Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 November 2021
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Childhood Adversity and Life-course Consequences View all 10 articles

Cumulative Childhood Adversity and Its Associations With Mental Health in Childhood, Adolescence, and Adulthood in Rural China

  • Department of Sociology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Sha Tin, Hong Kong SAR, China

Capitalizing on a 15-year longitudinal dataset of 9–12 years old children in rural China, this study adopts a life course perspective and analyzes cumulative childhood adversity and its associations with mental health problems from childhood to adulthood. Four domains of childhood life are selected to construct cumulative childhood adversity: socioeconomic hardship, family disruption, physical issue, and academic setback. Overall, cumulative childhood adversity significantly associates with children’s internalizing and externalizing problems as well as adults’ depression and self-esteem. However, cumulative childhood adversity has no significant relationship with internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence. Furthermore, different domains of childhood adversity matter differently for mental health problems in different life stages. Physical issue and academic setback have the strongest association with internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood, while only socioeconomic hardship has a significant relationship with depression and self-esteem in adulthood. The relationship between cumulative childhood adversity and adult mental health problems is fully mediated by educational attainment. Finally, there is no gender difference in either the occurrence of cumulative childhood adversity or the association between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health problems.

Introduction

Childhood adversity is a common challenge faced by children worldwide (McLaughlin, 2016). The prevalence of exposure to childhood adversity has been found to be 38–39% across high-, middle, and low-income countries (Kessler et al., 2010). Much research also demonstrates that adversity in childhood could have lifelong costs that impair learning, behavior, and health (Green et al., 2010; Shonkoff et al., 2012; Appleton et al., 2017; Edalati et al., 2017; Jakubowski et al., 2018; You et al., 2019). Despite the extensive amount of evidence linking childhood adversity to negative health consequences, debates and inconsistencies still remain regarding the definitions, measurement, and time-varying consequences of childhood adversity (Oh et al., 2018a), particularly when the contexts in which childhood adversity is defined vary.

Most of the existing studies of childhood adversity focus on western countries (Björkenstam et al., 2019), perhaps because of the richness of data in these countries. However, the social contexts of developing countries could be sharply different from those in western countries. Even within developing countries, there is a high degree of heterogeneity in social contexts. Therefore, whether the findings obtained from particular countries in the existing literature still hold in other countries remains unclear. This uncertainty calls for more empirical studies based on developing countries, particularly the longitudinal studies that reveal the complex relationships between cumulative childhood adversity and child development outcomes over a long time span.

In China, there have been some longitudinal studies about early exposure to adversity and subsequent developmental outcomes. For example, Chen et al. (2012) examine the reciprocal direct and indirect effects among aggression, peer relationships, and depression based on a sample of 1,162 children in Beijing from the third grade to the sixth grade (ages 9–12). They find that children’s initial aggression and peer isolation positively contribute to later depression, which suggests that early adverse social-behavioral conditions exacerbate children’s developmental problems. Similarly, relying on the life history framework, Lu and Chang (2019) analyze a longitudinal sample of 198 rural adolescents in China. They find that adverse environmental factors, such as parental separation, are positively associated with subsequent aggression and risk-taking behaviors.

Based on a 6-year longitudinal sample of 1,245 adolescents from 9 counties including China, Chang et al. (2019) investigate how environmental harshness and unpredictability (measured by unsafe neighborhood, negative life events, family chaos, and family income change) affects adolescents’ developmental outcomes. Results show that environmental harshness and unpredictability longitudinally corresponds to more externalizing problems and lower academic performance, and such results are invariant to countries. In another study of 206 Chinese adolescents in rural areas, Chang and Lu (2018) examine the associations of family risk factors (such as stress, parental absence, and exposure to mortality and morbidity) and subsequent psychosocial outcomes after 18 months. Results demonstrate that family risk factors are significantly linked to more risky behaviors as well as academic underperformance.

In the Chinese context, while such studies provide valuable insights on the associations between early adversity and later developmental outcomes, due to data constraints, such longitudinal studies are mainly focused on childhood and adolescence. It remains unclear how the influences of early adversity will vary or persist over a long time span from childhood to adulthood. Thus, more research is needed to reveal the long-term associations between childhood adversity and developmental outcomes not only in childhood and adolescence but also in adulthood.

Drawing on a 15-year longitudinal dataset of children in rural China, this study tries to fill these gaps. I will examine the cumulative adversity, its different domains, and its associations with mental health problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. The following sections will first lay the theoretical background for research questions, and then describe the data and methods. After presenting the empirical results, this paper concludes with a discussion.

Theoretical Background

Definition and Measurement of (Cumulative) Childhood Adversity

Childhood adversity is often defined as the adverse childhood experiences or even trauma that could impair children’s health and wellbeing over a long time period (e.g., Patterson et al., 2014; Reid et al., 2017; Racine et al., 2020). Since adversity might take place in different aspects of life, researchers have developed many comprehensive measures, such as Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs), to capture the complexity of childhood adversity. While some have used the weighting methods to differentiate the importance of various adverse events, “weighting of events, either through regression-based techniques or by independent judges, does not typically improve correlations with outcomes” (Turner and Butler, 2003, p. 95). Also, “previous findings have suggested that the number of adverse events within a specified time period is more important than the novelty or types of events, and that adolescents are at greatest risk when simultaneously experiencing multiple adaptive challenges” (Davidson and Adams, 2013, p. 534). Therefore, cumulative childhood adversity, which is a composite score of a series of adverse experiences, has been extensively used in the existing research (e.g., Clark et al., 2010; Ford et al., 2011; Tan et al., 2017; Danielson and Sanders, 2018).

