Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 05 January 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Effects of Daytime Electric Light Exposure on Human Alertness and Higher Cognitive Functions: A Systematic Review

  • 1Department of Psychology, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Monash University Malaysia, Subang Jaya, Malaysia
  • 2Department of Electrical and Computer Systems Engineering, School of Engineering, Monash University Malaysia, Subang Jaya, Malaysia
  • 3School of Medical and Life Sciences, Sunway University, Subang Jaya, Malaysia

This paper reports the results of a systematic review conducted on articles examining the effects of daytime electric light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions. For this, we selected 59 quantitative research articles from 11 online databases. The review protocol was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42020157603). The results showed that both short-wavelength dominant light exposure and higher intensity white light exposure induced alertness. However, those influences depended on factors like the participants’ homeostatic sleep drive and the time of day the participants received the light exposure. The relationship between light exposure and higher cognitive functions was not as straightforward as the alerting effect. The optimal light property for higher cognitive functions was reported dependent on other factors, such as task complexity and properties of control light. Among the studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure, ten studies (morning: 3; afternoon: 7) reported beneficial effects on simple task performances (reaction time), and four studies (morning: 3; afternoon: 1) on complex task performances. Four studies with higher intensity white light exposure (morning: 3; afternoon: 1) reported beneficial effects on simple task performance and nine studies (morning: 5; afternoon: 4) on complex task performance. Short-wavelength dominant light exposure with higher light intensity induced a beneficial effect on alertness and simple task performances. However, those effects did not hold for complex task performances. The results indicate the need for further studies to understand the influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure with higher illuminance on alertness and higher cognitive functions.

Introduction

Besides its role in vision, light also influences several physiological and psychological processes – known as non-image-forming (NIF) effects of light. The “Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage” (CIE) later adopted the term of “intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell (ipRGCs) influenced light (IIL) responses” (CIE, 2018) to characterize these effects. “Illuminating Engineering Society” (IES) categorized IIL responses as circadian, neuroendocrine and neurobehavioral responses (IES, 2018). Circadian responses entrain our internal 24-h clock to earth’s 24-h rotation (de Kort, 2019). Neuroendocrine responses control how the brain regulates hormone expression, especially melatonin secretion (Lockley, 2008; Cajochen and Chellappa, 2016). Neurobehavioral responses indicate the relationship between the nervous system and human behavior. They typically include psychological processes such as alertness (Lockley et al., 2006; Cajochen, 2007), mood (Xiao et al., 2021), cognitive performance (Alkozei et al., 2017) as well as different physiological states like heart rate (Cajochen et al., 2005; Askaripoor et al., 2018) and body temperature (te Kulve et al., 2016).

The melanopsin enriched intrinsically photoreceptive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) play a significant role in IIL responses (Berson et al., 2002). The ipRGCs have a non-visual retinal-hypothalamic connection with the mammalian circadian oscillator “Suprachiasmatic Nuclei” (SCN), thus regulating the IIL responses (Gooley et al., 2003). CIE (2018) has characterized the spectral sensitivity curve peaking at 490 nm for ipRGCs. At least three subtypes of ipRGCs are reported in humans with distinct responses to light (Mure et al., 2019). These three subtypes of ipRGCs have different sensitivities and responses to light and they are related to distinct target areas in the brain. The role played by other photoreceptors’ (S-, M-, L- cones and rods) in the IIL responses is still not fully understood (Gooley et al., 2010, 2012; Lucas et al., 2012). The ipRGCs can combine their own intrinsic inputs, which are melanopsin-driven in nature, with extrinsic inputs originated from rods and cones (Smith et al., 2005). Thus, we can say ipRGCs, rods and cons may all influence visual and IIL responses in Houser et al. (2020) and Lucas et al. (2014).

Understanding the interaction of different light properties and their relationship with IIL responses is fundamental to maintain and facilitate the desired neurobehavioral, circadian, and neuroendocrine responses. However, from the current state of literature, it is not easy to find a one size fits all solution as there are many variables at play, including individual differences (chronotypes, sleep habit, homeostatic sleep drive), light’s spectral power distribution, temporal pattern (duration of light, time, and prior history) and spatial pattern (luminance distribution in the space) as well as the complexity of the task used in measuring IIL responses.

To contribute to a better understanding of IIL responses, we present in this paper a systematic review of daytime effects of electric light on our alertness and higher cognitive functions reported in studies published from 1806 to 2021. Before presenting our systematic review and its findings, we include hereafter an excursus on light metrology and IIL response circuitry.

Light Metrology

Light is electromagnetic radiation that can directly stimulate retinal photoreceptors of the visual system to form a visual sensation (CIE, 2019). Although this definition limits light only to visual responses, it is a widespread practice in IIL literature to use “light” to describe a particular proportion of optical radiation that might induce both visual and IIL responses (Houser et al., 2020). In this paper, we have used “light” following the latter convention. Figure 1A depicts a simplified illustration of electromagnetic spectrum. Conventionally different photometric and radiometric indices are used to describe different properties of light. Radiometry characterizes electromagnetic waves’ physical properties based on a scalar field where different quantities represent radiation direction (Rossi, 2019). Photometric quantities explicitly deal with only the visible portion of the energy spectrum (380–780 nm) and describe this visible proportion according to human photoreceptors’ sensitivity. The indices used in photometry are basically cone-dominated metrics. Correlated color temperature (CCT) is such a cone-dominated metric that defines the visible color characteristics of a light source according to a black body locus. The black body locus is the path line of the color of an incandescent black body in relation to the temperature change defined by the CIE-1931 chromaticity diagram (CIE, 1931). The more power it provides to the shorter-wavelength region of visible light, the higher the CCT (Figure 1B). However, these cone dominated matrices are not sufficient to quantify light properties for IIL responses as all photoreceptors (rods, cones and ipRGCs) may contribute to IIL responses and their contributions may vary depending on light’s spectrum, intensity, temporal pattern, spatial pattern and as well as individual differences (Lucas et al., 2014). To address this issue, CIE (2018) has published an international standard (S026:E:2018) with SI units such as α- opic equivalent daylight (D65) illuminances (α-opic EDIs) to account for spectral sensitivity functions more precisely in terms of the light exposures’ ability to stimulate each of the five α-opic retinal photoreceptors. CIE (2018) has also introduced a new “M/P ratio”- melanopic daylight (D65) efficacy ratio (MDER), which is the ratio of a test light’s melanopic equivalent daylight (D65) illuminance to its photopic illuminance (Schlangen and Price, 2021). A high MDER ratio indicates the light source’s higher capability to stimulate the ipRGCs.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Electromagnetic Spectrum and (B) correlated color temperature (CCT) of lights (disclaimer: figure is for illustration purpose only).

The Intrinsically Photosensitive Retinal Ganglion Cell-Influenced Light Responses

Neuroimaging studies showed that ipRGCs project intrinsic and/or extrinsic light information to different brain regions, including the master pacemaker-suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) via the retinohypothalamic tract (Gooley et al., 2003). Additionally, various hypothalamic regions also receive signals from ipRGCs, including ventral lateral preoptic area (VLPO), locus coeruleus (LC), dorsal pathway (DR) and lateral hypothalamic area (LH) (Vandewalle et al., 2009; Lok et al., 2018a). The ipRGCs also target the amygdala (AM), the superior colliculus (SC), periaqueductal gray (PAG) and the supraventricular zone (SPZ) (Gooley et al., 2003; Vandewalle et al., 2009).

