Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 11 October 2021
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Effects of Physical Activity on Psychological Well-being View all 20 articles

Sport-Related Affective Benefits for Teenagers Are Getting Greater as They Approach Adulthood: A Large-Scale French Investigation

Annie Carton&#x;&#x;Annie Carton1Alexis Barbry,&#x;&#x;Alexis Barbry2,3Jrmy Coquart,&#x;Jérémy Coquart2,4Herv OvigneurHervé Ovigneur3Camille Amoura&#x;Camille Amoura1Gabor Orosz
&#x;Gabor Orosz1*
  • 1Univ. Artois, Univ. Lille, Univ. Littoral Côte d’Opale, ULR 7369 - URePSSS - Unité de Recherche Pluridisciplinaire Sport Santé Société, Liévin, France
  • 2Centre des Transformations des Activités Physiques et Sportives, Université de Rouen-Normandie, Rouen, France
  • 3L’Institut des Rencontres de la Forme, Wattignies, France
  • 4Univ. Lille, Univ. Artois, Univ. Littoral Côte d’Opale, ULR 7369 - URePSSS - Unité de Recherche Pluridisciplinaire Sport Santé Société, Lille, France

The present investigation examined how sports club membership is related to adolescents’ daily negative and positive affects as they age. Robust prior results demonstrated that sports club membership is positively related to positive affect and negatively related to negative affect. However, surprisingly, no prior studies examined whether these benefits are consistently present throughout the teenage years or there are certain critical periods when teenagers can affectively profit more from being members of a sports club. The present cross-sectional investigation examined these questions on a comprehensive sample of French adolescents (N=17,337, female=7,604, aged between 10 and 18, Mage=12.45years, SDage=1.94years). Besides the expected affective benefits of a sports club membership, there was no interaction between age and negative affect. However, late adolescents reported greater daily positive affective benefits of sports club membership than early adolescents. These results suggest that late adolescents can use the extra affective benefits of sports club membership to gain advantages for the first steps of their adult life, such as coping with career start or transition to higher education. These results can provide guidelines for future studies to prioritize late adolescents with heightened positive sport-related affective benefits. It can also be useful information to promote sport among late adolescents.

Introduction

In the past 20years, robust results suggest that regular participation in physical activity and sport has a beneficial effect on physical and mental health at all ages and for a broad variety of people (Biddle and Mutrie, 2001; Haworth and Lewis, 2005; Eime et al., 2013; Gisladottir et al., 2013b). For example, Gisladottir et al. (2013b) examining more than 10,000 14-16-year-old adolescents found that practicing sports in a club is associated with a better body image (d=0.6), a better physical (d=1.06) and mental condition (d=0.76). These positive effects appear to be rather large. Adolescence is no exception, physical activity has a great impact on mental health in this critical and vulnerable period (Janssen and Leblanc, 2010; Ahn and Fedewa, 2011; Biddle and Asare, 2011; Rodriguez-Ayllon et al., 2019). More precisely, several authors (Richman and Shaffer, 2000; Brettschneider, 2001; Liu et al., 2015) found that doing sports is associated with a more positive self-concept and increased self-esteem, both of which are reliable indicators of mental health. Bang et al. (2020) demonstrated that belonging to sports clubs predicted positive self-esteem and also improved school engagement. Prior work reported that sport activities increased feelings of physical competence, satisfaction with physical appearance, and resulted in positive body image, which can lead to an increase in overall self-esteem (Findlay and Bowker, 2009; Zamani Sani et al., 2016; Gomez-Baya et al., 2019). Biddle et al. (2019) demonstrated that physical activity tends to decrease symptoms of depression which is among the most common mental disorders in this age group (Saxena et al., 2013). Physical activity benefited the social aspects of mental health as adolescents could develop positive social relationships through their collective practice (see Eime et al., 2013). The lack of physical activity during adolescence is correlated with poor mental health (Hume et al., 2011; Gunnel et al., 2016). All in all, it appears that physical activity and sport have a positive impact on mental health and this is especially true regarding hedonic wellbeing (maximizing positive emotions and minimizing negative ones, Diener, 2000). Indeed, sports practices allow adolescents to experience positive emotions in various ways. The broaden-and-build theoretical framework (Fredrickson, 2001) posits that positive emotions have various beneficial effects on psychological functioning and we suppose that it can provide an explanation why and how adolescents can gradually benefit from sports club membership. According to this theory, positive emotions increase physical, personal, and social resources that mobilize and unleash their potentials. Positive affects can trigger broad, curious, and optimistic thought patterns as well as spontaneous and energetic behaviors. Positive emotions accumulate over time in ways that incrementally build people’s enduring resources.

