- 1Graduate School of Education, Peking University, Beijing, China
- 2School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, China
- 3Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong SAR, China
- 4Institute of Curriculum and Instruction, Faculty of Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
As past studies of self-control and Internet addiction showed mixed results, this meta-analysis of 83 primary studies with 80,681 participants determined whether (a) these students with less self-control had greater Internet addiction, and (b) age, culture, gender, Internet addiction measures, or year moderated these relations. We used a random-effects meta-analysis of Pearson product-moment coefficients r with Fisher’s z-transformation and tested for moderation with the homogeneity tests. The results showed a positive link between impulsivity and Internet addiction (r = 0.371, 95% CI = [0.311, 0.427]) and a negative link between restraint and Internet addiction (r = −0.362, 95% CI = [−0.414, −0.307]). The moderation analysis indicated that the correlation between impulsivity indicators and greater Internet addiction was stronger among undergraduates (18–22 years old) than among adolescents (10–17 years old). Furthermore, the negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was greater (a) among students in East Asia than those in Western Europe/North America, (b) among males than females and (c) when using the Internet addiction measures GPIUS or IAT rather than CIAS. Hence, these results indicate a negative link between self-control and Internet addiction, and this link is moderated by age, culture, gender, and Internet addiction measure.
Introduction
The Internet has become an indispensable part in people’s daily life, which brought us both positive and negative effects. One of the typical negative phenomena concerning excessive use and abuse of computers or the Internet is Internet addiction. A meta-analysis has found that an estimated 6% of adolescents worldwide have suffered from Internet addiction (Cheng and Li, 2014). It is an impulse-control disorder that does not involve an intoxicant (Young, 1998a, 2004), which has been characterized by poorly-controlled preoccupations, urges, or behaviors regarding Internet access that lead to impairment or distress (Shaw and Black, 2008). Some studies have indicated that Internet addiction disrupts adolescents’ time management, especially daily sleep and exercise routines, which weakens their immune system (Rosen et al., 2014), interferes with their social relationships (Tsitsika et al., 2014), hinders academic achievement (Chou et al., 2005), and increases their likelihood of depression symptoms (Lam and Peng, 2010).
Self-control is defined as an inhibition capacity to regulate one’s thoughts, emotions, and behaviors in the face of external demands (DeLisi, 2014). Given the dual-systems model of self-control (Hofmann et al., 2009) and some empirical evidence like the Brief Self-Control Scale (Dvorak and Simons, 2009; Maloney et al., 2012), the domain of self-control can be divided into two dimensions: restraint and impulsivity (Carver, 2005). When facing temptation, individuals need to deal with two opposite forces, namely, the reflective force that promotes individuals to behave reasonably and the impulsive force that encourages individuals to satisfy their desires (Hofmann et al., 2009). Thus, restraint and impulsivity are distinct processes affecting self-regulatory outcomes (Strack and Deutsch, 2004; Carver, 2005; Hofmann et al., 2009). According to impulsiveness theory (Ainslie, 1975), as students with greater control over their emotions, thoughts, and behaviors are often less impulsive (Peluso et al., 1999), they might engage in fewer short-term behaviors, such impulsive Internet addiction, that conflict with their long-term goals. However, other researchers argue that self-control is unrelated to Internet addiction (Iftikhar and Tariq, 2014; Öğütçü et al., 2016), and past studies of this possible link showed mixed results (Kim et al., 2008; Tao and Li, 2009; Błachnio and Przepiorka, 2016), possibly due to moderator effects. To synthesize these results, this meta-analysis of past studies during 2002–2019 determines (a) the overall link between self-control and Internet addiction and (b) whether culture, age, gender, Internet addiction measure or year moderates this link.
Self-Control and Internet Addiction
We first discuss why self-control might be linked to Internet addiction. Then, we consider how culture, age, gender, Internet addiction measures or publication year might moderate such links.