Lok et al. (2018a), in their review elaborately discussed the possible IIL response circuitry (Figure 2), especially for alerting responses. Our state of alertness is mostly controlled by the brain region associated with the thalamus (Aston-Jones, 2005) and is also subject to SCN regulation (Lok et al., 2018a). By light stimulation, ipRGCs can influence SCN controlled activity which in turn may influence our state of alertness. The nocturnal alerting effects are mostly contributed to the light’s ability to suppress melatonin through inputs from ipRGCs (Cajochen et al., 2005; Lockley et al., 2006; Houser et al., 2020). However, in the case of daytime light exposure, when the melatonin level is undetectable, alerting effects follow a different mechanism. As shown in Figure 2, different hypothalamic brain regions, including VLPO, LC, DR receive intrinsic and extrinsic inputs from the ipRGCs via direct projection from ipRGCs and/or indirect projection from SCN (Gooley et al., 2003; Moore et al., 2012). VLPO and LC are known to regulate our sleep regulation and arousal (Fort et al., 2009). When VLPO is inactive, alertness is promoted (Moore et al., 2012). Activation of LC promotes arousal, which in turn promotes an increased state of alertness (Berridge and Foote, 1991). Both LC and DR play a substantial role in wakefulness (Lee et al., 2005). In addition to the activation of different brain regions, daytime light exposure may also induce alertness by reducing homeostatic sleep drive (Rahman et al., 2014), which regulates our sleep cycle internally. As our waking hour increases, our homeostatic sleep drive increases. Daytime light exposure can reduce this sleep drive and makes us more alert.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. IpRGCs induced light response circuitries.

Lighting Variables

The IIL responses are highly dependent on the spectrum, spatial and temporal patterns (Houser et al., 2020), of which we will only be focusing on the spectrum and temporal patterns. Temporal pattern of light includes exposure duration, timing and prior history of light exposure. Studies included in our review have employed electric lights with different spectral properties: monochromatic short-wavelength light (Lockley et al., 2006; Rahman et al., 2014), polychromatic white light with narrow pass filter (Alkozei et al., 2017; Daneault et al., 2018), polychromatic white light with high power over the short-wavelength region (Askaripoor et al., 2018; te Kulve et al., 2018) or, higher intensity polychromatic white light to investigate their influence in IIL responses (Cajochen et al., 2000; Lok et al., 2018a). Table 1 summarizes the spectral properties of electric lights identified in this systematic review. Monochromatic light sources radiate only one wavelength, whereas polychromatic light radiates more than one peak wavelengths in the visible spectrum. When a polychromatic light radiates a mixture of more than one peak wavelength resulting in a white light, it is called polychromatic white light. Interference filters like narrow pass filters are used to suppress the long wavelengths of a polychromatic white light source. Polychromatic white light with high power in the short-wavelength region are basically lights with higher power over the blue region of the visible light spectrum. This systematic review clustered studies with monochromatic short-wavelength light, polychromatic white light with narrow pass filter, and polychromatic white light with high power in the short wavelength region under the label “short-wavelength dominant light exposure.” The group of studies that used polychromatic white light and investigated the differential influence of higher illuminance level on the IIL responses is clustered under the label “higher intensity white light exposure.”

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Different types of electric lights included in this systematic review.

Alertness and Higher Cognitive Functions

Alertness is the state of being awake, aware, attentive and prepared to act or react (Vandebos, 2007). Studies of attention defined alertness as achieving and maintaining a state of vigilance to respond adequately to a given stimulus (Oken et al., 2006; Shapiro et al., 2006). Alertness may also indicate a subjective experience of feeling alert (Schmidt et al., 2007). In IIL response literature, “alertness” is most commonly operationally defined by observable task performance such as faster reaction time in different cognitive tasks, or subjective evaluation of an individual’s current state of being awake. Cognitive tasks employed to measure alertness such as go-no-go and n-back tasks also incorporate different aspects of higher cognitive functions such as memory, executive functions, and inhibition. In addition, considering the IIL response circuitry that includes different brain regions related to physiological arousal, measures like electroencephalograph (EEG), electrooculography (EOG) and electrodermal activity (EDA) are also used to measure neurophysiological correlates of alertness (Cajochen, 2007).

Due to its varied nature, it is challenging to categorize the measures of alertness used in IIL response literature. Schmidt et al. (2007) classified the subjective reporting of feeling alert as subjective alertness and cognitive tasks that measure different cognitive domains, including alertness as objective performance measures. However, Lok et al. (2018a), in their work further differentiate among the objective performance measures as sustained attention tasks and executive performance tasks. Sustained attention tasks relied primarily on alertness, whereas executive performance tasks in addition to alertness may require higher-order cognitive functions (inhibition, executive functions) as well (Lok et al., 2018a). Cognitive tasks include in this review measure sustained attention, inhibition, executive functioning, and memory. In this systematic review, we considered self-reported measures of alertness as subjective measures of alertness. Neurophysiological correlates of alertness and sustained attention tasks are considered objective alertness measures. Performance in cognitive tasks that require higher-order cognitive functions is clustered under the label “higher cognitive functions.” Table 2 briefly summarizes different neurophysiological correlates and cognitive measures of alertness and higher cognitive functions. In EEG, both the power density of the alpha band (8–10 Hz) and theta band (10–13 Hz) activities of our brain were employed to measure attentional processing (Askaripoor et al., 2019). EOG used indices including slow eye movement (SEM) and eye blink to measure neurophysiological correlates of alertness (Phipps-Nelson et al., 2003; Lok et al., 2018b). ECG used heart rate index (Askaripoor et al., 2019) and EDA used skin conductance level (SCL) (Smolders and de Kort, 2017) to measure neurophysiological correlates of alertness. Sustained attention tasks measuring objective alertness used reaction time as an alertness index. And cognitive tasks measuring higher cognitive functions used reaction time and accuracy as performance indices.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Different physiological and cognitive measures of alertness and higher cognitive functions.

In this systematic review, we considered the reaction time index in the latter group as easy task performance and accuracy as complex task performance. Lastly, for the attention network task (ANT), we only report the attention network as the measure of objective alertness. The executive control network of ANT is reported as “higher cognitive functions.”

The Relevance of This Systematic Review

There are already several reviews focusing on the alerting effects of electric light (Figueiro et al., 2017; Lok et al., 2018a; Souman et al., 2018) and mood (Xiao et al., 2021) – none of these reviews except Lok et al. (2018a) incorporated higher cognitive functions. However, these authors focused their review mainly on the effects of white light exposure. Our systematic review incorporates the influence of all types of daytime electric light exposure (monochromatic, narrowband, polychromatic white, higher power over short-wavelength region) on alertness and higher cognitive functions. Though the use of monochromatic and narrowband light may seem impractical in real-world scenarios, we believe IIL literature is still in its exploratory stage, where much is yet to be discovered. The inclusion of all these light stimuli can help researchers to make a more informed decision while choosing their light stimuli. Additionally, to facilitate the understanding, we have clustered the studies that have reported the alpha-opic indices for the testing lights and focused on the findings of high MDER light intervention on alertness and higher cognitive functions, which is a novelty in a review of this nature.

We limit our systematic review to daytime light exposure because literature in this area is less conclusive than nocturnal IIL response (Rüger et al., 2006; Cajochen, 2007). It is fundamental to understand the extent of daytime electric light exposure’s alerting effects to facilitate the possible implications. This systematic review is timely in view of modern spectrally tuneable lighting systems that await the knowledge of the best ways to modify light to enhance alertness and higher cognitive functions.