In the light of the abundance of scientific studies, it is surprising that we have very little accumulated scientific knowledge about (1) the developmental aspects of these benefits, and (2) how physical activity or sports club influences daily negative and positive affects along adolescence. The present paper aims to fill this gap among adolescents aged between 10 and 18 and explore the potential affective benefits of belonging to a sports club throughout adolescence.

During and after physical activities, people experience various affective benefits that can contribute to their wellbeing. One of these is related to physical feeling states that capture sensory experiences (e.g., arousal). These experiences are distinct from the general positive/negative affects that might appear as the benefit of physical activities (Dunton et al., 2014). These two aspects have not the same duration, level of intensity and even the underlying cognitive processes are different. In the present study, instead of the sensory experiences, we will focus on core affects (Russell, 1980; Russell and Barrett, 1999); the most elementary, consciously accessible emotions and we will distinguish them based on their positive or negative valence. In the present case, positive affects included states, such as joy, enthusiasm, pride, and full-of-energy, while negative affects included anxiety, fear, sadness, shame, and guilt (Van Geel and De Mey, 2003). Our main goal was not assessing affects during or right after physical activities, but we aimed to explore these affective states in a more general way, focusing on adolescents’ feelings in the few days before the assessment.

There is a consensus in the literature that sports and exercise elevate positive and reduce negative affects among not only adults, but adolescents as well (see, for example, Buckworth et al., 2013). In a recent meta-analysis, Bourke et al. (2021) demonstrated positive associations between physical activity, feeling energized, positive emotions, and vitality among children and adolescents. Their participation in sports club can also increase their social abilities (Sabiston et al., 2016; Vella et al., 2017). Being a member of a sports club has a positive influence on the adolescents’ self-esteem (Brettschneider, 2001) and social competence (Howie et al., 2010). These benefits are important during the emotionally challenging times of adolescence. Considering the dynamics of emotions in this critical period of development, it appears that positive affects decline and negative affects increase throughout adolescence (Weinstein et al., 2007; Forbes et al., 2010; Frost et al., 2015; Martin-Krumm et al., 2017). Adolescents clearly benefit affectively from physical activities (Kühnhausen et al., 2013; Cushing et al., 2018), yet it is still unclear exactly in which part of this period they can benefit the most.

The Present Research

The present work focuses on an under-investigated topic at the intersection of developmental, positive, and sport psychology. The results will be interpreted in the positive psychological theoretical framework broaden-and-build. Namely, how sports club membership can be related to increased positive and reduced negative affects throughout adolescence. Contrasting to prior studies, the present work is particular in its relatively large sample of adolescents of various ages (ranged between 10 and 18).

Based on (1) the systematic review of Liao et al. (2015), physical activity and sports club participation (Miller and Hoffman, 2009) improve daily positive affective states; (2) robust prior findings show that adolescents experience more negative and less positive affects over time (Weinstein et al., 2007; Forbes et al., 2010; Frost et al., 2015; Martin-Krumm et al., 2017); and (3) the fact that adolescents affectively benefit from physical activities (e.g., Buckworth et al., 2013), we expect that in the later parts of adolescents will experience more affective benefits from sports club membership than the earlier parts of adolescents.

