of zero-order correlations supported this approach. Explicitly, although intra-experiential measure associations were high, there remained a large proportion of unexplained variance. This, as predicted, indicated that while Experience, Visiting, and Ability overlap, each factor makes a unique contribution.

In comparison to Drinkwater et al. (2021b), the observed relationships in the present paper, both within and between experiential measures and paranormal belief, were stronger. Given sample equivalence, this suggests that scores derived from multiple indices of experience may be subject to variation. Further research should investigate this. Nonetheless, findings supported the notion that experiential paranormal factors and belief are inherently interconnected and reciprocally reinforcing (Dagnall et al., 2015, 2020; van Elk, 2017).

Class comparisons found significant differences across measures of executive function (General, Everyday and Working Memory, Emotion Regulation, and Decision Making) and Belief in the Paranormal. These were consistent for the highest (Class 1, Low Experience, Moderate Visiting and High Ability) vs. least experience (Class 4, Low Experience, Visiting, and Ability) groups. Class 1 (vs. Class 4) membership was associated with greater reported executive functioning difficulties. Analysis of Everyday Memory and Emotion Regulation scales revealed commensurate variations in performance. Everyday Memory, Class 4 (vs. Class 1) reported higher incidence of Attentional Tracking (sustained focus) and Factor 3 (visual reconstruction) difficulties; Retrieval (distinct memory failure) scores did not differ significantly. Emotion Regulation, Class 4 scored higher on Expressive Suppression (control).

Intermediate group comparisons (Class 2, Moderate Experience, Low Visiting, and Ability vs. Class 3, Low Experience, Moderate Practitioner Visiting and Low Ability) found that Class 3 reported higher levels of disruption on General Executive Function, Working Memory, Everyday Memory and Paranormal Belief. No differences were evident on Decision Making and Emotion Regulation. Finally, contrasts between the high (Class 1) and low (Class 4) and intermediate (Class 2 and Class 3) groupings identified further subtle variations in executive functioning. Collectively, outcomes indicated a relationship between range of paranormal encounters and self-reported executive functioning; greater experience was associated with higher levels of perceived disruption. This outcome aligned with Drinkwater et al. (2021b), who reported experience related differences on cognitive-perceptual factors allied to thinking style and delusion formation (reasons for these differences are discussed within section “Limitations and Future Research”).

Limitations and Future Research

Although the present paper was exploratory and intended only to indicate potential pathways for subsequent projects, it is important to acknowledge limitations. Firstly, selection of experiences was limited to core receptive phenomena. Future studies should include additional commonly reported occurrences such as superstitious behaviors. Secondly, paranormal experience has a broader base than that assessed by Experience, Visiting, and Ability. Hence, ensuing research should include additional indices. For example, engagement with paranormal media (documentaries, reality programs, etc.), group affiliation (i.e., clubs), and background (family/friends). This could also examine which combination of factors are most representative of paranormal experience and most likely to predict executive functioning variations.

Thirdly, the researchers used self-report measures to generate data. The degree to which subjective, extemporaneous evaluations accurately reflect higher-order processes is questionable due to propensity to interpretative bias. Moreover, individuals may not possess access to complex cognitive-perceptual processes (as opposed to metacognitive evaluations or implicit a priori theories of cognitive functioning) (Nisbett and Wilson, 1977; Dagnall et al., 2008a; Chan et al., 2015). A related concern is that increased disruption scores may represent differences in abstractive focus (i.e., preoccupation with internal processes) rather than actual decrements in executive functions. This supposition is based on the observation that experience of and belief in the paranormal correlate positively with proneness to reality testing deficits (Dagnall et al., 2018), and a range of individual differences variables associated with odd and unusual cognitions, perceptions, and sensations (i.e., schizotypy and transliminality; Dagnall et al., 2010b). These relationships, in part, explicate why paranormal attributions are typically less structured, more productive/fluid, and involve heightened stimulus sensitivity.