This systematic review aims to map currently available findings on daytime electric light exposure, human alertness and higher cognitive functions. We have posed four research questions: (1) what are the effects of short-wavelength dominant daytime electric light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions? (2) what are the effects of the light’s intensity level on alertness and higher cognitive functions? (3) what is the potential mediation of task complexity in the relationships among light exposure, alertness and higher cognitive functions? (4) what are the effects of high MDER light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions?

Methods

Protocol and Registration

We have followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) statement guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) for this systematic review. The review protocol has been registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO registration number CRD42020157603).

Eligibility Criteria

To identify studies investigating IIL responses of daytime electric light exposure on adult participants across diverse disciplines, we extend our inclusion criteria to all quantitative scholarly articles published in referred journals (Table 3). Here we focus on the studies that were performed into (a) daytime or (b) studies that compared daytime versus nighttime IIL responses. For the second group, we have reported only the results pertaining to daytime electric light exposure.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. List of inclusion-exclusion criteria.

Information Sources

Before starting the database search for keywords, we commenced a database check to confirm the novelty of our systematic review in Cochrane, Prospero and PsychINFO. A robust literature search with specific keywords was conducted in PsycARTICLE via Ovid (1806–2021), PsycINFO via Ovid (1806–2021), Cochrane Library via Wiley (1973–2021), MEDLINE via Ovid (1946–2021), EMBASE and EMBASE Classic via Ovid (1947–2021), Social Science Premium Collection via ProQuest (1914–2021), ProQuest Central Via ProQuest (1970–2021), PubMed (1800–2021), Web of Science (1971–2021), Scopus (1970–2021) and Google Scholar. Finally, the forward and backward search was conducted.

Search

Four main keywords, “light,” “cognition,” “circadian cycle” and “daytime” were analyzed to accumulate thesaurus terms (Table 4). The keywords were entered with several combinations using AND/OR. The results of each database search were documented separately. Additionally, an alert for new evidence in each database was created.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Thesaurus analysis of keywords.

Study Selection

MS (Mushfiqul Anwar Siraji) and SH (Shamsul Haque) conjointly completed the article selection process using Covidence1. The first author conducted the database searches resulting in 19,427 records (Figure 3) and imported them to Covidence. The system automatically identified 4,855 duplicate entries and removed them. Based on the eligibility criteria, both authors independently screened 14,572 records based on the titles. This title screening process further excludes 14,194 records leaving 378 records to screen against abstract. Two hundred and forty-three records were excluded, leaving 135 records for full-text review. At this stage, 76 records were excluded. For the final review, 59 articles were selected. Any disagreement between the reviewers was resolved in discussion.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. PRISMA flow chart of literature search for publication identification and selection.

Interventions

Studies included in this systematic review focused on the effects of daytime electric light (monochromatic, narrowband, short-wavelength dominant polychromatic white) exposure of any duration on human alertness and their higher cognitive functions. We chose studies with any duration of daytime light exposure as light can activate brain regions related to alertness with as little as 2 s (Vandewalle et al., 2013). We further grouped these studies as “short-wavelength dominant light exposure” and “higher intensity white light exposure.”

Outcomes

We checked each study on whether it reports the positive enhancing effect of daytime light exposure on subjective alertness (self-reported), objective alertness (measured by EEG, ECG, EOV, reaction time in psychomotor vigilance test-PVT and attention network component of ANT) and higher cognitive functions (accuracy and reaction time in different cognitive tasks).

Data Extraction Process

The first author extracted the key information necessary for synthesis, including author(s) and year, participants, design, settings, study objectives, spectral and temporal characteristics of light exposure, adaptation period, exclusion and inclusion criteria for participants, methods, measures of subjective and objective alertness, measures of higher cognitive functions and data analysis. SH verified the extracted data before synthesis.

Risk of Study Bias and Quality Assessment

Two authors (MS, SH) independently assessed the risk of study bias systematically using the methods described in the Cochrane risk of bias assessment tool (Higgins et al., 2011). Using this tool, studies were assessed to identify any potential selection bias (random sequence generation and allocation), reporting bias (selective reporting), performance bias for each outcome (blinding of the participant, personnel and outcome assessors), detection bias (blinding of outcome assessment, attrition bias) and any other sources of bias (Supplementary Figure 1). As none of the selected studies showed any potential risk of bias, we retain all 59 studies.

Data Synthesis

We tabulated and summarized the quantitative data following the guidance from the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (CRD, 2009). Based on the studies’ focus, we categorized them into two groups: short-wavelength-dominant electric light exposure and higher intensity white light exposure. Figure 4 depicted the trend of IIL literature focusing on alertness and higher cognitive functions. In total, we have identified 30 studies investigating the influence of short wavelength dominant light exposure and 26 studies investigating the influence of the higher intensity white light on alertness and higher cognitive functions. Only three studies investigated the interaction effect of light intensity and spectral composition on alertness and higher cognitive functions (Ru et al., 2019; Zeeuw et al., 2019; Zhu et al., 2019). We included these studies in both groups and reported only the main effects for the respective group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The trend of IIL literature focusing on alertness and higher cognitive functions.

We categorized the studies according to the time of day at which the light exposure started (morning: 6.00–12.00; afternoon 12.00–18.00). Further, for studies that have reported α-opic indices for their light stimuli, we have calculated the MDER following the recommendations of Schlangen and Price (2021). Subsequently, we reported the influence of higher MDER on alertness and higher cognitive functions. From the tabulated data it was observed that the included studies varied highly in terms of their sample size and intervention. Such variations in intervention and methodology made it impossible to conduct a meta-analysis.

Results

Controlling Interindividual Variability

Studies investigating the non-image forming effects of daytime light exposure employed several unique exclusion/inclusion criteria to minimize interindividual variability of homeostatic sleep drive, circadian cycle and sleep habits. Most studies employed a specific sleep schedule (ranging from three days to three weeks prior to the experiment; 39 studies) chronotype control (35 studies) and sleep quality assessment (20 studies) to minimize the internal circadian phase differences and match the homeostatic sleep drive of the participants.

Effects of Electric Light Exposure on Human Alertness and Higher Cognitive Functions

Thirty-Three studies compared the effects of short-wavelength dominant light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions (Supplementary Table 1). Among these, 17 studies compared the influence of blue (∼470 nm) or blue-enriched white light to either amber (∼578 nm), green (∼560 nm), red (∼630 nm), or dim lights (<1–5 lux) on alertness and higher cognitive functions and another 13 compared the influence of high CCT light (4,000 K–17,000 K) to low CCT (2,500 K–4,000 K) on alertness and higher cognitive functions. Three studies compared the interaction of spectral composition and light intensity. We have reported only the main effect of spectral composition cited in these studies. The light stimuli used in these studies are also significantly different and are reported accordingly. Twenty-eight studies investigated the effect of higher intensity light exposure levels (1,000–8,121 lx) to lower intensity light exposure (10 l–500 lx) on alertness and higher cognitive functions (Supplementary Table 2).

Fourteen studies have reported α-opic indices for their test lighting conditions. Among these, only two studies (Zeeuw et al., 2019; Grant et al., 2021) have employed the new standard suggested by CIE (2018). The rest reports the α-opic indices in accordance with the CIE Technical Note 003:2015 (CIE, 2015) using toolbox provided by Lucas et al. (2014). We calculated the MDER for these studies by following the recommendations of Schlangen and Price (2021). The results are summarized in Supplementary Table 3.