Restraint, Impulsivity, and Internet Addiction
According to impulsiveness theory (Ainslie, 1975), some students have more restraint than others and are less impulsive (Peluso et al., 1999), so they are more likely to sacrifice short-term amusement to invest effort toward attaining long-term goals. For example, students with less restraint than others are more likely to avoid studying for the next day’s test by watching videos on the Internet. (Not all uses of the internet are harmful; for example, a student can search for relevant information on the Internet for an essay comparing theocracy in Iran and monarchy in North Korea). By contrast, students with less restraint than others are more likely to make decisions based primarily on short-term benefits (such as watching an Internet video) rather than on investments for long-term benefits, like studying for a test (Metcalfe and Mischel, 1999; Duckworth and Steinberg, 2015). If such students with less restraint have few other means to satisfy their emotional needs, they might increasingly use the Internet to do so (Ponton and Rhea, 2006). Thus, their repeated decisions to enjoy Internet use can foster Internet addiction (Baumeister et al., 2010; Shek and Lu, 2012). In contrast, other researchers argue that such decisions need not be impulsive, so that self-control might be unrelated to Internet addiction (e.g., Hur, 2006). In other words, when students are addicted to the Internet, they make rational decisions because they know the characteristics of the Internet as anonymity, convenience, and escape, not because of impulse (Young, 1998a).
Past studies of self-control and Internet addiction have shown mixed results. Some studies have revealed that restraint indicators (e.g., impulse control and resist temptation) are negatively related to Internet addiction (Larose et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2008; Li et al., 2014). Also, other studies have demonstrated that impulsivity indicators (e.g., impulsivity and temper) are positively related to Internet addiction (Gao and Zhao, 2009; Tao and Li, 2009).
However, many other studies have shown no significant relationship. Some studies have found no significant link between restraint indicators (e.g., self-control, self-regulation, etc.) and Internet addiction (Iftikhar and Tariq, 2014; Lee and Cho, 2015; Błachnio and Przepiorka, 2016). Likewise, other studies have shown no significant link between impulsivity indicators (e.g., impulsivity, dyscontrol, etc.) and Internet addiction (Choi et al., 2013; Öğütçü et al., 2016; Zhou, 2017). Although moderators (e.g., culture, age, and gender) might account for some of these differences in results, most studies did not test for them (Teng et al., 2014). Also, some of these differences in results might stem from using different measures (restraint vs. impulsivity indicators, different Internet addiction measures) or across the years of the studies. Hence, we consider possible moderation effects in the next section.
Moderation: Culture, Age, Gender, Internet Addiction Measures, and Year
As many studies include information on the participants’ culture, age, gender, internet addiction measures and year, our meta-analysis can test for their moderation effects. Hence, we discuss the potential moderation of each variable.
Culture
A society’s cultural values might moderate the link between self-control and Internet addiction. While some societies highlight short-term goals [e.g., United States (US)], others encourage attention to long-term goals (e.g., China; Hofstede et al., 2008). As emphasis of long-term goals can reinforce their importance to students with restraint, they might attend to and devote effort to these long-term goals (Dewitte and Cremer, 2001), thereby further reducing the attractiveness of short-term Internet amusements and the likelihood of Internet addiction. For example, the correlation between restraint and Internet addiction is weak among university students in the United Kingdom (Mehroof and Griffiths, 2010) but much stronger among those in mainland China (Du, 2013). However, other studies show strong negative links in the United States (Khang et al., 2013) and the Netherlands (Van Deursen et al., 2015) but weak negative links in South Korea (Lee and Cho, 2015).
In contrast, students with low self-control are likely to ignore such long-term goals, so this cultural value is not likely to influence them (Metcalfe and Mischel, 1999). Hence, the link between impulsivity indicators and Internet addiction is likely substantial across countries. For example, studies show moderate to large links between impulsivity indicators and Internet addiction in Australia (Chou and Ting, 2003), Canada (Davis et al., 2002), mainland China (Bai and Xu, 2009), Hungary (Demetrovics et al., 2016), and South Korea (Choi et al., 2014).
Thus, a society with a long-term orientation might further strengthen the negative link between restraint and Internet addiction. However, this cultural value might not moderate the positive link between impulsivity and Internet addiction. Hence, this meta-analysis tested whether the link between self-control and Internet addiction differed across countries.
Age
Among low self-control students, younger students’ impulsivity might render them more vulnerable to Internet addiction (Wang et al., 2017), but university students’ loss of friendships and parental monitoring might increase their loneliness and likelihood of seeking solace on the Internet (Throuvala et al., 2019; Teng et al., 2020). On the one hand, as children grow into adults, their brains develop; specifically, their anterior insulas become thinner (Churchwell and Yurgelun-Todd, 2013), so they become less impulsive and plan more (Steinberg et al., 2008; Kasen et al., 2011). As a result, less impulsive, older students might be less susceptible to short-term Internet amusements and hence less likely to suffer from Internet addiction.