Cumulative childhood adversity covers a wide range of childhood life, which should include at least four domains. First, cumulative childhood adversity is most frequently measured by family disruption or dysfunction such as parental separation, divorce, abuse, or neglect (Schilling et al., 2008; Clark et al., 2010; Danielson and Sanders, 2018; Edalati et al., 2020). Second, it is also often measured by household low socioeconomic status such as economic hardship (Surtees and Wainwright, 2007; Benjet et al., 2009; Tan et al., 2017) and parents’ low levels of education and occupation (Wheaton et al., 1997; Yazgan et al., 2021). In addition to these two domains, another domain that could be integrated into cumulative childhood adversity is physical issue since physical health problems could cause huge or even lifelong stress on children. For example, in the existing literature, cumulative childhood adversity has incorporated such measures of children’s physical conditions as hospitalization, chronic disease, or poor health (Turner and Lloyd, 1995; Wheaton et al., 1997; Surtees and Wainwright, 2007; Davidson and Adams, 2013; Shen et al., 2017). Finally, educational adversity also contributes to cumulative childhood adversity since schooling and education comprises a critical part of childhood life. Children “spend more time in school than any other setting except their bed” (Eccles and Roeser, 2011, p. 225). Also, poor academic performance could result in tremendous mental distress among students (Chen et al., 2000; Roeser and Eccles, 2000; Davidson and Adams, 2013; Huang, 2015). For instance, research has showed that children with poor academic performance feel more pressure from parents, receive more criticism from teachers, and get less friendliness from peers (Shen, 2020). For these reasons, the domain of education deserves consideration when constructing cumulative childhood adversity.

While cumulative childhood adversity provides a convenient tool to investigate the consequences of overall adverse experiences in childhood, a single score might mask some critical, differential information between different types of adversity. Some research has shown that “categorization based on adversity type did appear to result in varying strength of association between each index and mental health outcomes” (Schilling et al., 2008, p. 1141). Therefore, in addition to cumulative childhood adversity, its different domains also need to be examined, which will reveal whether these different domains have equal or differential influences on child development.

Health Consequences of Cumulative Childhood Adversity

Research to date has demonstrated a series of health and developmental problems associated with cumulative childhood adversity. For example, a meta-analysis of 35 studies shows that cumulative childhood adversity corresponds to delays in cognitive development, infection, and sleep disruption at age 20 (Oh et al., 2018b). Cumulative childhood adversity is also linked to mental health problems in later life periods. Such mental health problems might include anxiety, depression, eating disorders, self-harm behaviors, internalizing problems, externalizing problems, antisocial behavior, and personality disorder, among others (Turner and Butler, 2003; Schilling et al., 2008; Putnam et al., 2013; McLaughlin, 2016; Björkenstam et al., 2017, 2021; Steine et al., 2017; Hébert et al., 2018).

Undoubtedly, there has been an extensive amount of evidence showing the detrimental effects of cumulative childhood adversity on adolescents’ and adults’ mental health. In contrast, much less evidence is presented in the existing literature regarding whether such detrimental effects of childhood adversity change across different life stages and whether different domains of childhood adversity contribute equally to mental health in different life periods. More research is needed to address these issues.

The Mediating Role of Educational Attainment and Moderating Role of Gender

In addition to mental health problems, research to date has found that cumulative childhood adversity impairs academic functioning like doing homework, staying calm, and curiosity in learning (Tan et al., 2017). Childhood adversity might also lower status attainment such as educational attainment (Haas, 2006; Shen et al., 2017). Some research has pointed out that “childhood adversity is associated with worse outcomes through lower adult socioeconomic status” (Jakubowski et al., 2018, p. 702). Since educational attainment is a critical component of adult socioeconomic status (e.g., King and Bearman, 2011) and also closely related to mental health (e.g., Esch et al., 2014; Mojtabai et al., 2015), it is necessary to investigate whether educational attainment mediates the relationship between childhood adversity and mental health problems in adulthood.

Furthermore, due to historical and cultural reasons, girls are often a particularly vulnerable group compared with boys. For instance, girls are more likely than boys to have abuse experiences (Mathews et al., 2017) and suffer depression (Kessler, 2003). Research on childhood adversity also finds that girls are more likely than boys to experience childhood adversity (Baglivio et al., 2014). Despite such gender differences in childhood adversity and mental health, whether girls and boys have the same relationship between childhood adversity and mental health over life periods remain unclear.

Research Questions

As many previous studies (e.g., Schilling et al., 2008; Montez and Hayward, 2014), the present study adopts a life course perspective to investigate how childhood adversity relates to mental health over a long life course from childhood to adulthood. The theoretical framework is illustrated in Figure 1. Capitalizing on a 15-year longitudinal dataset that traced the development of children from childhood to early adulthood in rural China, I will investigate the following research questions:

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the life-course theoretical framework.

1. What are the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and children’s mental health over a long time span including childhood, adolescence, and adulthood?

2. Do different domains of childhood adversity have similar or differential weights (or effects) on children’s short-term and long-term mental health?

3. Could educational attainment, as a critical marker of socioeconomic status, mediate the potential association between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in adulthood?

4. Are there gender differences in the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and children’s short-term and long-term mental health?

Materials and Methods

Participants

The data came from the Gansu Survey of Children and Families (GSCF, 2000, 2004, 2015). GSCF is a longitudinal study of 2,000 children in 100 rural villages in Gansu. Gansu was a rural province in Northwest China where over 60% of the population resided in rural areas in 2010 (Shen et al., 2021). The percentage of rural population was even higher when the data was first collected in the year 2000. Thus, although the sample was not a representative sample of all children in China, they came from a less developed and more impoverished area and were more likely to experience adversity than the average Chinese children. Moreover, to my knowledge, this is the only available longitudinal data in China that has focused on child development an education and traced their life trajectories from childhood to adulthood in 15 years. For this reason, the data was suitable for this study.