Additionally, the notion of preoccupation with internal processes is consistent with the finding that paranormal believers prefer intuitive, experiential-based evidence (Irwin and Young, 2002; Dagnall et al., 2010a; Drinkwater et al., 2012). Tentatively, this concurs with the ability to main external attention and avoid unnecessary disruption. This may explain why believers are more inclined to metacognitive errors such as false memories (French, 2003; Dagnall et al., 2008b). Thus, to ensure that higher levels of reported executive function disruption are not simply artifacts of cognitive style/personality, succeeding investigations should attempt to replicate current outcomes using objective neuropsychological tests (see Buchanan, 2016). These should include fine-grained analysis of the subset of executive functions included in this paper (i.e., general executive function, working memory, decision making, and emotion regulation), and additional elements such as attentional control and cognitive inhibition.

Finally, LPA is statistically driven rather than theoretically derived. Hence, it can generate profiles that lack conceptual coherence. In the present study, classes represented meaningful variations in experience, which corresponded to differences in paranormal belief. Subsequent work should iteratively seek to develop standardized experience measures. Part of this process requires cross-validation methods, such as double cross-validation (Collins et al., 1994) or progressive elaboration (Donovan and Chung, 2015). These are essential to prevent LPA misspecification. In this context, cross-validation methods objectively evaluate model fit and assess class stability.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Manchester Metropolitan University, Faculty of Health, Psychology and Social Care Ethics Committee (October 2018; Project ID, 954). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KD and ND designed the study. KD researched and collated measures and organized data collection. AP appraised measures and content on executive functions. AD conducted all analyses, which were checked by ÁE-G. ND summarized findings and synthesized content for all sections. KD, AD, AP, and ÁE-G edited the final manuscript and prepared the draft for submission. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the BIAL Foundation (Project ID:082/2018). The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

AD is employed by the company Adelphi Values Ltd.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank BIAL for their support.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52, 317–332. doi: 10.1007/BF02294359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azari, N. P., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellmann, L., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1649–1652. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugger, P., Dowdy, M. A., and Graves, R. E. (1994). From superstitious behavior to delusional thinking: the role of the hippocampus in misattributions of causality. Med. Hypotheses 43, 397–402. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(94)90015-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, T. (2016). Self-report measures of executive function problems correlate with personality, not performance-based executive function measures, in nonclinical samples. Psychol. Assessment 28, 372–385. doi: 10.1037/pas0000192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, T., Heffernan, T. M., Parrott, A. C., Ling, J., Rodgers, J., and Scholey, A. B. (2010). A short self-report measure of problems with executive function suitable for administration via the Internet. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 709–714. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, P. W., and Simons, J. S. (2005). “Theories of frontal lobe executive function: clinical applications,” in Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits, eds P. W. Halligan and D. T. Wade (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 211–231.

Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2013). Structural Equation Modeling With Mplus: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Chan, C. C., Spencer, C. C., West, C., Viegas, C., and Bedwell, J. S. (2015). Metacognitive processes in psychometrically defined schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. 230, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, L. M., Graham, J. W., Long, J. D., and Hansen, W. B. (1994). Crossvalidation of latent class models of early substance use onset. Multivariate Behav. Res. 29, 165–183. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2902_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Clough, P. (2016a). Toward a better understanding of the relationship between belief in the paranormal and statistical bias: the potential role of schizotypy. Front. Psychol. 7:1045. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Parker, A., and Rowley, K. (2016b). Misperception of chance, conjunction, framing effects and belief in the paranormal: a further evaluation. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 409–419. doi: 10.1002/acp.3217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Clough, P. (2016c). Paranormal experience, belief in the paranormal and anomalous beliefs. Paranthropology 7, 4–15.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Parker, A., Drinkwater, K., and Walsh, R. S. (2018). Confirmatory factor analysis of the inventory of personality organization-reality testing subscale. Front. Psychol. 9:1116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., and Parker, A. (2015). Suggestion, belief in the paranormal, proneness to reality testing deficits and perception of an allegedly haunted building. J. Parapsychol. 79, 87–104.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Parker, A., Munley, G., and Drinkwater, K. (2010c). Common paranormal belief dimensions. J. Sci. Exploration 24, 431–447.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Munley, G., Parker, A., and Drinkwater, K. (2010b). Paranormal belief, schizotypy, and transliminality. J. Parapsychol. 74, 117–141.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Munley, G. (2010a). Reality testing, belief in the paranormal, and urban legends. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 25, 25–55.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., and Rowley, K. (2014). Misperception of chance, conjunction, belief in the paranormal and reality testing: a reappraisal. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 28, 711–719.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Elliott, C., Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., and Parker, A. (2020). Predictors of hearing electronic voice phenomena in random noise: schizotypy, fantasy proneness, and paranormal beliefs. J. Parapsychol. 84, 96–113. doi: 10.30891/jopar.2020.01.09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Munley, G., and Parker, A. (2008a). Memory aberrations, transliminality, and delusional ideation. Perceptual Motor Skills 106, 67–75. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.1.67-75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Parker, A., and Munley, G. (2008b). News events, false memory and paranormal belief. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 23, 173–188.

Google Scholar

Dagnall, N., Parker, A., and Munley, G. (2007). Paranormal belief and reasoning. Person. Individ. Differ. 43, 1406–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., and Parker, A. (2018). Latent profile analysis of schizotypy and paranormal belief: associations with probabilistic reasoning performance. Front. Psychol. 9:35. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, J. E., and Chung, T. (2015). Progressive elaboration and cross-validation of a latent class typology of adolescent alcohol involvement in a national sample. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 76, 419–429. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douse, N. A., and McManus, I. C. (1993). The personality of fantasy game players. Br. J. Psychol. 84, 505–509. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1993.tb02498.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., and Bate, L. (2013). Into the unknown: using interpretative phenomenological analysis to explore personal accounts of paranormal experiences. J. Parapsychol. 77, 281–294.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., Grogan, S., and Riley, V. (2017a). Understanding the unknown: a thematic analysis of subjective paranormal experiences. Australian J. Parapsychol. 17, 23–46.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., and Parker, A. (2017b). An assessment of the dimensionality and factorial structure of the revised paranormal belief scale. Front. Psychol. 8:1693. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., and Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theories, and paranormal beliefs. J. Parapsychol. 76, 57–77.

Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., and Parker, A. (2018). The Australian sheep-goat scale: an evaluation of factor structure and convergent validity. Front. Psychol. 9:1594. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., and Williams, C. (2021b). Paranormal belief, thinking style and delusion formation: a latent profile analysis of within-individual variations in experience-based paranormal facets. Front. Psychol. 12:670959. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.670959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., and Williams, C. (2021a). Differences in cognitive-perceptual factors arising from variations in self-professed paranormal ability. Front. Psychol. 12:681520. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, K. G., Denovan, A., and Dagnall, N. (2020). Lucid dreaming, nightmares, and sleep paralysis: associations with reality testing deficits and paranormal experience/belief. Front. Psychol. 11:471. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckblad, M., and Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 51, 215–225. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.51.2.215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolà-Gascón, A. (2020). Researching unexplained phenomena II: new evidences for anomalous experiences supported by the Multivariable Multiaxial Suggestibility Inventory-2 (MMSI-2). Curr. Res. Behav. Sci. 1:100005. doi: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2020.100005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French, C. (2003). Fantastic memories: the relevance of research into eyewitness testimony and false memories for reports of anomalous experiences. J. Consciousness Stud. 10, 153–174.