Materials and Methods

Procedure and Participants

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and with the approval of the National Ethics Committee Board (n°00012476–2021-28-05-109). Participants for this study were recruited through the project of the “French physical and mental health inventory” program. Besides the ethical permission, the data gathering, and its further use were approved by the National Commission on Informatics and Liberty (RF 1232206). As the respondents were minors, written and signed informed consent was obtained from their parents. Respondents of the survey were informed about the content of the investigation, and they were requested to indicate their intention for participation (provide assent).

The dataset was part of an extensive, multi-year data gathering (between 2008 and 2019). For the present paper, we only focus on the data of adolescents who reported their sports club membership status. The sample consisted of 17,337 adolescents (7,604 girls, 43.86%). Most of them (12,895, 74.39%) reported that they belong to a sports club and approximately one quarter of the participants did not belong to any sports club. The age range was 10–18years among both the members of the sports club (M=12.45years, SD=1.94) and those who are not members (M=12.45years, SD=1.95), and their mean age was almost identical (Mall=12.45years, SDall=1.94years). The sample was uneven regarding year groups.1

Measures

Positive and Negative Affects

Among other measurements (height, body mass, and physical fitness level), participants were requested to describe how much they experienced a set of positive and negative emotions in the past 3 or 4days. The survey was completed 30min after a physical fitness test. Seven adjectives described negative affects: angry, sad, anxious, ashamed, guilty, annoyed, and worried; five described positive ones: joyful, enthusiastic, proud, full-of-energy, and happy. Responses were provided on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much).2

Sports Club Participation

Before the assessment of physical fitness level and after the measurements of height and weight, the participants responded to the following question: “Are you a member of a sports club?” Respondents could answer this question with a simple “yes” or “no.” If the answer was “yes,” they could report the sport they practiced the most often.

Results

Analytic Strategy

We examined the effect of sports club participation and its interaction with age (as a continuous variable) on the positive and negative affects in separate models. For this purpose, OLS regressions were conducted with using R [as well as for plotting the figure (ggplot2)]. The data met the assumption of independent errors (Negative Durbin-Watson value=1.76 and Positive Durbin-Watson value=1.90). The histogram of standardized residuals showed that in both the positive and negative affect models, the data included approximately normally distributed errors. It was also the case regarding the normal P–P plots of standardized residuals. In the positive affect plot, points at the two extremes of age were not completely on the line, but close. Finally, both the positive and negative scatterplots of standardized residuals indicated that the data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and linearity.

Psychometric Properties of the Affect Measures

We supposed that the 12 items belong to two factors. Based on an exploratory factor analysis, conducted in R with varimax method as the two factors were almost independent (r=0.15), indicating two sharply separate factors (positive loadings ranged between 0.45 and 0.72; negative loadings range between 0.66 and 0.85, with the largest cross-loading of 0.13), a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted (in R, lavaan) with two first-ordered factors, with robust maximum likelihood estimator.

Main Results

Negative Affect

Adolescents who did not belong to any sports clubs experienced more negative affects then their peers belonging to a sports club, β=−0.09, t(17,337)=−4.33, 95%CI (−0.13; −0.04), p<0.001, d=0.09. However, the interaction between age and sports club participation was not significant (p=0.503), R2=0.003 see Figure 1A.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Negative and positive affects along sports club membership and age of adolescents. The left panel (A) depicts negative affect-related results, and it indicates a significant main effect of the sports club membership. The right panel (B) depicts the main effect of the sports club membership on positive affects, as well as its interaction with the age of the students showing that late adolescents experience more positive emotions than early adolescents if they belong to a sports club (compared to the lack of membership).