On the other hand, university students often move into a new environment without their family and old friends (Asendorpf, 2002). So, university students might feel lonelier and have less parent monitoring than otherwise, both of which might encourage impulsive university students to engage in more Internet activity and subsequently suffer from Internet addiction (Bozoglan et al., 2013; Yao et al., 2014). When students move away to university, they generally have fewer opportunities to keep their parents and childhood friends company, and making new friends might require substantial effort (Mattanah et al., 2010). As a result, they often miss their family (homesickness) and friends (friendsickness, Paul and Brier, 2001) and feel lonely (Larose and Boivin, 1998). Such lonely university students with impulsivity are easily tempted by the ready availability and immediate gratification of Internet entertainment. Without their parents or other family members nearby to monitor them (Henderson and Mapp, 2002), such impulsive university students might spend excessive time on the Internet and become addicted.
In summary, the research literature suggests two conflicting hypotheses. On the one hand, younger children are more impulsive than older children, more inclined to indulge in immediate Internet enjoyment excessively, and thus more likely to become addicted. On the other hand, older university students might feel lonely, use the Internet excessively without parent monitoring, and become addicted. As students with restraint are more disciplined and more focused on long-term goals, they are likely less susceptible to short-term amusements and Internet addiction. As few studies examined students at different ages, this meta-analysis can help determine whether age moderates the link between self-control and Internet addiction.
Gender
Unlike males, females pay more attention to their peers, monitor one another’s actions more, and are more influenced by one another (Rudman and Goodwin, 2004; Wills et al., 2004). In contrast, males are less attentive to their peers and less influenced by them, relying more on their judgments (Charness and Rustichini, 2011). As many males rely more on themselves than on their peers compared with females, males’ self-control is more likely to affect their behaviors and outcomes (Benda, 2013; Koon–Magnin et al., 2016), such as Internet addiction. Hence, we expect the link between self-control and Internet addiction to be stronger for males than for females.
Internet Addiction Measures and Year
In this study, we examine whether the following three Internet addiction measures moderate findings regarding the relationship between self-control and Internet addiction. Most past studies collected data on Internet addiction via surveys. For example, Young’s (1998b) Internet Addiction Test (IAT) comprises 20 items with a five-point scoring system, in which normal Internet users typically have a total score below 50 points. Higher total scores indicate greater severity of Internet addiction. Caplan’s (2002) Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale (GPIUS) consists of 29 items with a five-point scoring system, capturing mood alteration, social benefits, negative outcomes, compulsivity, excessive time, withdrawal, and interpersonal control. Chen et al.’s (2003) Chinese Internet Addiction Scale (CIAS) has 26 items with a four-point scale and focuses on five constructs: compulsive use, withdrawal, tolerance, interpersonal relationship difficulties and health/time management difficulties. Other Internet addiction scales [e.g., Brenner’s (1997) Internet-Related Addictive Behavior Inventory (IRABI)] have not been widely used to study self-control and Internet addiction.
The internet has substantially changed over time, especially the increase in social media activities (Leung, 2014). Hence, we also test for differences across years.
Purpose of This Study
This study aims to (a) synthesize the results of past studies on the relation between self-control and Internet addiction among students (10–22 years old) and (b) identify factors that influence this relationship. Specifically, this meta-analysis (a) calculates the overall effect size of the link between self-control and Internet addiction and (b) determines whether culture, age, gender, Internet addiction measures or publication year moderated this link.
Materials and Methods
Literature Search
This meta-analysis included Chinese- and English-language publications during January 2002 to September 2020. The Chinese-language articles were retrieved from the China Academic Journals Full-text Database, the China Selected Doctoral Dissertations and Masters’ Thesis Full-text Database, and Wanfang Data. We used the keywords ‘self-control (自我控制),’ ‘self-regulation (自我调节),’ and ‘impulse control (冲动控制).’ For Internet addiction, we used ‘Internet addiction (网络成瘾),’ ‘addiction (成瘾),’ ‘Internet abuse (网络滥用),’ and ‘Internet dependence (网络依赖).’
The English-language articles were retrieved from the Google Scholar, ProQuest Dissertations, Taylor Francis, Springer, Web of Science, PsycINFO, EBSCO, Elsevier SDOL. For self-control, we used the keywords ‘self-control,’ ‘self-regulation,’ ‘impulse control,’ and ‘impulse.’ For Internet addiction, we used ‘Internet addiction,’ ‘addiction,’ ‘Internet abuse,’ and ‘Internet dependence.’ These searches initially retrieved 279 articles.