Google Scholar

French, D. J., West, R. J., Elander, J., and Wilding, J. M. (1993). Decision-making style, driving style, and self-reported involvement in road traffic accidents. Ergonomics 36, 627–644. doi: 10.1080/00140139308967925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gignac, G. E., and Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Person. Individ. Differ. 102, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafman, J., Cristofori, I., Zhong, W., and Bulbulia, J. (2020). The neural basis of religious cognition. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 29, 126–133.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannon, E. M., and Richards, A. (2010). Is inattentional blindness related to individual differences in visual working memory capacity or executive control functioning? Perception 39, 309–319. doi: 10.1068/p6379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, H. J., Dagnall, N., and Drinkwater, K. (2012a). Paranormal belief and biases in reasoning underlying the formation of delusions. Australian J. Parapsychol. 12, 7–21.

Google Scholar

Irwin, H. J., Dagnall, N., and Drinkwater, K. (2012b). Paranormal beliefs and cognitive processes underlying the formation of delusions. Australian J. Parapsychol. 12, 107–126.

Google Scholar

Irwin, H. J., and Young, J. M. (2002). Intuitive versus reflective processes in the formation of paranormal beliefs. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 17, 45–53.

Google Scholar

Kees, J., Berry, C., Burton, S., and Sheehan, K. (2017). An analysis of data quality: professional panels, student subject pools, and Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. J. Advertising 46, 141–155. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2016.1269304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnaveni, R., and Deepa, R. (2013). Controlling common method variance while measuring the impact of emotional intelligence on well-being. Vikalpa 38, 41–48. doi: 10.1177/0256090920130104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krummenacher, P., Mohr, C., Haker, H., and Brugger, P. (2010). Dopamine, paranormal belief, and the detection of meaningful stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1670–1681. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, K., and Gupta, P. (2017). Impact of age and decision making on false memory. J. Psychosoc. Res. 12, 381–389.

Google Scholar

Lange, R., Irwin, H. J., and Houran, J. (2000). Top-down purification of Tobacyk’s revised paranormal belief scale. Person. Individ. Differ. 29, 131–156. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00183-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindeman, M., and Svedholm, A. M. (2012). What’s in a term? Paranormal, superstitious, magical and supernatural beliefs by any other name would mean the same. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 16, 241–255. doi: 10.1037/a0027158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindeman, M., Svedholm, A. M., Riekki, T., Raij, T., and Hari, R. (2013). Is it just a brick wall or a sign from the universe? An fMRI study of supernatural believers and skeptics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 943–949. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., and Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika 88, 767–778. doi: 10.1093/biomet/88.3.767

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logue, S. F., and Gould, T. J. (2014). The neural and genetic basis of executive function: attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 123, 45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lustig, C., May, C. P., and Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 130, 199–207. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mack, A. (2003). Inattentional blindness: looking without seeing. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 12, 180–184. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muramoto, O. (2004). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in human religious activity. Med. Hypotheses 62, 479–485. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2003.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus User’s Guide Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables, 7th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.

Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., and Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev. 84, 231–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. R., Hughes, B., McRae, K., Cooper, J. C., Weber, J., et al. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1322–1331.