Positive Affect

Those respondents who indicated their sports club participation, experienced more positive affects in the few days before the assessment than their peers, β=0.20, 95%CI (0.16; 0.24), t(17,337)=9.83, p<0.001, d=0.20. Age also had a negative main effect on positive affect, β=−0.52, 95%CI (−0.60; −0.45), t(17,337)=−13.66, p<0.001, d=0.52. Examination of the interaction revealed that the sports club membership was associated with higher levels of positive affect among late adolescents than early adolescents (compared to those who do not belong to any sports clubs), β=0.18, t(17,337)=3.95, 95%CI (0.09; 0.26), p<0.001, d=0.18, R2=0.029, see Figure 1B. With using Fisher’s r-to-z comparison, we found that the negative relationship between age and positive affect was stronger (z=4.89, p<0.001) among students who do not belong to sports clubs (β=0.52, t=−13.40, p<0.001) compared to their peers who belong to sports clubs (β=−0.35, t=−15.52, p<0.001).

Discussion

Being a member of a sports club during adolescence has tremendous benefits as it is associated with enhanced social competence, a better health profile, and with more positive emotions and less negative ones (Brettschneider, 2001; Howie et al., 2010; Eime et al., 2013; Gisladottir et al., 2013a). Considering these solid prior results, our main goal was to examine a missing piece in the relevant literature at what age can adolescents benefit the most emotionally from sports club membership? Potential affective benefits for late adolescents—that the present results suggest—can be especially relevant, as many career and life decisions must be made in this period. In the following, we aim to provide some explanations for this effect, discuss some nuances, highlight some limitations, and propose some questions for future studies focusing on this topic.

The positive emotional balance deriving from practicing sports can be interpreted in the light of the broaden-and-build theory of Fredrickson (1998, 2001). More precisely, according to this theory, positive emotions can accumulate and build enduring resources over time. They can also create a spiral leading to an increased likelihood of experiencing further positive emotions in the future. For example, when adolescents are experiencing positive emotions, they become more resilient in coping with potentially emerging negative feelings. Therefore, it is possible that the affective benefits deriving from sports club membership can not only serve as a source of positive emotions, but also as an antidote to negative emotions.

The framework of the broaden-and-build theory can be connected to prior sport-related results. For example, adolescents engaged in a sports club progressively develop a positive physical self-image as the result of their satisfying body-related experiences, progress in motor control, and sport-related successes (Bowker, 2006; Gomez-Baya et al., 2019). The increased physical self-image can be also linked to physical competence and can also become a key component of global self-esteem (Marsh, 1986; Sonstroem, 1997; Fox, 2000; Richman and Shaffer, 2000; Bowker, 2006; Chen et al., 2012). To put it simply, such a potential chain reaction of physical activities makes adolescents feel good about themselves. It is possible that broad-scale positive effects do not happen overnight and require years of practice. Until now, we only discussed individual characteristics and aspects; however, adolescents are fundamentally social, and these sports club-related benefits can have an effect on their functioning in their social groups, as well.

The explanation of the broaden-and-build theory can be extended to the social aspects of physical activity, such as the sports club context. Within sports clubs, among various experiences, positive affects can be socially shared and can reinforce the bonds between club members (Rimé, 2009). In a recent study, Brown and Fredrickson (2021) put emphasis on co-experiencing positive emotions. Compared to emotions experienced individually, such collective experiences can lead to greater longevity and magnitude of the positive affects. Besides these immediate effects, teens can gradually develop a network of friends in a sports club that can prevent them from feeling lonely. Findlay and Coplan (2008) suggested that sports club participation may play a unique protective role for shy individuals who have difficulties initiating contact with peers. The social context of the sports clubs might not only increase the sense of belonging (Baumeister and Leary, 1995), but also it can serve as a “training ground” for developing social competencies (e.g., Bedard et al., 2020). Moreover, it appears that along with other competencies, social skills acquired in the sports clubs can help teens find their ways adaptively in their future lives (Fraser-Thomas and Côté, 2009; Strachan and Davies, 2015). The social benefits of a sports club membership can contribute to the recursive processes deriving from the broaden-and-build theory of Fredrickson (1998, 2001). Similarly, to the above-mentioned intra-individual benefits, social benefits also require time that can explain why late adolescents benefit most from sports club membership.