Influence of Short Wavelength Dominant Light Exposure

Table 5 and Figure 5 summarize the findings of our current review. 47.4% (9 out of 19) studies investigating the influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure in the morning, reported beneficial influence on subjective alertness, 46.2% (6 out of 13) reported beneficial influence on objective alertness, 37.5% (3 out of 8) reported beneficial influence on reaction time and 30% (3 out of 10) reported beneficial influence on accuracy.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Summary of the results.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Percentage of studies reporting a significant influence of light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions.

54.5% (6 out of 11) the studies investigating the influence short-wavelength dominant light exposure in the afternoon, reported beneficial influence on subjective alertness, 45.5% (5 out of 11) on objective alertness, 70% (7 out of 10) on reaction time and 14.3% (1 out of 7) reported beneficial influence on accuracy.

Figure 6A summarizes the approximate vertical photopic illuminance for the studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure reporting beneficial influences on alertness and higher cognitive functions. The photopic luminance for beneficial influences on subjective alertness ranged between 11.8 and 518 lx; for objective alertness it ranged between 40 and 1,000 lx. Beneficial influences of short-wavelength dominant light exposure were reported for reaction time with vertical photopic illuminance ranging between 40 and 333 lx. For accuracy, the vertical photopic illuminance ranged between 50 and 350 lx. The average vertical photopic illuminance for short-wavelength dominant light exposure reporting a beneficial effect on alertness and/or higher cognitive functions ranged between 135 and 395 lx.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Approximate vertical photopic illuminance for studies with (A) short-wavelength dominant light (B) higher intensity white light reporting beneficial influences on alertness and higher cognitive functions.

Influence of Higher Intensity Bright Light

Among the studies with higher intensity bright light exposure in the morning, 64.7% (11 out of 17) reported beneficial influence on subjective alertness, 31.3% (5 out of 16) on objective alertness, 33.3% (3 out of 9) on reaction time and 41.7% (5 out of 12) reported beneficial influence on accuracy. Among the studies with higher intensity bright light exposure in the afternoon, 57.1% (8 out of 14) reported beneficial influence on subjective alertness, 25% (2 out of 8) on objective alertness, 16.7% (1 out of 6) on reaction time and 40% (4 out of 10) reported beneficial influence on accuracy.

Figure 6B summarizes the approximate vertical photopic illuminance for the studies with higher intensity white light reporting beneficial influence on alertness and higher cognitive functions. The photopic luminance for beneficial influence on subjective alertness ranged between 100 and 5,000 lx; for objective alertness it ranged between 750 and 5,000 lx. Beneficial influences of higher intensity white light were reported for reaction time with vertical photopic illuminance ranged between 1,200 and 5,000 lx. For accuracy, the vertical photopic illuminance ranged between 1,000 and 8,121 lx. The average vertical photopic illuminance for higher intensity white light exposure reporting beneficial effects on alertness and/or higher cognitive functions ranged between 1,082 and 2,424 lx.

Influence of High Melanopic Daylight Efficacy Ratio Light Exposure

The pattern of influence of high MDER light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions varies highly (Figure 7). Among the studies investigating the influence of high MDER light exposure on subjective alertness, 50% (6 out of 12) reported a beneficial influence of light exposure with MDER ranging between (0.70–1.4). For objective alertness, 41.7% of the studies (5 out of 12) reported beneficial influences of light exposure with MDER ranging between (0.4–1.4). For reaction time performance 30% of the studies (3 out of 10) reported beneficial influences of light exposure with MDER ranging between (4.3–1.1) against dim light conditions (<∼0–5 lx). For accuracy, 9% of the studies (1 out of 11) reported a beneficial influence of high MDER (0.9) light exposure on accuracy against control light (0.48).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Influence of high MDER light exposure on (A) subjective alertness, (B) objective alertness, (C) reaction time, (D) accuracy. Blue squares represent high MDER light exposure in a study that reported significant beneficial effects; the corresponding blue circle represents low MDER light exposure used in the same study. The red diamond shape represents high MDER light exposure that reported insignificant effects or significant deteriorating effects. The corresponding inverted red triangle represents low MDER light exposure used in the same study.

Discussion

The studies included in this review varied in their light modification method (short-wavelength dominant light exposure and higher intensity white light exposure), type of light (monochromatic, polychromatic, filtered), nature of exposure (continuous exposure, intermittent exposure), duration of exposure in a single session (1 s–24 h), session number (single session to 5 sessions per week) and time of day (morning and afternoon). Such variations in intervention and methodology, and the absence of the reporting of effect size in many studies made it difficult to summarize the results.

Influence on Alertness and Higher Cognitive Functions

In general, more studies have reported alerting effects of light using subjective measures than objective measures. In the morning, 11 out of 17 (64.7%) studies with higher intensity white light exposure reported increasing subjective alertness, whereas 9 out of 19 (47.5%) studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure reported a similar result However, in the afternoon, the percentage of reporting beneficial influence on subjective alertness is almost similar for studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure (54.5%; 6 out of 11) and higher intensity white light exposure (57.1%; 8 out of 14). For objective alertness, a higher percentage of studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure reported a beneficial influence both in the morning (46.2%; 6 out of 13) and in the afternoon (45.5%; 5 out of 11) compared to studies with higher intensity white light (31.3%; 5 out of 16 and 25%; 2 out of 8, respectively).

Among the studies measuring the influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure on higher cognitive functions, a higher percentage of studies reported beneficial influences on reaction time (easy task performance) in the afternoon (70%; 7 out of 10) than morning (37.5%; 3 out of 8). However, for accuracy (complex task performance), the percentage of studies reporting a beneficial influence reduced to almost half in the afternoon (14.3%; 1 out of 17) compared to morning (30%; 3 out of 10). Among the studies measuring the influence of higher intensity white light on higher cognitive functions, a higher percentage of studies reported beneficial influence on reaction time in the morning (33.3%; 3 out of 9) than afternoon (16.7%; 1 out of 6). For accuracy, the percentage of studies reporting a beneficial effect remains almost equal for the morning (41.7%; 5 out of 12) and afternoon (40%; 4 out of 10).

The findings on beneficial influences of daytime electric light on alertness and higher cognitive functions indicate possible different mechanisms of short-wavelength dominant light exposure and high intensity white light exposure. In the afternoon, when the homeostatic sleep drive is generally higher than the morning, higher intensity white light might not contribute as much as short-wavelength dominant light toward alertness. Likewise, easy task performance (reaction time) might be benefitted more from short-wavelength dominant light exposure when our homeostatic sleep drive is high. However, further investigation is required to better understand these beneficial influences of light.

Influenced Light Responses and Task Complexity

We found that 9 out of 22 (40%) studies reported a beneficial influence of higher intensity light exposure on accuracy (complex task performance). In contrast, only 4 out of 17 (23%) studies reported similar effects with short-wavelength dominant light exposure, regardless of when and for how long the exposure was given. This may indicate that higher intensity white light has a better ability to improve complex task performance compared to short-wavelength-dominant light exposure. However, such a conclusion cannot be firmly drawn without a meta-analysis. From the studies focusing on the higher intensity white light, it is also evident that most successful studies reporting positive influence on higher cognitive functions employed at least 1,000 lx (Figure 6). Such a high illuminance level (≥1,000 lx) has been reported to increase physiological arousal levels (Rüger et al., 2006; Smolders et al., 2012). This increased physiological arousal might play a vital role in enhancing higher cognitive functions based on the task complexity, as described by the Yerkes-Dodson Law (Yerkes, 1910). In complex task performance such as accuracy, increased performance would only appear at an optimal arousal level. This required optimal level of arousal may vary from task to task. On the other hand, easy task performance such as reaction time would linearly increase with the arousal level. Therefore, it can be suggested that higher intensity white light is required to attain a level of arousal that would be optimal for higher cognitive functions, although this mechanism may vary according to task complexity. The relationship between higher intensity white light and task complexity was explored by Huiberts et al. (2016). Their study reported an improved performance in the complex task performance for the highest intensity level (1,700 lx). However, that study failed to report any significant alerting effect. This finding could have been driven by the fact that all the light intensities (165, 600, and 1,700 lx) were already in the saturation zone of alerting effects. Such an indication of saturation is evident in the work of Cajochen et al. (2000) where a dose-dependent relationship of nighttime white light exposure and alertness was investigated and saturation of alertness was reported to start at ∼110 lx. Though no such relationship was established for daytime white light exposure, Lok et al. (2018b), in their study reported a possible saturating effect of alertness for daytime at the of 75 lx. These findings indicate a comparatively low-intensity white light is required to induce alertness and higher intensity white light is required to improve higher cognitive functions.