The sampled children were first interviewed between ages 9 and 12 in the year 2000, and last interviewed in early adulthood in the year 2015. In the 2000 and 2004 waves, questionnaires were administered at schools and in homes to children, teachers, school principals, mothers, and household heads. In 2015, to reduce non-response and attrition due to out-migration, interviews were conducted in family homes during Chinese New Year, when out-migrants were likely to return home for family reunions during the festival. For respondents who did not return home during the festival, basic demographic and education information from the household head and self-reported information through phone interviews were collected. This resulted in 1,613 out of the initial 2,000 respondents successfully followed in 2015. In the 2000 and 2004 waves, both children’s internalizing and externalizing problems were measured, while in 2015, the now-adults’ depression and self-esteem were measured as the indicators of their mental health.

Measures

In the present study, childhood adversity consisted of four domains that covered a wide range of childhood life experience: socioeconomic hardship, family disruption, physical issue, and academic setback. Each domain included three binary indicators with 1 denoting a particular adversity and 0 denoting the lack of that particular adversity. All adversity indicators were measured in the year 2000.

Socioeconomic hardship measured the socioeconomic status of a household. In extensive research, socioeconomic status comprises three indicators—education, income, and occupation (e.g., Kessler, 1982; Duncan et al., 2002; Baker, 2014; Ayoub et al., 2018). As the sample were drawn from rural areas where all children’s parents were farmers, their occupation had no variation and thus only education and income were utilized for socioeconomic hardship. Specifically, socioeconomic hardship was measured by maternal education, paternal education, and income insufficiency. Back to 2000 in the study site, the educational levels of most parents were really low. For instance, in the sample, 51% of mothers and 24% of fathers received no education at all. Thus, no education instead of a low level of education (such as primary education) was selected as the indicator of adversity. A parent who received no education was coded as 1 which indicated adversity for children. Back to the year 2000 in rural Gansu, many households still suffered from poverty and some even relied on borrowing money to afford children’s education (Shen and Hannum, 2020). Therefore, income insufficiency was another measure of socioeconomic hardship. It was obtained from the question “whether your household income in the past year was sufficient” in mother’s questionnaire. An answer of “no” was coded as 1 for income insufficiency.

Family disruption has been found to have long-term negative influences on children’s status attainment and mental health (e.g., Biblarz and Raftery, 1993; Gilman et al., 2003; Steele et al., 2009). In the existing research, family disruption is defined as the “discontinuation of cohabitation between the child’s biological parents” (Eriksen et al., 2017, p. 1080). It often includes the separation and divorce of parents (e.g., Somers et al., 2011). In GSCF, family disruption measured both the formal and informal separations of parents. Formal separation denoted the dissolution of marriage such as divorce, while informal separation denoted the separation due to migration. Therefore, family disruption included three indicators: maternal migration, paternal migration, and parental divorce. A parent was defined as being in migration if he or she was absent from home for at least 6 months in the past year (Graham and Jordan, 2011; Sun and Wang, 2016; Viet Nguyen, 2016).

Physical issue denoted the physiological problems that may directly or potentially harm children’s physical, emotional, and academic functioning. In GSCF, physical issue was measured by whether children had any chronic disease, insufficient breakfast, or myopia. Children who cannot have enough breakfast, either because of poverty or because of eating habit, tend to have low protein intake (Chitra and Reddy, 2007). “Protein malnutrition is prevalent in the developing parts of the world and children are the most affected” (Navam et al., 2014, p. 1). In China, research has shown that children who had no or little breakfast would have higher risks for malnutrition (Li et al., 2018). In GSCF, 15% of sampled children reported that they could not have breakfast enough to feel full. In this context, insufficient breakfast was taken as a marker of adversity since it was strongly related to malnutrition of children. Myopia among children and youth has become a global problem (Leo, 2017). Yet it is rarely considered as adversity because glasses to correct myopia are often easily accessible and affordable, particularly in urban areas. However, in the context of impoverished rural areas in this study, people either lacked the awareness or had insufficient financial resources to correct for children’s myopia. For instance, in this study, 20% of the sampled children reported that they had difficulties in reading blackboard or doing homework due to short-sightedness. For this reason, myopia was included as an indicator of adversity.

Academic setback denoted the problems and difficulties children encountered in their schooling and education, such as poor school performance (Davidson and Adams, 2013; Shen et al., 2017). This was measured by two questions in the data: whether children perceived their language (i.e., Chinese) performance was very poor and whether they perceived their math performance was very poor. An answer of “yes” was coded as 1 to denote a particular educational adversity. Furthermore, some children might experience grade retention. Grade retention, often due to falling behind peers, could result in a series of mental health problems such as low self-concept, misconduct in school, and depressive symptoms (Demanet and Van Houtte, 2013; Klapproth et al., 2016; Hu and Hannum, 2020). It was also adopted as an indicator of cumulative adversity in past research (e.g., Wheaton et al., 1997). Thus, grade retention was also included as a third marker of educational adversity.

These measures of (cumulative) childhood adversity, although together describing the total adverse events children encountered in particular domains of childhood life, were not necessarily correlated with each other. For instance, a child with poor math performance might not have any chronic disease, and poor math performance was also not indicative of whether parents were divorced. Therefore, Cronbach’s reliability score was not calculated for adversity measures. This is consistent with the practice in many existing studies (e.g., Schilling et al., 2008; Montez and Hayward, 2014; Björkenstam et al., 2018). In fact, the reliability of adversity measures concerns more about whether the reported adversity was truly experienced by children. Many past studies adopted retrospective data on childhood adversity and showed a good reliability (Surtees and Wainwright, 2007). Childhood adversity in GSCF were all measured in the year 2000 when children were still in childhood and when such events just took place. Thus, those adversity measures should be more reliable than the retrospective recalls.

In 2000 and 2004, children’s mental health was measured by their internalizing and externalizing behavioral problems. Internalizing and externalizing problems were measured by a set of questions adapted from the Youth-Self Report (Achenbach, 1991), edited through local piloting for cultural relevance (Liu, 2003). Each type of behavioral problems contained 18 questions, and each question was rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1–4: totally disagree, disagree somewhat, agree somewhat, and fully agree). A summative score was calculated for each type of behavioral problems, with a higher score implying more behavioral problems. For internalizing problems, the Cronbach’s alpha scores in 2000 and 2004 were 0.834 and 0.802, respectively. For externalizing problems, the Cronbach’s alpha scores in 2000 and 2004 were both 0.870. In 2000, the sampled children were in their late childhood. 4 years later in 2004, they entered their adolescence. Thus, the 2000 and 2004 measures of behavioral problems depicted their mental health from childhood to adolescence.