Normally, the adolescents’ life becomes more complicated over the teenage years. For example, Britni et al. (2021) found that prevalence of behavioral problems and internalizing disorders, such as anxiety or depression, emerge more frequently in the later years of adolescence. Slobodskaya (2021) found that over the years, adolescents become more aware of their environmental demands. In the later part of adolescence, teens might need more resources to cope with these progressively increasing difficulties. Sports clubs can provide opportunities to build both relevant and useful resources. However, it seems reasonable that utilizing the benefits of the sports club (e.g., positive self-esteem, belonging, and social support) requires time, and when the perceived difficulties peak in late adolescence they should be available.

One of the surprising findings was not observing an interaction of age and sports club membership regarding the negative affects. Based on the prior literature (Sabiston et al., 2016; Doré et al., 2019) and based on the broaden-and-build theory, one could expect that over the years of adolescence, teens belonging to sports clubs will report less and less daily negative affects compared to their non-sportive peers. Further research is needed to explore the potential reasons. One of the potential explanations might be that sports club membership can reduce the feelings of sadness, anxiety, or worries during adolescence equally. The beneficial effect of sports club membership can be hardly accumulated over the years as academic and social challenges increase in parallel with the passing years and sport activities can only maintain their affective benefits without any visible accumulation.

Limitations

This study has some limitations. First, regarding information on sports club practice, we only know whether the adolescents practiced in a sports club or not. No further information was collected on the number of training sessions per week, the level of practice in competition, and the number of years of practice. However, if they belonged to multiple clubs simultaneously, they were requested to indicate the one they spent the most time practicing with. All this information should be incorporated in future studies. In addition, the distribution between year groups was uneven and it was not controlled for in the analysis. Second, regarding every single student, affects were surveyed 30min after a physical fitness assessment that could have possibly—although homogeneously—influence the retrieval and evaluation of respondents’ affects regarding the previous few days. Although our affect measure showed consistent factor structure and great internal consistency. We have to mention that the temporal stability of the positive and negative factors of the affect measure has not been investigated yet. Future work might either use a more commonly utilized measure or it is also possible to validate this measure. It is also possible that future studies might try to use ecological momentary assessment (Trull and Ebner-Priemer, 2013) if they are interested in short term and less pervasive benefits. The relatively low explained variances of the regression models can also be considered as a limitation of this study.

Conclusion

In sum, it appears that in the light of the present and prior results, the “Build-and-Broaden Theory” of Fredrickson can provide some hints about the reasons why sports club membership contributes to more positive affects in late adolescence than early adolescence. This theory describes the form of positive emotions as to broaden awareness and their function as to build resources. To confirm these results, further studies with longitudinal and experimental methods are required that could appropriately measure the accumulation of potential affective benefits over time. This is not the only limitation of the present work. Alternatively, the use of other more recognized tools measures should also demonstrate the present results. It would be useful to consider the teens’ level of advancement in their sports club. It is also possible that certain sports have more, and others have less intra-individual and inter-individual affective benefits. Future studies could not only distinguish between these effects, but they can also examine teens’ different trajectories of development and the impact of sports club memberships on the affective landscape of various students. In sum, it appears that the present study only found a reasonable interaction effect; however, further studies are required to explain these preliminary results in detail.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of 00012476–2021-28-05-109. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

CA, AB, AC, JC, HO, and GO contributed to the study design, literature review, data gathering, manuscript writing, data analyses, and interpretation. All authors commented on the draft and contributed to the final version, approved the publication of the manuscript, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

GO was supported by the Young Researcher STARS grant from Conseil Régional Hauts de France. AB was supported by the CIFRE n°2020/0331 grant from the Association Nationale Recherche Technologie (ANRT).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Institut des Rencontres de la Forme (IRFO) for making the data available.

Footnotes

1. ^Eleven-year-old teens were overrepresented (39.92%), while the 17 years old were underrepresented (2.50%), while the other age groups were the following: 10 years old 4.44%, 12 year old 21.43%, 13 year old 9.81%, 14 years old 7.10%, 15 years old 7.77%, 16 years old 4.19%, and 17 years old 2.82%.