Google Scholar

Ott, T., and Nieder, A. (2019). Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 213–234. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, J., and Neppe, V. M. (2004). Exploratory analyses of refined predictors of subjective ESP experiences and temporal lobe dysfunction in a neuropsychiatric population. Eur. J. Parapsychol. 19, 44–65.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramaswamy, V., DeSarbo, W. S., Reibstein, D. J., and Robinson, W. T. (1993). An empirical pooling approach for estimating marketing mix elasticities with PIMS data. Marketing Sci. 12, 103–124. doi: 10.1287/mksc.12.1.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, A., Hellgren, M. G., and French, C. C. (2014). Inattentional blindness, absorption, working memory capacity, and paranormal belief. Psychol. Consciousness: Theory Res. Pract. 1, 60–69. doi: 10.1037/css0000003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., Aleneff, M., Halme, A., and Nuortimo, A. (2013). Paranormal and religious believers are more prone to illusory face perception than skeptics and non-believers. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 27, 150–155. doi: 10.1002/acp.2874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., and Raij, T. T. (2014). Supernatural believers attribute more intentions to random movement than skeptics: an fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 400–411. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.906366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rim, J. I., Ojeda, J. C., Svob, C., Kayser, J., Drews, E., Kim, Y., et al. (2019). Current understanding of religion, spirituality, and their neurobiological correlates. Harvard Rev. Psychiatry 27, 303–316. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rominger, C., Fink, A., Weiss, E. M., Schulter, G., Perchtold, C. M., and Papousek, I. (2019). The propensity to perceive meaningful coincidences is associated with increased posterior alpha power during retention of information in a modified Sternberg paradigm. Consciousness Cogn. 76:102832. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rominger, C., Weiss, E. M., Fink, A., Schulter, G., and Papousek, I. (2011). Allusive thinking (cognitive looseness) and the propensity to perceive “meaningful” coincidences. Person. Individ. Differ. 51, 1002–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royle, J., and Lincoln, N. B. (2008). The everyday memory questionnaire–revised: development of a 13-item scale. Disabil. Rehabil. 30, 114–121. doi: 10.1080/09638280701223876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salak, B., Lindberg, K., Kienast, F., and Hunziker, M. (2021). Hybrid choice model dataset of a representative Swiss online panel survey on peoples’ preferences related to mixed renewable energy scenarios in landscapes and the effect of landscape-technology fit. Data Brief 36:107025. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2021.107025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schjoedt, U., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., and Bulbulia, J. (2013). Cognitive resource depletion in religious interactions. Religion Brain Behav. 3, 39–55. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2012.736714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmeichel, B. J., and Tang, D. (2015). Individual differences in executive functioning and their relationship to emotional processes and responses. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 24, 93–98. doi: 10.1177/0963721414555178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sclove, S. L. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52, 333–343. doi: 10.1007/BF02294360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaner, A. (1999). Delusions, superstitious conditioning and chaotic dopamine neurodynamics. Med. Hypotheses 52, 119–123. doi: 10.1054/mehy.1997.0656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spector, P. E. (2019). Do not cross me: optimizing the use of cross-sectional designs. J. Bus. Psychol. 34, 125–137. doi: 10.1007/s10869-018-09613-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics, 4th Edn. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Tobacyk, J. J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. Int. J. Transpersonal Stud. 23, 94–98. doi: 10.24972/ijts.2004.23.1.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallat-Azouvi, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., and Azouvi, P. (2012). The working memory questionnaire: a scale to assess everyday life problems related to deficits of working memory in brain injured patients. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 22, 634–649. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2012.681110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M. (2017). The self-attribution bias and paranormal beliefs. Consciousness Cogn. 100, 313–321. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., and Aleman, A. (2017). Brain mechanisms in religion and spirituality: an integrative predictive processing framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 73, 359–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, C., Denovan, A., Drinkwater, K., and Dagnall, N. (2021). Thinking style and paranormal belief: the role of cognitive biases. Imaginat. Cogn. Person. doi: 10.1177/02762366211036435 (Advance online publication).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Cunningham, W. A. (2007). “Executive function: mechanisms underlying emotion regulation,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, ed. J. J. Gross (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 135–158.

Google Scholar

Keywords: paranormal experience/belief, executive functions, latent profile analysis, self-report measures, metacognitive processes

Citation: Drinkwater KG, Dagnall N, Denovan A, Parker A and Escolà-Gascón Á (2022) Paranormal Experience Profiles and Their Association With Variations in Executive Functions: A Latent Profile Analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:778312. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.778312

Received: 17 September 2021; Accepted: 06 December 2021;
Published: 10 January 2022.

Edited by:

Hui-Jie Li, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China

Reviewed by:

Chuanhua Gu, Central China Normal University, China
Siman Liu, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China

Copyright © 2022 Drinkwater, Dagnall, Denovan, Parker and Escolà-Gascón. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kenneth Graham Drinkwater, ay5kcmlua3dhdGVyQG1tdS5hYy51aw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.