In 2015, the respondents’ mental health was measured by their depression and self-esteem. Depression in 2015 was measured by a shortened (10-item) form of the CES-D scale, validated and used in many studies (Andresen et al., 1994; Baron et al., 2017; González et al., 2017). For each question, a Likert scale of 1–4 denoted never, occasionally, sometimes, and often for the frequency of each depressive symptom. Thus, higher values indicated a higher likelihood or level of depression. The Cronbach’s reliability score was 0.737. Self-esteem in 2015 was measured by the widely used and validated 10-item Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (e.g., Schmitt and Allik, 2005; Zhang and Kong, 2021). Each item was rated on a 4-point Likert scale denoting totally disagree, disagree somewhat, agree somewhat, and fully agree. A summative score was calculated and a higher score indicated a higher level of self-esteem. The Cronbach’s reliability score was 0.722.

Educational attainment in 2015 was measured by the total years of education that children had attained, transformed from a categorical variable of children’s highest level of education. According to children’s age, all sampled children should have completed formal education by 2015. Thus, the 2015 measure represented their stable or fixed educational attainment. In addition, children’s age in 2000 and gender (1 for female and 0 for male) were used as control variables. Children’s age was calculated by taking the difference between the survey year 2000 and children’s birth year reported in the household questionnaire.1 The descriptive statistics for all variables are listed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Statistical Analysis

This study utilized structural equation models (SEM) for statistical analysis. SEM has the following advantages. First, it can estimate how childhood adversity corresponds to different mental health problems simultaneously. Second, it has a convenient and powerful technique of handling missing data, i.e., the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) method. Different from the conventional multiple imputation method, the results estimated by FIML are unaffected by the imputation model and also asymptotically efficient (Allison, 2015). Third, the multiple group analysis in SEM can estimate the same model for different subgroups simultaneously, which facilitates the comparison between girls and boys for the relationships between childhood adversity and mental health outcomes. Given these advantages, SEM is a preferred method for this study.

For each life stage (e.g., childhood, adolescence, and adulthood), a model with mental health problems as endogenous variables was estimated. In each model, the error terms of the two types of mental health problems in the same life stage were also correlated to indicate that they could be both affected by some unobserved factors. Age in 2000 and gender were included as control variables (except that gender was not included as a control variable for the gender difference model). For each life stage, another model that used the four domains of childhood adversity was also estimated in order to show the differential contributions of these four domains.

Next, educational attainment was added to the adulthood model to examine whether it mediates the relationship between childhood adversity and adults’ mental health. As for gender differences, the same model was estimated for both girls and boys, and then their parameters were compared. The childhood and adolescence models used all the 2,000 observations. In 2015, only 1,613 respondents were followed, and thus the adulthood model only analyzed these 1,613 observations. FIML was used for parameter estimation and the handling of missing data. All models were saturated models and thus no goodness-of-fit test statistics were reported. To adjust for non-normality in dependent variables, robust standard errors were calculated for all models (Chou et al., 1991; Hu et al., 1992). All the SEM models were estimated by STATA 17.

Results

Table 1 depicts the descriptive statistics. The average of cumulative childhood is 2.022, which indicates that the overall frequency of childhood adversity is not high. The most common childhood adversity is socioeconomic hardship. This is not surprising given that the sample was collected in rural areas. Academic setback is the second most frequent childhood adversity. This result illustrates the importance of incorporating educational adversity into the construct of childhood adversity when children spend so much time in school.

Table 2 and Figure 2 show that after adjusting for children’s age and gender, cumulative childhood adversity is significantly associated with both internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood. However, when childhood adversity is decomposed to four domains, not all domains matter for children’s behavioral problems. Academic setback has the strongest association with children’s behavioral problems. The second strongest association exists between physical issue and children’s behavioral problems. In contrast, socioeconomic hardship and family disruption have no significant relationship with children’s behavioral problems in childhood.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Maximum likelihood estimates of the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in childhood.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of the associations between childhood adversity and childhood mental health in childhood. Age and gender are not shown in this figure for simplicity. Numbers on arrows are unstandardized regression coefficients. * < 0.05, ** < 0.01, *** < 0.001.

When children become adolescents, the results are listed in Table 3 and Figure 3. Surprisingly, neither cumulative childhood adversity nor any of its four domains measured in childhood has a significant relationship with behavioral problems in adolescence. One particular reason might be that when children enter adolescence and middle school, their living environments change dramatically and thus their behavioral problems also change. Such sharp changes render the previous significant relationship between childhood adversity and behavioral problems no longer significant.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Maximum likelihood estimates of the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in adolescence.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Illustration of the associations between childhood adversity and childhood mental health in adolescence. Age and gender are not shown in this figure for simplicity. Numbers on arrows are unstandardized regression coefficients. * < 0.05, ** < 0.01, *** < 0.001.

Table 4 and Figure 4 show that more cumulative childhood adversity is significantly associated with higher depression and lower self-esteem in adulthood. Yet, this significant association mainly results from socioeconomic hardship in childhood. Moreover, more academic setback in childhood significantly reduces adult self-esteem but not adult depression.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Maximum likelihood estimates of the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in adulthood.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Illustration of the associations between childhood adversity and childhood mental health in adulthood. Age and gender are not shown in this figure for simplicity. Numbers on arrows are unstandardized regression coefficients. * < 0.05, ** < 0.01, *** < 0.001.

The significant association between cumulative childhood adversity and adult mental health problems in Table 4 seems to contradict the results in Table 3. If the effect of childhood adversity on mental health has diminished in adolescence, how could the effect become significant in adulthood? Of course, one reason could be that the measures of mental health problems in adolescence and adulthood used in this study are not exactly the same. But another reason could be the mediating role of educational attainment.