Study Selection via Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
The articles were screened according to inclusion and exclusion criteria (Azer and Azer, 2015, see Figure 1). The inclusion criteria were: (a) it reported the relation between self-control and Internet addiction; (b) it reported either Pearson’s product-moment coefficients r, β, T, or F-values (the latter two can be converted to r-values); (c) it reported the sample size; (d) the sample predominantly comprised adolescents or university students; (e) when multiple publications use a data set, the result that used the complete data was used (based on reading the article titles, abstracts, and full text); (f) if multiple publications used the same data set, journal articles are preferred over conference proceedings, which were preferred over dissertations. The exclusion criteria were: (g) editorials, letters to the editor, opinion pieces, commentaries, personal views, and abstracts only, (h) review papers, (i) studies not written in English or Chinese.
We trained two reviewers to separately apply the inclusion and exclusion criteria to the studies. After the initial screening, 98 studies were identified by both reviewers. One reviewer also identified 6 additional studies. Both reviewers discussed the 6 studies, agreed to keep 4 of them, yielding a final set of 83 studies.
Coding Variables
The collected articles were coded for the following features: internet addiction measures, author, publication year, sample size, proportion of female students, culture, ages, and r effect size (see Table 1). The studies used one of three Internet addiction measures: GPIUS (Caplan, 2002), IAT (Young, 1998b), CIAS (Chen et al., 2003), and others (e.g., IRABI). Furthermore, Internet addiction’s correlation with low self-control indicator or high self-control was encoded. If multiple effect sizes were obtained for the link between self-control and Internet addiction in the same sample, only the overall effect size was used. Also, the relation between self-control and Internet addiction was encoded among different groups of participants. If multiple methods were used to measure the relation between self-control and Internet addiction in the same study, the most statistically accurate one was used (e.g., β preferred over r). Comparison of the results using each encoding method showed high consistency.
We used the country of the study as a rough proxy for culture. Specifically, culture was coded as ‘East Asia,’ ‘Western Europe/North America,’ or ‘Others’ (Lei et al., 2018). ‘East Asia’ referred to studies of students in Asian countries such as China (including Hong Kong and Taiwan), South Korea, Indonesia, Thailand, Japan, Vietnam, Pakistan, and so on. ‘Western Europe/North America’ referred to students in European and North American countries such as the European Union (EU), Dutch, United Kingdom, the United States of America, Canada, Australia, and so on. ‘Others’ referred to students in Saudi Arabia, Iran, and so on. We also test for differences across countries within a culture. Age was coded as adolescent (10–17 years; Olds et al., 2009) or university (undergraduate) student (18–22 years). We also recorded the proportions of female students and male students. As the studies span nearly two decades, we test for differences across years.
Assessment of Study Quality
The literature quality was assessed with the Medical Education Research Study Quality Instrument (MERSQI) (Cook and Reed, 2015). Although developed for medical education, MERSQI is discipline neutral and hence suitable for assessing the quality of non-medical education research (Jensen and Konradsen, 2018). The ten items of MERSQI cover six domains: (a) study design, (b) sampling, (c) type of data, (d) validity evidence for evaluation instrument scores, (e) data analysis, and (f) outcome. As each domain has a maximum score of 3, the maximum total score for a study is 18. The mean consensus MERSQI score was 10.322, with a standard deviation of 1.08 and a median score of 10.0, indicating fair overall study quality. Total consensus MERSQI scores for each paper are shown in Table 1.
Meta-Analysis
In this standard meta-analysis, effect sizes between affective self-control and Internet addiction were calculated for each sample (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). Then, we tested whether the links between affective self-control and Internet addiction were moderated by culture, age, Internet addiction measure, gender, or year.
Effect Size Calculation
Meta-analysis of Pearson’s product-moment coefficients r yielded the effect size (Borenstein et al., 2010). Fisher’s z-transformation was applied to r, weighted by the sample size with 95% confidence intervals: Z = 0.5* ln [(1 + r)/(1 − r)], where the variance of Z is VZ = 1/n − 3 and the standard deviation of Z is SEZ = (1/n − 3)0.5. The effect size is rz = (e2 × z − 1)/(e2 × z + 1). The confidence interval is computed as follows:
rU = + Z(1 − a) × SEes
= − Z(1 − a) × SEes
SEes = √1/ΣWi
Wi = ni − 3
The homogeneity calculation formula is computed as follows:
Qw = ΣWi (rZi − )2
I2 = [Q − (K − 1)]/Q × 100%.
Moreover, this study follows the normative guidelines for interpreting the magnitude of a correlation proposed by Gignac and Szodorai (2016) with the quantitative investigation that has avoided the subjectivity of Cohen’s (1988) effect size guidelines: 0.10, 0.20, and 0.30 for small, medium, and large correlations.