On the contrary, a recent study of Grant et al. (2021) has reported a beneficial influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure on higher cognitive functions with a light intensity of 50 lx and a high MDER of.9 among moderately sleep-restricted young healthy adult. Together these findings demonstrate a plausible differential influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure and higher intensity white light exposure on higher cognitive functions. The optimal light intensity to improve higher cognitive functions may vary depending on spectral composition (short-wavelength dominance) and task complexity.

Importance of Spectral Composition and Illuminance

The importance of considering both the spectral composition and intensity of light can be understood from the studies employing high MDER light exposure with different light intensities (Supplementary Table 3). Among the studies reporting significant positive influence on subjective alertness, the MDER is generally high (above 70%). This pattern also holds true for objective alertness (Figure 7) with one exception (Rodriguez-Morilla et al., 2018) where a beneficial influence of light with MDER.4 is observed when compared to the no-light condition. For reaction time, it appears that having only high MDER is not sufficient. The beneficial influence on the reaction time is only reported when the high MDER experimental light exposure is coupled with comparatively higher light intensity exposure in the experimental condition than the control condition. Lastly, for accuracy only one study with high MDER light exposure (0.90) with low light intensity (50 lx) reported a beneficial influence against a control light condition with MDER (0.48) (Grant et al., 2021). Only two studies (Ru et al., 2019; Choi and Suk, 2020) employed high-intensity light exposure (lx = 1,000). However, both studies failed to report any beneficial effects on accuracy.

All these studies vary largely in terms of intensity differences between experimental conditions and control conditions. And some of the studies reported beneficial influences of light only when compared to low light (Askaripoor et al., 2018, 2019) (∼5 lx) or no light conditions (Rodriguez-Morilla et al., 2018). The current literature indicates that both short wavelength dominant light and higher intensity white light exposure induce alertness and a positive influence on higher cognitive functions. Factors that contribute toward these effects include homeostatic sleep drive, time of day, task complexity and the control light condition. However, the real magnitude of these beneficial effects remains an object of debate. Also, further investigation is required to understand the influence of short-wavelength dominant light exposure with higher intensity on alertness and higher cognitive functions.

Methodological Issues

Since studies included in this review employed a strict exclusion-inclusion criterion to limit the interindividual variability, it is understandable that most of these studies used small samples (34.78 ± 31.70). However, such sample sizes may have led to an overall reduced statistical power in the field. Additionally, very few studies reported the use of a prior power calculation while determining the sample size. Priori analysis is crucial to determine the appropriate sample size to draw a precise and accurate decision (Nayak, 2010; Katherine et al., 2013; Faber and Fonseca, 2014).

Importantly, most of these studies also lacked a detailed description of the stimuli used, which creates an obstacle to potential replication attempts. Describing the stimuli only in terms of illumination, correlated color temperature, or maximum peak wavelength is insufficient (Spitschan et al., 2019; CIE, 2020). Instead, representing all this information using a spectral power distribution would provide a much clearer representation. Additionally, in the studies we have reviewed, light stimuli varied highly (monochromatic, polychromatic, filtered, fluorescent, or LED), which strengthens the need of further studies reporting the spectral radiance of the stimuli. Though the more recent studies report the spectral representation of the stimulus, only a few have reported the spectral irradiance in relation to α-opic radiances by weighting the spectrum by the spectral sensitivity of each of the five photoreceptors separately (S-, M-, L-cones, rods and ipRGCs). We strongly recommend future studies to follow the guidelines of CIE (2020) regarding how stimuli should be reported.

Conclusion

Light affects our physiology and cognitive behavior. In this review, we focused on the beneficial influence of light on the indices of alertness and higher cognitive functions. We have categorized the studies into two groups: short-wavelength dominant light exposure and higher intensity white light exposure. We then tried to summarize the findings of these studies on our chosen outcomes. We asked four specific questions and the key findings pertaining to those four questions are summarized below:

• Both short-wavelength dominant light exposure and higher intensity light exposure can beneficially influence the indices of alertness. The pattern of influence depends on factors such as time of day and homeostatic sleep drive.

• The relationship between higher cognitive functions and light is not as straightforward as the alerting effect. The optimal level of intensity to induce improved higher cognitive performance may depend on the spectral composition, task complexity and time of day.

• A higher percentage of studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure reported a beneficial influence on easy task performance (reaction time) in the afternoon compared to the morning. However, a reverse pattern was found for higher intensity white light.

• For complex task performance (accuracy), a higher percentage of studies reported beneficial influences of higher intensity white light on accuracy than studies with short-wavelength dominant light exposure, does not matter when and for how long the exposure was given.

• Studies with high MDER light exposure are successful in inducing alertness. However, beneficial influences are only reported for easier task performance (reaction time) when high MEDR light exposure is paired with comparatively higher intensity light. This pattern was not observed for complex task performance (accuracy).

Overall, the findings of this review indicate that the effects of daytime light exposure on alertness and higher cognitive functions are complex and dependent on various factors such as spectral composition, intensity, homeostatic sleep drive, control light condition, task complexity and time of day. Further, we found that this field of research suffered from a number of limitations, such as small sample sizes and a frequent lack of details in reporting the nature of stimuli used. Further, we also note the relative lack of studies taking place in real-life settings. Only a few studies have investigated the beneficial influence of light on alertness and higher cognitive functions in a real setting (Mills et al., 2007; Riemersma-Van Der Lek, 2008; Viola et al., 2008; Rautkylä et al., 2010; Iskra-Golec et al., 2012; Figueiro and Pedler, 2020) and most of them have limited their investigation to subjective alertness. More focused investigations considering objective alertness and especially higher cognitive functions are needed to advance the field.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

MS, VK, AS, and SH: conceptualization. MS: data curation, formal analysis, and writing – original draft. VK and SH: supervision and writing – review and editing. AS: writing – review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by a research grant provided by ITRAMAS Corporation Sdn. Bhd (Grant Code ENG/ITRAMAS/08-2018/008).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.765750/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Risk of bias summary. Green denotes low risk.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.covidence.org

References

Akerstedt, T., and Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 52, 29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akerstedt, T., Landstrom, U., Byström, M., Nordström, B., and Wibom, R. (2003). Bright light as a sleepiness prophylactic: a laboratory study of subjective ratings and EEG. Percept. Mot. Skills 97, 811–819. doi: 10.2466/pms.2003.97.3.811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alkozei, A., Smith, R., Dailey, N. S., Bajaj, S., and Killgore, W. D. (2017). Acute exposure to blue wavelength light during memory consolidation improves verbal memory performance. PLoS One 12:e0184884. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

An, M., Huang, J., Shimomura, Y., and Katsuura, T. (2009). Time-of-day-dependent effects of monochromatic light exposure on human cognitive function. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 28, 217–223.