As shown in Table 5 and Figure 5, after educational attainment is included in the model, cumulative childhood adversity no longer has a direct, significant relationship with either depression or self-esteem in adulthood. Rather, cumulative childhood adversity has significant, indirect effects on adult depression and self-esteem via educational attainment. Such indirect effect sizes are comparable to the effect sizes identified in Table 4 (i.e., the adulthood model without educational attainment).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Educational attainment as a mediator of cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in adulthood.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Illustration of educational attainment as a mediator of cumulative childhood adversity and mental health in adulthood. Age and gender are not shown in this figure for simplicity. Numbers on arrows are unstandardized regression coefficients. * < 0.05, ** < 0.01, *** < 0.001.

Finally, Table 6 describes the gender differences in the relationships between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health problems across different life stages. Consistent with previous models in each life stage, for both girls and boys, cumulative childhood adversity is significantly linked to more behavioral problems in childhood, as well as high depression and low self-esteem in adulthood, but is not significantly related to behavioral problems in adolescence. More importantly, there are no significant gender differences in the relationships between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health problems across different life stages. (The significant gender difference in the association between cumulative childhood adversity and externalizing problems in 2004 is more like a statistical artifact since the coefficients for both girls and boys are non-significant.) A further analysis shows that there is no gender difference in the occurrence of cumulative childhood adversity either (t-test p-value = 0.157).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Gender differences in the associations between cumulative childhood adversity and mental health.

Discussion

This study focuses on cumulative childhood adversity and examines its associations with mental health problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood, capitalizing on a unique, 15-year longitudinal dataset in rural China. Consistent with previous literature (e.g., Turner and Butler, 2003; McLaughlin, 2016; Steine et al., 2017), cumulative childhood adversity is associated with various mental health problems in both short and long terms. However, such associations vary across different life stages and different domains of childhood adversity.

Different from a 45-year study of birth cohorts in Britain that finds unattenuated effects of childhood adversity on psychological problems over time (Clark et al., 2010), this study in China shows that cumulative childhood adversity has no significant relationship with behavioral problems in adolescence, although that relationship is significant at first in childhood. Indeed, the samples of these two studies are not comparable. But the different results regarding the time-varying effects of childhood adversity again reminded us of the importance of social contexts in which childhood adversity is studied.

Furthermore, this study shows the different values of childhood adversity as a summative score and as separate domains. As a cumulative score, it facilitates the discovery of the overall effect of childhood adversity. As separate domains, childhood adversity could reveal the unequal effects of different types of childhood adversity at different times. For example, this study finds that in childhood, physical issue and academic setback have the strongest associations with behavioral problems while in adulthood only socioeconomic hardship matters. Similarly, another study finds that family dysfunction and maltreat have different effects, with the former being more significant in early childhood and the latter being more significant in adolescence (Oh et al., 2018b). These pieces of evidence demonstrate that we cannot simply understand childhood adversity as a homogenous set of adverse experiences even if we continuously use cumulative childhood adversity.

This study also demonstrates that the effects of childhood adversity might be complex and implicit. In the long term, even if childhood adversity has no direct effect on adult mental health, it could indirectly affect adult mental health through educational attainment. This result also illuminates the importance of including education in the examination of childhood adversity. Previous research has found that among different types of childhood adversity, dropping out or failing out of school has one of the strongest effect on adult’s health (Kuhlman et al., 2018) and low school grades further heighten the risk for mental disorders among those who have experienced childhood adversity (Björkenstam et al., 2017). Similar findings are also observed in this present study. For example, academic setback has a strong association with not only children’s behavioral problems but also adults’ self-esteem 15 years later. Therefore, education—whether taken as a component of childhood adversity or a mediating or moderating role of childhood adversity’s effect—needs to be considered for the study of childhood adversity.

Some previous studies have found gender differences in childhood adversity experiences and mental health problems (e.g., Kessler, 2003; Mathews et al., 2017). However, this is not the case in the present study. In the present study, no gender difference is found in either the occurrence of childhood adversity or the association between childhood adversity and mental health problems. This further illustrates the complex relations between childhood adversity and mental health in varying contexts, and calls for more studies on the gender differences regarding both childhood adversity and its health consequences.

Of course, the differences between findings in the present study and those in other research might result from different samples. Admittedly, the sample analyzed in this study was drawn from rural China and might not be representative of the Chinese children. Thus, the results in the present study cannot be simply generalized to a different social setting. As pointed out in the introduction, more studies based on empirical evidence from developing countries are needed in order to better reveal the influences of childhood adversity in different contexts.

Another limitation in this study is that it has identified the associations between childhood adversity and mental health problems in different life stages, yet such associations are not causations. Thus, whether childhood adversity causes such mental health problems or their associations are just a result of some unobserved confounders remains unclear. Future research could develop better research design to identify more causal interpretations of the complex relationships between childhood adversity and mental health problems.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Harvard University and the University of Pennsylvania. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

WS contributes to data analysis and manuscript writing.

Funding

The Gansu Survey of Children and Families (GSCF) was supported by the Hong Kong Research Grants Council (HSSPFS 36600014). Earlier waves of data were supported by the United Kingdom Economic and Social Research Council and Department for International Development (ESRC RES-167-25-0250), the Spencer Foundation Small and Major Grants Programs, the World Bank, and NIH Grants 1R01TW005930-01 and 5R01TW005930-02. The writing and submission of the manuscript was supported by the Improvement on Competitiveness in Hiring New Faculties Funding Scheme of the Chinese University of Hong Kong.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Due to this age calculation formula (i.e., 2000—birth year), some children aged 12 (e.g., those born in November of 1987 and interviewed in July of 2000) could be rounded up to 13. Also, due to some unknown errors in response (e.g., respondents might confuse the lunar year and solar year when reporting birth year) or data input, there were 6 observations whose calculated ages fell out of children’s true age range. The ages of these 6 observations were treated as missing values in this study. I also excluded these 6 observations from the sample for a robustness check. Whether the ages of 6 observations were treated as missing values or excluded from analytical sample, results are always consistent without any substantial change.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the youth self-report and 1991 profile. Department of Psychiatry. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.