Data Processing and Analysis
We used the comprehensive meta-analysis software CMA 2.01. To test our hypotheses, we calculated sample sizes (k), weighted effect sizes (r), and 95% confidence intervals. We tested whether the mean effect sizes of the studies differed significantly (homogeneity test) via Cochrane’s Q and I2 (Huedo-Medina et al., 2006). If I2 exceeds 75, the effect sizes show significant heterogeneity; then, a random-effects model would be more suitable than a fixed-effects model for this meta-analysis (in a random-effects model, the selected studies are treated as random samples from a larger population to help generalize the findings). Averaged weighted correlation coefficients (within- and between-inverse-variance weights) of independent samples were used to compute mean effect sizes. If the homogeneity test showed significant variance in effect sizes between different samples’ characteristics, we tested for moderators. When the homogeneity test was significant (QBET > 0.05), post hoc analysis confirmed the different groups statistically. To examine moderator effects, we used meta-ANOVA for categorical variables and meta-regression analysis for continuous variables.
Results
Effect Size and the Homogeneity Test
After filtering the literature, we used 83 independent samples and calculated 85 effect sizes (46 between impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction, and 39 between restraint indicator and Internet addiction). In all, 80,681 students participated in the studies reviewed; sample sizes of individual studies ranged from 21 to 18,709.
The results indicated that impulsivity was positively related to Internet addiction (r = 0.371) and restraint was negatively related to Internet addiction (r = −0.362). These effect sizes were sufficiently large for moderator analysis (shown in Table 2).
Forest plots summarize the results of all studies (using 95% confidence intervals of the standardized difference in means). See Figures 2, 3.
Publication Bias
We used funnel plots and Egger’s regressions (Egger et al., 1997) to test whether the results were biased. Both funnel plots of the correlation coefficients of impulsivity and restraint indicator with Internet addiction had symmetric distributions on both sides of the means, showing no publication bias (see Figures 4, 5).
Figure 4. Funnel plot of effect sizes of the correlation between impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction.
Figure 5. Funnel plot of effect sizes of the correlation between restraint indicator and Internet addiction.
Egger’s regressions for Internet addiction with both low self-control and high self-control indicators revealed no publication bias [t(46)LSCI = 1.106, p = 0.273; t(39)HSCI = 1.702, p = 0.097). Together, these findings suggested that the overall correlation between self-control and Internet addiction was stable, and provided no evidence of publication bias for these studies.
Moderator Analysis
To test for moderators of the links between self-control and Internet addiction, we conducted two homogeneity tests, one across the 58 independent samples with impulsivity indicators and one across the 46 independent samples with restraint indicators. The results showed significant homogeneity coefficients and hence, significant moderation of the links between self-control indicators and Internet addiction [QT(46)Ipulsivity = 2609.769, p < 0.001; QT(39)Restraint = 771.822, p < 0.001). Next, we tested for moderators, using meta-ANOVA for categorical variables (internet addiction measure, culture [and separate countries], age) and meta-regression analysis for continuous variables (% female, year).
Culture
The homogeneity coefficient showed that culture did not moderate the positive link between impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction (East Asia vs. Western Europe/North America, QBET = 1.294, df = 1, p > 0.05, see Table 3). However, the homogeneity test found significant differences in the correlation between restraint indicator and Internet addiction across the two cultures (QBET = 6.096, df = 1, p < 0.05); in this case, the link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger in East Asia (r = −0.400) than in Western Europe/North America (r = −0.239). (Further analyses of studies by country showed no significant differences across countries within culture, results are available upon request).
Age
The homogeneity test (QBET = 12.572, df = 2, p < 0.01) suggested that age moderated the link between impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction (see Table 3). The positive link between an impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction was significantly stronger for undergraduates (r = 0.408) than for adolescents (r = 0.218). However, the homogeneity test (QBET = 4.548, df = 1, p > 0.05) suggested that age did not moderate the link between restraint indicator and Internet addiction.
Internet Addiction Measures
The homogeneity coefficient showed that Internet addiction measures did not moderate the relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction (QBET = 2.570, df = 3, p > 0.05, see Table 3). However, the homogeneity test found significant differences in the link between high restraint indicator and Internet addiction across the Internet addiction measures (QBET = 7.626, df = 3, p > 0.05); in this case, the relationship was weaker among CIAS, compared to other internet addiction measures (| rCIAS| < | rIAT| < | rOthers| < | rGPIUS| : | −0.198| < | −0.363| < | −0.410| < | −0.423|).