Google Scholar

Askaripoor, T., Motamedzade, M., Golmohammadi, R., Farhadian, M., Babamiri, M., and Samavati, M. (2018). Non-image forming effects of light on brainwaves, autonomic nervous activity, fatigue, and performance. J. Circadian Rhythms 16:9. doi: 10.5334/jcr.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Askaripoor, T., Motamedzade, M., Golmohammadi, R., Farhadian, M., Babamiri, M., and Samavati, M. (2019). Effects of light intervention on alertness and mental performance during the post-lunch dip: a multi-measure study. Ind. Health 57, 511–524. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2018-0030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G. (2005). Brain structures and receptors involved in alertness. Sleep Med. 6, S3–S7. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(05)80002-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badia, P., Myers, B., Boecker, M., Culpepper, J., and Harsh, J. R. (1991). Bright light effects on body temperature, alertness, EEG and behavior. Physiol. Behav. 50, 583–588.

Google Scholar

Baek, H., and Min, B. K. (2015). Blue light aids in coping with the post-lunch dip: an EEG study. Ergonomics 58, 803–810.

Google Scholar

Berridge, C. W., and Foote, S. L. (1991). Effects of locus coeruleus activation on electroencephalographic activity in neocortex and hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 11, 3135–3145. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-10-03135.1991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berson, D. M., Dunn, F. A., and Takao, M. (2002). Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science 295:1070. doi: 10.1126/science.1067262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodner, G., and Guay, R. (1997). The purdue visualization of rotations test. Chem. Educator 2, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s00897970138a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borisuit, A., Linhart, F., Scartezzini, J. L., and Münch, M. (2015). Effects of realistic office daylighting and electric lighting conditions on visual comfort, alertness and mood. Light. Res. Technol. 47, 192–209. doi: 10.1177/1477153514531518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borragán, G., Slama, H., Destrebecqz, A., and Peigneux, P. (2016). Cognitive fatigue facilitates procedural sequence learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:86. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boubekri, M., Lee, J., MacNaughton, P., Woo, M., Schuyler, L., Tinianov, B., et al. (2020). The impact of optimized daylight and views on the sleep duration and cognitive performance of office workers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:3219. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17093219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burattini, C., Piccardi, L., Curcio, G., Ferlazzo, F., Giannini, A. M., and Bisegna, F. (2019). Cold LED lighting affects visual but not acoustic vigilance. Build. Environ. 151:148. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2019.01.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C. (2007). Alerting effects of light. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 453–464.

Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., and Chellappa, S. L. (2016). Commentary: melanopsin regulates both sleep-promoting and arousal-promoting responses to light. Front. Neural Circuits 10:94. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Freyburger, M., Basishvili, T., Garbazza, C., Rudzik, F., Renz, C., et al. (2019). Effect of daylight LED on visual comfort, melatonin, mood, waking performance and sleep. Light. Res. Technol. 51, 1044–1062. doi: 10.1177/1477153519828419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Münch, M., Kobialka, S., Kräuchi, K., Steiner, R., Oelhafen, P., et al. (2005). High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate to short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 90, 1311–1316. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-0957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Zeitzer, J. M., Czeisler, C. A., and Dijk, D. J. (2000). Dose-response relationship for light intensity and ocular and electroencephalographic correlates of human alertness. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 75–83.

Google Scholar

Choi, K., Shin, C., Kim, T., Chung, H. J., and Suk, H.-J. (2019). Awakening effects of blue-enriched morning light exposure on university students’ physiological and subjective responses. Sci. Rep. 9:345. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36791-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, K., and Suk, H.-J. (2020). The gradual transition from blue-enriched to neutral white light for creating a supportive learning environment. Build. Environ. 180:107046. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2020.107046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CIE (1931). Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage Proceedings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

CIE (2015). CIE TN003:2015. Report on the First International Workshop on Circadian and Neurophysiological Photometry, 2013. Vienna: CIE.

Google Scholar

CIE (2018). CIE International Standard (CIE S 026/E:2018) “System for Metrology of Optical Radiation for ipRGC-Influenced Responses to Light”. Vienna: CIE.

Google Scholar

CIE (2019). Position Statement on Non-Visual Effects of Light: Recommending Proper Light and the Proper Time. Vienna: CIE.

Google Scholar

CIE (2020). What to Document and Report in Studies of ipRGC-Influenced Responses to Light. Vienna: CIE.

Google Scholar

CRD (2009). Systematic Reviews: Centre for Reviews and Dissemination’s Guidance for Undertaking Reviews in Healthcare. York: Centre for Reviews and Dissemination.

Google Scholar

Daneault, V., Dumont, M., Masse, E., Forcier, P., Bore, A., Lina, J. M., et al. (2018). Plasticity in the sensitivity to light in aging: decreased non-visual impact of light on cognitive brain activity in older individuals but no impact of lens replacement. Front. Physiol. 9:1557. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daurat, A., Aguirre, A., Foret, J., Gonnet, P., Keromes, A., and Benoit, O. (1993). Bright light affects alertness and performance rhythms during a 24-h constant routine. Physiol. Behav. 53, 929–936.

Google Scholar

de Kort, Y. A. (2019). Tutorial: theoretical considerations when planning research on human factors in lighting. Leukos 15, 85–96. doi: 10.1080/15502724.2018.1558065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, J., and Fonseca, L. M. (2014). How sample size influences research outcomes. Dental Press J. Orthod. 19, 27–29. doi: 10.1590/2176-9451.19.4.027-029.ebo

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferlazzo, F., Piccardi, L., Burattini, C., Barbalace, M., Giannini, A. M., and Bisegna, F. (2014). Effects of new light sources on task switching and mental rotation performance. J. Environ. Psychol. 39, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiro, M., Nagare, R., and Price, L. L. A. (2017). Non-visual effects of light: how to use light to promote circadian entrainment and elicit alertness. Light. Res. Technol. 50, 38–62. doi: 10.1177/1477153517721598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiro, M., and Pedler, D. (2020). Red light: a novel, non-pharmacological intervention to promote alertness in shift workers. J. Safety Res. 74, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2020.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fort, P., Bassetti, C. L., and Luppi, P. H. (2009). Alternating vigilance states: new insights regarding neuronal networks and mechanisms. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1741–1753. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06722.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabel, V., Miglis, M., and Zeitzer, J. M. (2020). Effect of artificial dawn light on cardiovascular function, alertness, and balance in middle-aged and older adults. Sleep 43:zsaa082. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooley, J., Ho Mien, I., St Hilaire, M. A., Yeo, S.-C., Chua, E. C.-P., van Reen, E., et al. (2012). Melanopsin and rod-cone photoreceptors play different roles in mediating pupillary light responses during exposure to continuous light in humans. J. Neurosci. 32:14242. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1321-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooley, J., Rajaratnam, S. M. W., Brainard, G. C., Kronauer, R. E., Czeisler, C. A., and Lockley, S. W. (2010). Spectral responses of the human circadian system depend on the irradiance and duration of exposure to light. Sci. Transl. Med. 2:31ra33. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3000741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooley, J. J., Lu, J., Fischer, D., and Saper, C. B. (2003). A broad role for melanopsin in nonvisual photoreception. J. Neurosci. 23, 7093–7106. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-18-07093.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, L. K., Kent, B. A., Mayer, M. D., Stickgold, R., Lockley, S. W., and Rahman, S. A. (2021). Daytime exposure to short wavelength-enriched light improves cognitive performance in sleep-restricted college-aged adults. Front. Neurol. 12:624217. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.624217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept. Mot. Skills 44, 367–373. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartstein, L. E., Durniak, M. T., Karlicek, R. F., and Berthier, N. E. (2017). A comparison of the effects of correlated colour temperature and gender on cognitive task performance. Light. Res. Technol. 50, 1057–1069. doi: 10.1177/1477153517721728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The cochrane collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343, 22–893. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houser, K. W., Boyce, P. R., Zeitzer, J. M., and Herf, M. (2020). Human-centric lighting: myth, magic or metaphor? Light. Res. Technol. 53:1477153520958448. doi: 10.1177/1477153520958448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huiberts, L., Smolders, K., and de Kort, Y. (2016). Non-image forming effects of illuminance level: exploring parallel effects on physiological arousal and task performance. Physiol. Behav. 164(Pt A), 129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huiberts, L., Smolders, K., and de Kort, Y. A. W. (2015). Shining light on memory: effects of bright light on working memory performance. Behav. Brain Res. 294, 234–245. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IES (2018). Light and Human Health: An Overview of the Impact of Optical Radiation on Visual, Circadian, Neuroendocrine and Neurobehavioral Responses. Houston, TX: IES.