Google Scholar

Allison, P. (2015). Maximum likelihood is better than multiple imputation: Part II. Statistical Horizons. Available online at: https://statisticalhorizons.com/ml-is-better-than-mi (accessed date 5 May 2015).

Google Scholar

Andresen, E. M., Malmgren, J. A., Carter, W. B., and Patrick, D. L. (1994). Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 10, 77–84.

Google Scholar

Appleton, A. A., Holdsworth, E., Ryan, M., and Tracy, M. (2017). Measuring childhood adversity in life course cardiovascular research: A systematic review. Psychos. Med. 79, 434–440. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayoub, M., Gosling, S. D., Potter, J., Shanahan, M., and Roberts, B. W. (2018). The relations between parental socioeconomic status, personality, and life outcomes. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 9, 338–352. doi: 10.1177/1948550617707018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baglivio, M. T., Epps, N., Swartz, K., Huq, M. S., Sheer, A., and Hardt, N. S. (2014). The prevalence of adverse childhood experiences (ACE) in the lives of juvenile offenders. J. Juven. Just. 3:2.

Google Scholar

Baker, E. H. (2014). “Socioeconomic status, definition,” in The Wiley Blackwell encyclopedia of health, illness, behavior, and society, eds W. C. Cockerham, R. Dingwall, and S. R. Quah (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell), 2210–2214.

Google Scholar

Baron, E. C., Davies, T., and Lund, C. (2017). Validation of the 10-item centre for epidemiological studies depression scale (CES-D-10) in Zulu, Xhosa and Afrikaans populations in South Africa. BMC Psychiatry 17:6. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1178-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjet, C., Borges, G., Medina-Mora, M. E., Zambrano, J., Cruz, C., and Méndez, E. (2009). Descriptive epidemiology of chronic childhood adversity in Mexican adolescents. J. Adol. Health 45, 483–489. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biblarz, T. J., and Raftery, A. E. (1993). The effects of family disruption on social mobility. Am. Soc. Rev. 1993, 97–109. doi: 10.2307/2096220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkenstam, E., Burström, B., Hjern, A., Vinnerljung, B., Kosidou, K., and Berg, L. (2019). Cumulative childhood adversity, adolescent psychiatric disorder and violent offending in young adulthood. Eur. J Public Health 29, 855–861. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckz089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkenstam, E., Ekselius, L., Burström, B., Kosidou, K., and Björkenstam, C. (2017). Association between childhood adversity and a diagnosis of personality disorder in young adulthood: a cohort study of 107,287 individuals in Stockholm County. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 32, 721–731. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0264-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkenstam, E., Helgesson, M., and Mittendorfer-Rutz, E. (2021). Childhood adversity and common mental disorders in young employees in Sweden: Is the association affected by early adulthood occupational class? Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. 56:237. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-01874-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkenstam, E., Hjern, A., Björkenstam, C., and Kosidou, K. (2018). Association of cumulative childhood adversity and adolescent violent offending with suicide in early adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry 75, 185–193. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L., and Lu, H. J. (2018). Resource and extrinsic risk in defining fast life histories of rural Chinese left-behind children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L., Lu, H. J., Lansford, J. E., Skinner, A. T., Bornstein, M. H., Steinberg, L., et al. (2019). Environmental harshness and unpredictability, life history, and social and academic behavior of adolescents in nine countries. Dev. Psychol. 55:890. doi: 10.1037/dev0000655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Chen, H., Kaspar, V., and Noh, S. (2000). Adolescent social, emotional, and school adjustment in mainland China. Internat. J. Group Tensions 29, 51–78. doi: 10.1023/A:1005178713623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Huang, X., Wang, L., and Chang, L. (2012). Aggression, peer relationships, and depression in Chinese children: A multiwave longitudinal study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 1233–1241. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02576.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chitra, U., and Reddy, C. R. (2007). The role of breakfast in nutrient intake of urban schoolchildren. Public Health Nutr. 10, 55–58. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007219640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, C.-P., Bentler, P. M., and Satorra, A. (1991). Scaled test statistics and robust standard errors for non-normal data in covariance structure analysis: A Monte Carlo study. Br. J. Mathemat. Stat. Psychol. 44, 347–357. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1991.tb00966.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C., Caldwell, T., Power, C., and Stansfeld, S. A. (2010). Does the influence of childhood adversity on psychopathology persist across the lifecourse? A 45-year prospective epidemiologic study. Ann. Epidemiol. 20, 385–394. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielson, R., and Sanders, G. F. (2018). An effective measure of childhood adversity that is valid with older adults. Child Abuse Neglect 82, 156–167. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, S., and Adams, J. (2013). Adversity and internalizing problems among rural Chinese adolescents: The roles of parents and teachers. Internat. J. Behav. Dev. 37, 530–541. doi: 10.1177/0165025413503421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demanet, J., and Van Houtte, M. (2013). Grade retention and its association with school misconduct in adolescence: A multilevel approach. School Effect. School Improv. 24, 417–434. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2012.727834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., Daly, M. C., McDonough, P., and Williams, D. R. (2002). Optimal indicators of socioeconomic status for health research. Am. J. Public Health 92, 1151–1157. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.92.7.1151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J. Res. Adoles. 21, 225–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00725.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edalati, H., Nicholls, T. L., Crocker, A. G., Roy, L., Somers, J. M., and Patterson, M. L. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of criminal justice involvement and victimization among homeless adults with mental illness. Psychiatric Serv. 68, 1288–1295. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201600330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edalati, H., Nicholls, T. L., Schütz, C. G., Somers, J. M., Distasio, J., Aubry, T., et al. (2020). Examining the relationships between cumulative childhood adversity and the risk of criminal justice involvement and victimization among homeless adults with mental illnesses after receiving housing first intervention. Can. J. Psychiatry 65, 409–417. doi: 10.1177/0706743720902616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, H.-L. F., Hvidtfeldt, C., and Lilleør, H. B. (2017). Family disruption and social, emotional and behavioral functioning in middle childhood. J. Child Family Stud. 26, 1077–1089. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0631-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esch, P., Bocquet, V., Pull, C., Couffignal, S., Lehnert, T., Graas, M., et al. (2014). The downward spiral of mental disorders and educational attainment: A systematic review on early school leaving. BMC Psychiatry 14:237.