2. ^Our items are very similar to other well-established instruments, such as the Multiple Affect Adjective Check List (Zuckerman and Lubin, 1965), the Profile of Mood States (McNair et al., 1971), and the PANAS (Watson and Clark, 1988).

References

Ahn, S., and Fedewa, A. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the relationship between children’s physical activity and mental health. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 36, 385–397. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bang, H., Wong, D., and Park, S. (2020). School engagement, self-esteem, and depression of adolescents: the role of sport participation and volunteering activity and gender differences. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 113:105012. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong. Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedard, C., Hanna, S., and Cairney, J. (2020). A longitudinal study of sport participation and perceived social competence in youth. J. Adolesc. Health 66, 352–359. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.09.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biddle, S. J. H., and Asare, M. (2011). Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: A review of reviews. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 886–895. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biddle, S. J. H., Ciaccioni, S., Thomas, G., and Vergeer, I. (2019). Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: A review of reviews and an analysis of causality. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 42, 146–155. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biddle, S. J. H., and Mutrie, N. (2001). Psychology of Physical Activity: Determinants, Well-Being and Interventions. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Bourke, M., Hilland, T. A., and Craike, M. (2021). A systematic review of the within-person association between physical activity and affect in children’s and adolescents’ daily lives. Psychol. Sport Exercise 52:101825. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowker, A. (2006). The relationship between sports participation and self-esteem during early adolescence. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 38, 214–229. doi: 10.1037/cjbs2006009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brettschneider, W. (2001). Effects of sport club activities on adolescent development in Germany. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 1, 1–11. doi: 10.1080/17461390100071201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britni, R., Belcher, J. Z., Azad, A., Campbell, C. E., Sandhya, C. P., and Herting, M. (2021). The roles of physical activity, exercise, and fitness in promoting resilience During adolescence: effects on mental well-being and brain development. Biol. Psychiatry Cognit. Neurosci. Neuro. 6, 225–237. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C., and Fredrickson, B. (2021). Characteristics and consequences of co-experienced positive affect: understanding the origins of social skills, social bonds, and caring, healthy communities. Behav. Sci. 39, 58–63. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckworth, J., Dishman, R. K., O’Connor, P. J., and Tomporowski, P. D. (2013). Exercise Psychology. 2nd Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Chen, W., Chen, C., Lin, Y., and Chen, T. (2012). Sport participation and self-esteem as mediated by perceived peer acceptance and sport self-concept in Taiwanese college students. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 40, 699–704. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2012.40.4.699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cushing, C. C., Bejarano, C. M., Mitchell, T. B., Noser, A. E., and Crick, C. J. (2018). Individual differences in negative affectivity and physical activity in adolescents: An ecological momentary assessment study. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 2772–2779. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1128-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55, 34–43. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doré, I., Sabiston, C. M., Sylvestre, M.-P., Brunet, J., O’Loughlin, J., Nader, P. A., et al. (2019). Years participating in sports During childhood predicts mental health in adolescence: A 5-year longitudinal study. J. Adolesc. Health Off. Pub. Soc. Adolesc. Med. 64, 790–796. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.11.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunton, G. F., Huh, J., Leventhal, A. M., Riggs, N., Hedeker, D., and Spruijt-Metz, D. (2014). Momentary assessment of affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in children. Health Psychol. 33, 255–263. doi: 10.1037/a0032640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eime, R. M., Young, J. A., Harvey, J. A., Charity, M. J., and Payne, W. R. (2013). A systematic review of the psychological and social benefits of participation in sport for children and adolescents: informing development of a conceptual model of health through sport. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 10:98. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlay, L., and Bowker, A. (2009). The link between competitive sport participation and self-concept in early adolescence: A consideration of gender and sport orientation. J. Youth Adolesc. 38, 29–40. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9244-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlay, L. C., and Coplan, R. J. (2008). Come out and play: shyness in childhood and the benefits of organized sports participation. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 40, 153–161. doi: 10.1037/0008-400X.40.3.153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, E. E., Ryan, N. D., Phillips, M. L., Manuck, S. B., Worthman, C. M., Moyles, D. L., et al. (2010). Healthy adolescents’ neural response to reward: associations with puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 162–172. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201002000-00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. R. (2000). “The effects of exercise on self-perceptions and self-esteem,” in Physical Activity and Psychological Well-Being. eds. S. J. H. Biddle, K. R. Fox, and S. H. Boutcher (New York: Routledge), 88–117.