Google Scholar

Iskra-Golec, I., Wazna, A., and Smith, L. (2012). Effects of blue-enriched light on the daily course of mood, sleepiness and light perception: a field experiment. Light. Res. Technol. 44, 506–513. doi: 10.1177/1477153512447528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaida, K., Takeda, Y., and Tsuzuki, K. (2012). Can a short nap and bright light function as implicit learning and visual search enhancers? Ergonomics 55, 1340–1349.

Google Scholar

Kaida, K., Takeda, Y., and Tsuzuki, K. (2013). The effects of short afternoon nap and bright light on task switching performance and error-related negativity. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 11, 125–134.

Google Scholar

Katherine, S. B., John, P. A. I., Claire, M., Brian, A. N., Jonathan, F., Emma, S. J. R., et al. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14:365. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. S., Park, S. H., Song, W. C., and Waterhouse, B. D. (2005). Retrograde study of hypocretin-1 (orexin-A) projections to subdivisions of the dorsal raphe nucleus in the rat. Brain Res. 1059, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leichtfried, V., Hanser, F., Griesmacher, A., Canazei, M., and Schobersberger, W. (2016). Brief morning light exposure, visuomotor performance, and biochemistry in sport shooters. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 11, 756–762. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2015-0553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leichtfried, V., Mair-Raggautz, M., Schaeffer, V., Hammerer-Lercher, A., Mair, G., Bartenbach, C., et al. (2015). Intense illumination in the morning hours improved mood and alertness but not mental performance. Appl. Ergon. 46, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2014.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2008). Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockley, S. W. (2008). Spectral sensitivity of circadian, neuroendocrine and neurobehavioral effects of light. J. Hum. Environ. Syst. 11:43.

Google Scholar

Lockley, S. W., Evans, E. E., Scheer, F. A. J. L., Brainard, G. C., Czeisler, C. A., and Aeschbach, D. (2006). Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 29, 161–168.

Google Scholar

Lok, R., Smolders, K. C. H. J., Beersma, D. G. M., and de Kort, Y. A. W. (2018a). Light, alertness, and alerting effects of white light: a literature overview. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 589–601. doi: 10.1177/0748730418796443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lok, R., Woelders, T., Gordijn, M. C. M., Hut, R. A., and Beersma, D. G. M. (2018b). White light during daytime does not improve alertness in well-rested individuals. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 637–648.

Google Scholar

Lok, R., van Koningsveld, M. J., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., and Hut, R. A. (2019). Daytime melatonin and light independently affect human alertness and body temperature. J. Pineal Res. 67:e12583. doi: 10.1111/jpi.12583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, R. J., Lall, G. S., Allen, A. E., and Brown, T. M. (2012). How rod, cone, and melanopsin photoreceptors come together to enlighten the mammalian circadian clock. Prog. Brain Res. 199, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-444-59427-3.00001-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, R. J., Peirson, S. N., Berson, D. M., Brown, T. M., Cooper, H. M., Czeisler, C. A., et al. (2014). Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 37, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackworth, J. F. (1959). Paced memorizing in a continuous task. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 206–211. doi: 10.1037/h0049090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maierova, L., Borisuit, A., Scartezzini, J. L., Jaeggi, S. M., Schmidt, C., and Münch, M. (2016). Diurnal variations of hormonal secretion, alertness and cognition in extreme chronotypes under different lighting conditions. Sci. Rep. 6:33591. doi: 10.1038/srep33591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, D., and Schvaneveldt, R. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. J. Exp. Psychol. 90, 227–234. doi: 10.1037/h0031564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, P. R., Tomkins, S. C., and Schlangen, L. J. (2007). The effect of high correlated colour temperature office lighting on employee wellbeing and work performance. J. Circadian Rhythms 5:2. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-5-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 151:264. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. T., Chen, J., Han, B., Meng, Q. C., Veasey, S. C., Beck, S. G., et al. (2012). Direct activation of sleep-promoting VLPO neurons by volatile anesthetics contributes to anesthetic hypnosis. Curr. Biol. 22, 2008–2016. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munch, M., Nowozin, C., Regente, J., Bes, F., De Zeeuw, J., Hadel, S., et al. (2017). Blue-enriched morning light as a countermeasure to light at the wrong time: effects on cognition, sleepiness, sleep, and circadian phase. Neuropsychobiology 74, 207–218.

Google Scholar

Mure, L. S., Vinberg, F., Hanneken, A., and Panda, S. (2019). Functional diversity of human intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Science 366, 1251–1255. doi: 10.1126/science.aaz0898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nayak, B. K. (2010). Understanding the relevance of sample size calculation. Indian J. Ophthalmol. 58, 469–470. doi: 10.4103/0301-4738.71673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, Y., and Nakagawa, S. (2015). Effects of daytime light exposure on cognitive brain activity as measured by the ERP P300. Physiol. Behav. 138, 313–318.

Google Scholar

Oken, B. S., Salinsky, M. C., and Elsas, S. M. (2006). Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1885–1901. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phipps-Nelson, J., Redman, J. R., Dijk, D. J., and Rajaratnam, S. M. (2003). Daytime exposure to bright light, as compared to dim light, decreases sleepiness and improves psychomotor vigilance performance. Sleep 26, 695–700. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.6.695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J., and Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 41, 103–146. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, S. A., Flynn-Evans, E. E., Aeschbach, D., Brainard, G. C., Czeisler, C. A., and Lockley, S. W. (2014). Diurnal spectral sensitivity of the acute alerting effects of light. Sleep 37, 271–281.

Google Scholar

Rautkylä, E., Puolakka, M., Tetri, E., and Halonen, L. (2010). Effects of correlated colour temperature and timing of light exposure on daytime alertness in lecture environments. J. Light Vis. Environ. 34, 59–68. doi: 10.2150/jlve.34.59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., Lowe, P., and Moore, J. J. (2002). The continuous performance test: a window on the neural substrates for attention? Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 17, 235–272.

Google Scholar

Riemersma-Van Der Lek, R. F. (2008). Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive function in elderly residents of group care facilities. JAMA 299:2642. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.22.2642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Morilla, B., Madrid, J. A., Molina, E., Perez-Navarro, J., and Correa, A. (2018). Blue-enriched light enhances alertness but impairs accurate performance in evening chronotypes driving in the morning. Front. Psychol. 9:688. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, M. (2019). Circadian Lighting Design in the LED Era. Cham: Springer.