Google Scholar

Ford, E., Clark, C., and Stansfeld, S. A. (2011). The influence of childhood adversity on social relations and mental health at mid-life. J. Affect. Dis. 133, 320–327. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilman, S. E., Kawachi, I., Fitzmaurice, G. M., and Buka, S. L. (2003). Family disruption in childhood and risk of adult depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 939–946. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González, P., Nuñez, A., Merz, E., Brintz, C., Weitzman, O., Navas, E. L., et al. (2017). Measurement properties of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (ces-d 10): Findings from Hchs/sol. Psychol. Assess. 29:372. doi: 10.1037/pas0000330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, E., and Jordan, L. P. (2011). Migrant Parents and the Psychological Well-being of Left-behind Children in Southeast Asia. J. Marr. Family 73, 763–787.

Google Scholar

Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archiv. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haas, S. A. (2006). Health selection and the process of social stratification: The effect of childhood health on socioeconomic attainment. J. Health Soc. Behav. 47, 339–354. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hébert, M., Langevin, R., and Oussaïd, E. (2018). Cumulative childhood trauma, emotion regulation, dissociation, and behavior problems in school-aged sexual abuse victims. J. Affect. Dis. 225, 306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., Bentler, P. M., and Kano, Y. (1992). Can test statistics in covariance structure analysis be trusted? Psychol. Bull. 112:351. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L.-C., and Hannum, E. (2020). Red flag: Grade retention and student academic and behavioral outcomes in China. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 113:104896. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.104896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. (2015). Academic achievement and subsequent depression: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. J. Child Family Stud. 24, 434–442. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9855-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubowski, K. P., Cundiff, J. M., and Matthews, K. A. (2018). Cumulative childhood adversity and adult cardiometabolic disease: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 37:701. doi: 10.1037/hea0000637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C. (1982). A disaggregation of the relationship between socioeconomic status and psychological distress. Am. Soc. Rev. 1982, 752–764. doi: 10.2307/2095211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C. (2003). Epidemiology of women and depression. J. Affect. Dis. 74, 5–13. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00426-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Br. J. Psychiatry 197, 378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, M. D., and Bearman, P. S. (2011). Socioeconomic status and the increased prevalence of autism in California. Am. Soc. Rev. 76, 320–346. doi: 10.1177/0003122411399389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klapproth, F., Schaltz, P., Brunner, M., Keller, U., Fischbach, A., Ugen, S., et al. (2016). Short-term and medium-term effects of grade retention in secondary school on academic achievement and psychosocial outcome variables. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 50, 182–194. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhlman, K. R., Robles, T. F., Bower, J. E., and Carroll, J. E. (2018). Screening for childhood adversity: The what and when of identifying individuals at risk for lifespan health disparities. J. Behav. Med. 41, 516–527. doi: 10.1007/s10865-018-9921-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leo, S. W. (2017). Current approaches to myopia control. Curr. Opin. Ophthal. 28, 267–275. doi: 10.1097/ICU.0000000000000367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Xu, P., Yang, T., Gan, Q., Cao, W., Pan, H., et al. (2018). Relationship between breakfast and nutrition status study of children aged 6-17 in China from 2010 to 2012. J. Hyg. Res. 47, 373–377.

Google Scholar

Liu, X. (2003). Parenting practices and the psychological adjustment of children in rural China. Gansu Surv. Child. Famil. Dis. 2003:2.

Google Scholar

Lu, H. J., and Chang, L. (2019). Aggression and risk-taking as adaptive implementations of fast life history strategy. Dev. Sci. 22:e12827. doi: 10.1111/desc.12827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, B., Bromfield, L., Walsh, K., Cheng, Q., and Norman, R. E. (2017). Reports of child sexual abuse of boys and girls: Longitudinal trends over a 20-year period in Victoria, Australia. Child Abuse Negl. 66, 9–22. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Future directions in childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. J. Clin. Child Adol. Psychol. 45, 361–382. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1110823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mojtabai, R., Stuart, E. A., Hwang, I., Eaton, W. W., Sampson, N., and Kessler, R. C. (2015). Long-term effects of mental disorders on educational attainment in the National Comorbidity Survey ten-year follow-up. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. 50, 1577–1591. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1083-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montez, J. K., and Hayward, M. D. (2014). Cumulative childhood adversity, educational attainment, and active life expectancy among US adults. Demography 51, 413–435. doi: 10.1007/s13524-013-0261-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navam, S. H., Tajudini, A. L., Srinivas, J. R., Sivarooban, T., and Kristofor, R. B. (2014). Physio-chemical and sensory properties of protein-fortified extruded breakfast cereal/snack formulated to combat protein malnutrition in developing countries. J. Food Proc. Technol. 5:1. doi: 10.4172/2157-7110.1000359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, D. L., Jerman, P., Boparai, S. K. P., Koita, K., Briner, S., Bucci, M., et al. (2018a). Review of tools for measuring exposure to adversity in children and adolescents. J. Pediatric Health Care 32, 564–583. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2018.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, D. L., Jerman, P., Marques, S. S., Koita, K., Boparai, S. K. P., Harris, N. B., et al. (2018b). Systematic review of pediatric health outcomes associated with childhood adversity. BMC Pediatr. 18, 1–19. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1037-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, M. L., Moniruzzaman, A., and Somers, J. M. (2014). Setting the stage for chronic health problems: Cumulative childhood adversity among homeless adults with mental illness in Vancouver, British Columbia. BMC Public Health 14, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, K. T., Harris, W. W., and Putnam, F. W. (2013). Synergistic childhood adversities and complex adult psychopathology. J. Traum. Stress 26, 435–442. doi: 10.1002/jts.21833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racine, N., Eirich, R., Dimitropoulos, G., Hartwick, C., and Madigan, S. (2020). Development of trauma symptoms following adversity in childhood: the moderating role of protective factors. Child Abuse Negl. 101:104375. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, J. A., Baglivio, M. T., Piquero, A. R., Greenwald, M. A., and Epps, N. (2017). Human trafficking of minors and childhood adversity in Florida. Am. J. Public Health 107, 306–311. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeser, R. W., and Eccles, J. S. (2000). “Schooling and mental health,” in Handbook of developmental psychopathology, eds A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, and S. M. Miller (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers), 135–156. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-4163-9_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilling, E. A., Aseltine, R. H., and Gore, S. (2008). The impact of cumulative childhood adversity on young adult mental health: Measures, models, and interpretations. Soc. Sci. Med. 66, 1140–1151. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P., and Allik, J. (2005). Simultaneous administration of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in 53 nations: Exploring the universal and culture-specific features of global self-esteem. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 89:623. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W. (2020). A tangled web: The reciprocal relationship between depression and educational outcomes in China. Soc. Sci. Res. 85:102353. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2019.102353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W., and Hannum, E. (2020). “Credit limits as an element of family socioeconomic status: An application to the case of children’s educational outcomes in rural Gansu province,” in Rural Education in China’s Social Transition, eds P. A. Kong, E. Hannum, and G. A. Postiglione (Milton Park: Routledge), 3–19.