Google Scholar

Fraser-Thomas, J., and Côté, J. (2009). Understanding adolescents’ positive and negative developmental experiences in sport. Sport Psychol. 23, 3–23. doi: 10.1123/tsp.23.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, A., Hoyt, L. T., Chung, A. L., and Adam, E. K. (2015). Daily life with depressive symptomes: gender differences in adolescents’ everyday emotional experiences. J. Adolesc. 43, 132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisladottir, M., Haga, M., and Sigmundsson, H. (2013a). Physical fitness measures among adolescents with high and low motor competence. Sage J. 3:2158244013500282. doi: 10.1177/2158244013500282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisladottir, T., Matthíasdóttir, Á., and Kristjánsdóttir, H. (2013b). The effect of adolescents’ sports clubs participation on self-reported mental and physical conditions and future expectations. J. Sports Sci. 31, 1139–1145. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.773402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Baya, D., Mendoza, R., de Matos, M. G., and Tomico, A. (2019). Sport participation, body satisfaction and depressive symptoms in adolescence: A moderated-mediation analysis of gender differences. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 16, 183–197. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2017.1364988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnel, K. E., Flament, M. F., Buchholz, A., Henderson, K. A., Obeid, N., Schubert, N., et al. (2016). Examining the bidirectional relationship between physical activity, screen time, and symptoms of anxiety and depression over time during adolescence. Prev. Med. 88, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haworth, J., and Lewis, S. (2005). Work, leisure and well-being. Br. J. Guid. Couns. 33, 67–79. doi: 10.1080/03069880412331335902

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howie, L. J. D., Lukacs, S. L., Pastor, P. N., Reuben, C. A., and Mendola, P. (2010). Participation in activities outside of school hours in relation to problem behavior and social skills in middle childhood. J. Sch. Health 80, 119–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00475.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hume, C., Timperio, A., Veitch, J., Salmon, J., Crawford, D., and Ball, K. (2011). Physical activity, sedentary behavior, and depressive symptoms among adolescents. J. Phy. Activity Health 8, 152–156. doi: 10.1123/jpah.8.2.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, I., and LeBlanc, A. G. (2010). Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 7:40. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnhausen, J., Leonhardt, A., Dirk, J., and Schmiedek, F. (2013). Physical activity and affect in elementary school children’s daily lives. Front. Psychol. 4:456. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, Y., Shonkoff, E. T., and Dunton, G. F. (2015). The acute relationships between affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: A review of current evidence. Front. Psychol. 6:1975. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, M., Wu, L., and Ming, Q. (2015). How does physical activity intervention improve self-esteem and self-concept in children and adolescents? Evidence from a meta-analysis. PLoS One 10:e0134804. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1986). Global self-esteem: its relation to specific facets of self-concept and their importance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1224–1236. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Krumm, C., Fenouillet, F., Csillik, A., Kern, L., Besançon, M., Heutte, J., et al. (2017). Changes in emotions from childhood to Young adulthood. Child Indic. Res. 11, 541–561. doi: 10.1007/s12187-016-9440-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., and Dropplman, L. F. (1971). Manual of Profile of Mood States. San Diego, California: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.