Google Scholar

Ru, T., de Kort, Y. A., Smolders, K. C. H. J., Chen, Q., and Zhou, G. (2019). Non-image forming effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature of office light on alertness, mood, and performance across cognitive domains. Build. Environ. 149, 253–263. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2018.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rüger, M., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., de Vries, B., and Daan, S. (2006). Time-of-day-dependent effects of bright light exposure on human psychophysiology: comparison of daytime and nighttime exposure. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 290:R1413. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00121.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahin, L., and Figueiro, M. G. (2013). Alerting effects of short-wavelength (blue) and long-wavelength (red) lights in the afternoon. Physiol. Behav. 116-117, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Sahin, L., Wood, B. M., Plitnick, B., and Figueiro, M. G. (2014). Daytime light exposure: effects on biomarkers, measures of alertness, and performance. Behav. Brain Res. 274, 176–185.

Google Scholar

Schlangen, L. J. M., and Price, L. L. A. (2021). The lighting environment, its metrology, and non-visual responses. Front. Neurol. 12:624861. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.624861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., and Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24, 755–789. doi: 10.1080/02643290701754158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segal, A. Y., Sletten, T. L., Flynn-Evans, E. E., Lockley, S. W., and Rajaratnam, S. M. W. (2016). Daytime exposure to short- and medium-wavelength light did not improve alertness and neurobehavioral performance. J. Biol. Rhythms 31, 470–482. doi: 10.1177/0748730416659953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, C. M., Auch, C., Reimer, M., Kayumov, L., Heslegrave, R., Huterer, N., et al. (2006). A new approach to the construct of alertness. J. Psychosom. Res. 60, 595–603. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slama, H., Deliens, G., Schmitz, R., Peigneux, P., and Leproult, R. (2015). Afternoon nap and bright light exposure improve cognitive flexibility post lunch. PLoS One 10:e0125359. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sletten, T. L., Revell, V. L., Middleton, B., Lederle, K. A., and Skene, D. J. (2009). Age-related changes in acute and phase-advancing responses to monochromatic light. J. Biol. Rhythms 24, 73–84. doi: 10.1177/0748730408328973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, V. C., Pokorny, J., Yau, K.-W., Liao, H.-W., Gamlin, P. D., Dacey, D. M., et al. (2005). Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature 433, 749–754. doi: 10.1038/nature03387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smolders, K., and de Kort, Y. A. (2014). Bright light and mental fatigue: effects on alertness, vitality, performance and physiological arousal. J. Environ. Psychol. 39, 77–91.

Google Scholar

Smolders, K., and de Kort, Y. A. (2017). Investigating daytime effects of correlated colour temperature on experiences, performance, and arousal. J. Environ. Psychol. 50, 80–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smolders, K., de Kort, Y. A., and Cluitmans, P. J. M. (2012). A higher illuminance induces alertness even during office hours: findings on subjective measures, task performance and heart rate measures. Physiol. Behav. 107, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Šmotek, M., Vlček, P., Saifutdinova, E., and Kopřivová, J. (2019). Objective and subjective characteristics of vigilance under different narrow-bandwidth light conditions: do shorter wavelengths have an alertness-enhancing effect? Neuropsychobiology 78, 238–248. doi: 10.1159/000502962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souman, J. L., Tinga, A. M., te Pas, S. F., van Ee, R., and Vlaskamp, B. N. S. (2018). Acute alerting effects of light: a systematic literature review. Behav. Brain Res. 337, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitschan, M., Stefani, O., Blattner, P., Gronfier, C., Lockley, S. W., and Lucas, R. J. (2019). How to report light exposure in human chronobiology and sleep research experiments. Clocks Sleep 1, 280–289. doi: 10.3390/clockssleep1030024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

te Kulve, M. T., Schellen, L., Schlangen, L. J., and Lichtenbelt, W. D. (2016). The influence of light on thermal responses. Acta Physiol. 216, 163–185. doi: 10.1111/apha.12552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

te Kulve, M. T., Schlangen, L., Schellen, L., Souman, J. L., and Lichtenbelt, W. (2018). Correlated colour temperature of morning light influences alertness and body temperature. Physiol. Behav. 185, 1–13.

Google Scholar

te Kulve, M. T., Schlangen, L. J. M., Schellen, L., Frijns, A. J. H., and Lichtenbelt, W. D. (2017). The impact of morning light intensity and environmental temperature on body temperatures and alertness. Physiol. Behav. 175, 72–81. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonetti, L., and Natale, V. (2019). Effects of a single short exposure to blue light on cognitive performance. Chronobiol. Int. 36, 725–732. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2019.1593191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandebos, G. (2007). APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G., Collignon, O., Hull, J. T., Daneault, V., Albouy, G., Lepore, F., et al. (2013). Blue light stimulates cognitive brain activity in visually blind individuals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2072–2085.

Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G., Maquet, P., and Dijk, D.-J. (2009). Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viola, A. U., James, L. M., Schlangen, L. J. M., and Dijk, D.-J. (2008). Blue-enriched white light in the workplace improves self-reported alertness, performance and sleep quality. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 34, 297–306. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, S. P., Delis, D. C., Scott, J. C., Kramer, J. H., and Holdnack, J. A. (2006). The California verbal learning test–second edition: test-retest reliability, practice effects, and reliable change indices for the standard and alternate forms. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 21, 413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, H., Cai, H., and Li, X. (2021). Non-visual effects of indoor light environment on humans: a review. Physiol. Behav. 228:113195. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Ru, T., Chen, Q., Mao, T., Ji, Y., and Zhou, G. (2019). The effects of ambient light on task switching depend on the chronotype. Light. Res. Technol. 51, 544–556. doi: 10.1177/1477153518777028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, M., Zheng, S. Q., Wang, M. L., and Ronnier, L. M. (2018). The effect of dynamic correlated colour temperature changes on alertness and performance. Light. Res. Technol. 50, 1070–1081. doi: 10.1177/1477153518755617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerkes, A. W. (1910). Review of Modifiability of behavior in its relation to the age and sex of the dancing mouse and the relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Educ. Psychol. 1, 415–417. doi: 10.1037/h0067328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshiike, T., Honma, M., Ikeda, H., and Kuriyama, K. (2019). Bright light exposure advances consolidation of motor skill accuracy in humans. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 166:107084. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeeuw, J., Papakonstantinou, A., Nowozin, C., Stotz, S., Zaleska, M., Hädel, S., et al. (2019). Living in biological darkness: objective sleepiness and the pupillary light responses are affected by different metameric lighting conditions during daytime. J. Biol. Rhythms 34, 410–431. doi: 10.1177/0748730419847845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., Yang, M., Yao, Y., Xiong, X., Li, X., Zhou, G., et al. (2019). Effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature on daytime cognitive performance, subjective mood, and alertness in healthy adults. Environ. Behav. 51, 199–230.

Google Scholar

Keywords: short-wavelength dominant light, higher intensity white light, subjective alertness, reaction time, higher cognitive functions

Citation: Siraji MA, Kalavally V, Schaefer A and Haque S (2022) Effects of Daytime Electric Light Exposure on Human Alertness and Higher Cognitive Functions: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 12:765750. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.765750

Received: 27 August 2021; Accepted: 30 November 2021;
Published: 05 January 2022.

Edited by:

Franco Delogu, Lawrence Technological University, United States

Reviewed by:

George Christopoulos, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Gianluca Ficca, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Siraji, Kalavally, Schaefer and Haque. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shamsul Haque, shamsul@monash.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.