Google Scholar

Shen, W., Hu, L.-C., and Hannum, E. (2017). Cumulative adversity, childhood behavioral problems, and educational mobility in China’s poorest rural communities. Chinese J. Soc. 3, 491–517. doi: 10.1177/2057150X17736664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W., Hu, L.-C., and Hannum, E. (2021). Effect pathways of informal family separation on children’s outcomes: Paternal labor migration and long-term educational attainment of left-behind children in rural China. Soc. Sci. Res. 97:102576. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2021.102576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., Siegel, B. S., Dobbins, M. I, Earls, M. F., McGuinn, L., et al. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 129, e232–e246. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somers, C. L., Chiodo, L. M., Yoon, J., Ratner, H., Barton, E., and Delaney-Black, V. (2011). Family disruption and academic functioning in urban, black youth. Psychol. Schools 48, 357–370. doi: 10.1002/pits.20559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, F., Sigle-Rushton, W., and Kravdal, Ø (2009). Consequences of family disruption on children’s educational outcomes in Norway. Demography 46, 553–574. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steine, I. M., Winje, D., Krystal, J. H., Bjorvatn, B., Milde, A. M., Grønli, J., et al. (2017). Cumulative childhood maltreatment and its dose-response relation with adult symptomatology: Findings in a sample of adult survivors of sexual abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 65, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, W., and Wang, Y. (2016). The effect of parental absence for work on children’s health: A micro panel data analysis. China Econ. Q. 15, 963–988.

Google Scholar

Surtees, P. G., and Wainwright, N. W. (2007). The shackles of misfortune: Social adversity assessment and representation in a chronic-disease epidemiological setting. Soc. Sci. Med. 64, 95–111. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, T. X., Wang, Y., and Ruggerio, A. D. (2017). Childhood adversity and children’s academic functioning: Roles of parenting stress and neighborhood support. J. Child Family Stud. 26, 2742–2752. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0775-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, H. A., and Butler, M. J. (2003). Direct and indirect effects of childhood adversity on depressive symptoms in young adults. J. Youth Adoles. 32, 89–103. doi: 10.1023/A:1021853600645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. J., and Lloyd, D. A. (1995). Lifetime traumas and mental health: The significance of cumulative adversity. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1995, 360–376. doi: 10.2307/2137325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viet Nguyen, C. (2016). Does parental migration really benefit left-behind children? Comparative evidence from Ethiopia, India, Peru and Vietnam. Soc. Sci. Med. 153, 230–239. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, B., Roszell, P., and Hall, K. (1997). “The impact of twenty childhood and adult traumatic stressors on the risk of psychiatric disorder,” in Stress and adversity over the life course: Trajectories and turning points, eds I. H. Gotlib and B. Wheaton (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press), 50–72.

Google Scholar

Yazgan, I., Hanson, J. L., Bates, J. E., Lansford, J. E., Pettit, G. S., and Dodge, K. A. (2021). Cumulative early childhood adversity and later antisocial behavior: The mediating role of passive avoidance. Dev. Psychop. 33, 340–350. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

You, D. S., Albu, S., Lisenbardt, H., and Meagher, M. W. (2019). Cumulative childhood adversity as a risk factor for common chronic pain conditions in young adults. Pain Med. 20, 486–494. doi: 10.1093/pm/pny106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., and Kong, P. A. (2021). Rural Chinese youth during the transition into adulthood: Family dynamics and psychological adjustment. Internat. J. Psychol. 2021:12749. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cumulative childhood adversity, internalizing problem, externalizing problem, depression, self-esteem

Citation: Shen W (2021) Cumulative Childhood Adversity and Its Associations With Mental Health in Childhood, Adolescence, and Adulthood in Rural China. Front. Psychol. 12:768315. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.768315

Received: 31 August 2021; Accepted: 11 October 2021;
Published: 03 November 2021.

Edited by:

Cheryl Zlotnick, University of Haifa, Israel

Reviewed by:

Jiajia Liu, Peking University, China
Lei Chang, University of Macau, China

Copyright © 2021 Shen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wensong Shen, d2Vuc29uZ3NoZW5AY3Voay5lZHUuaGs=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.