Google Scholar

Miller, K. E., and Hoffman, J. H. (2009). Mental well-being and sport-related identities in college students. Sociol. Sport J. 26, 335–356. doi: 10.1123/ssj.26.2.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richman, E., and Shaffer, D. (2000). “If you let me play sports” how might sport participation influence the self-esteem of adolescent females? Psychol. Women Quartely 24, 189–199. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb00200.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: theory and empirical review. Emot. Rev. 1, 60–85. doi: 10.1177/1754073908097189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Ayllon, M., Cadenas-Sanchez, C., Estevez-Lopez, F., Munoz, N., Gonzales, J.-M., Migueles, J., et al. (2019). Role of physical activity and sedentary behavior in the mental health of Preschoolers, children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 49, 1383–1410. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01099-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A., and Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 805–819. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabiston, C. M., Jewett, R., Ashdown-Franks, G., Belanger, M., Brunet, J., and O’Loughlin, E. (2016). Number of years of team and individual sport participation during adolescence and depressive symptoms in early adulthood. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 105–110. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxena, S., Funk, M., and Chishlom, D. (2013). World health assembly adopts comprehensive mental health action. Lancet 381, 1970–1971. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61139-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slobodskaya, H. R. (2021). Personality development from early childhood through adolescence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 172:110596. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonstroem, R. J. (1997). “The physical self-system: A mediator of exercise and self-esteem,” in The Physical Self: From Motivation to Well-Being. ed. K. R. Fox (United Kingdom: Human Kinetics), 3–26.

Google Scholar

Strachan, L., and Davies, K. (2015). Click! Using photo elicitation to explore youth experiences and positive youth development in sport. Qualitative Res. Sport Exercise Health 7, 170–191. doi: 10.1080/2159676X.2013.867410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trull, T. J., and Ebner-Priemer, U. (2013). Ambulatory assessment. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 151–176. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Geel, R., and De Mey, H. (2003). Self, other, positive, and negative affect scales of the self-confrontation method: factorial structure and unidimensionality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 35, 1833–1847. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00033-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vella, S. A., Swann, C., Allen, M. S., Schweickle, M. J., and Magee, C. A. (2017). Bidirectional associations between sport involvement and mental health in adolescence. Med. Sci. Sports Exercise 49:687694. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000001142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Clark, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, S. M., Mermelstein, R. J., Hankin, B. L., Hedeker, D., and Flay, B. R. (2007). Longitudinal patterns of daily affect and global mood During adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 17, 587–600. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00536.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamani Sani, S. H., Fathirezaie, Z., Brand, S., Pühse, U., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., Gerber, M., et al. (2016). Physical activity and self-esteem: testing direct and indirect relationships associated with psychological and physical mechanisms. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 12, 2617–2625. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S116811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M., and Lubin, B. (1965). Normative data for the multiple affect adjective check list. Psychol. Rep. 16, 438–438. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1965.16.2.438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adolescents, positive affect, sports club membership, positive psychology, broaden-and-build, physical activity

Citation: Carton A, Barbry A, Coquart J, Ovigneur H, Amoura C and Orosz G (2021) Sport-Related Affective Benefits for Teenagers Are Getting Greater as They Approach Adulthood: A Large-Scale French Investigation. Front. Psychol. 12:738343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.738343

Received: 08 July 2021; Accepted: 08 September 2021;
Published: 11 October 2021.

Edited by:

Nebojša Trajković, University of Niš, Serbia

Reviewed by:

Ivana Pedović, University of Niš, Serbia
Drazenka Macak, University of Novi Sad, Serbia

Copyright © 2021 Carton, Barbry, Coquart, Ovigneur, Amoura and Orosz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gabor Orosz, gabor.orosz@univ-artois.fr

These authors have contributed equally to this work

ORCID: Alexis Barbry; orcid.org/0000-0003-3991-9420; Annie Carton; orcid.org/0000-0002-6457-3348; Jérémy Coquart; orcid.org/0000-0001-6515-7736; Camille Amoura; orcid.org/0000-0001-9639-1461; Gábor Orosz

orcid.org/0000-0001-5883-6861

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.