Gender
To examine whether continuous variables (gender) moderated the links between self-control and Internet addiction, the r effect size was meta-regressed onto the percentage of female participants in each sample. Gender did not moderate the link between an impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction (QModel [1, k = 43] = 3.012, p > 0.05, see Table 4, top half). In contrast, gender moderated the link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction (QModel [1, k = 33] = 12.220, p < 0.001), showing a stronger link for an all-male sample (r = −0.460) than an all-female sample (r = −0.300).
Year
Year was not a significant moderator. Year moderated neither the impulsivity indicator’s link with Internet addiction (QModel [1, k = 45] = 0.090, p > 0.05, see Table 4, bottom half) nor the restraint indicator’s link with Internet addiction (QModel [1, k = 38] = 0.031, p > 0.05).
Discussion
As past studies of self-control and Internet addiction showed mixed results, this meta-analysis synthesized the results of such studies during 2002–2019. These results showed that restraint indicators were negatively linked to Internet addiction and that impulsivity indicators were positively linked to Internet addiction. Hence, self-control was negatively related to Internet addiction. Moreover, these links differed across culture, age, and gender. The negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger for students in East Asia than in Western Europe/North America. Also, the positive link between an impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction was greater among university students than among adolescents. Lastly, the negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger among males than females.
Self-Control and Internet Addiction
Self-control was negatively linked to Internet addition, supporting Ainslie’s (1975) theory of impulsiveness. Specifically, these results are consistent with the view that students with more self-control than others are less impulsive (Peluso et al., 1999), and hence, less likely to engage in short-term behaviors that can yield Internet addiction and more likely to make short-term sacrifices/investment for long-term goals. Thus, these meta-analysis results suggest that self-control is an essential component of a comprehensive theory of Internet addiction (Lei et al., 2020). Moreover, this result suggests a possible intervention; specifically, future studies can determine whether interventions to enhance students’ self-control can reduce their Internet addiction.
Moderation
Culture, age, gender, and Internet addiction measures moderated the links between self-control and Internet addiction. These results are consistent with short-term versus long-term orientation cultures, university student loneliness and reduced parent monitoring, and self-reliant males.
Culture
Culture moderated Internet addiction’s link with restraint indicators but not its link with impulsivity indicators. The negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger for students in East Asia than in Western Europe/North America. This result is consistent with the view that long-term orientation cultures (e.g., China; Hofstede et al., 2008) emphasize long-term goals, which largely supports students with high self-control to attend to and work toward them. By doing so, such students might further reduce their engagement with short-term Internet activities and hence are less likely to suffer from Internet addiction.
By contrast, the positive link between an impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction did not differ across cultures. This result is consistent with the view that impulsive students are less concerned with long-term goals (Metcalfe and Mischel, 1999). Hence, whether or not their country’s culture supports a long-term orientation does not affect their likelihood of Internet addiction.
These culture moderation results have theoretical and practical implications. First, any comprehensive theory of Internet addiction must include the moderation effect of culture. Also, if research shows that self-control interventions reduce Internet addictions, these culture moderation effects suggest that they might especially benefit not only impulsive students generally, but also self-disciplined students in short-term orientation cultures.
Age
Age moderated the link between impulsivity indicators and Internet addiction. Compared with adolescents, university students showed a stronger positive link between impulsivity indicators and Internet addiction, but no moderation effect for restraint indicators. These results are consistent with the view that among university students who have transited into a new environment without their family and friends around, those with less restraint often have feelings of homesickness, friendsickness, or loneliness, and use the Internet excessively without parent monitoring, thereby becoming addicted (Larose and Boivin, 1998; Paul and Brier, 2001; Henderson and Mapp, 2002; Mattanah et al., 2010). Conversely, these results reject the claim that among students with low self-control, adolescents are more susceptible than university students to Internet addiction. Also, these results support the view that age does not moderate the negative link between restraint and Internet addiction.
In addition to being a vital component in a theory of Internet addiction, age is an important consideration for those exploring self-control interventions for reducing Internet addiction. Specifically, future studies can test whether interventions to improve self-control are especially effective for reducing Internet addiction among university students with impulsivity.
Gender
Gender moderated the link between restraint indicator and Internet addiction. Specifically, the negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger among males than females. This result partially supports the view that males are more self-reliant than females (Charness and Rustichini, 2011), the latter being more influenced by others (Rudman and Goodwin, 2004; Benda, 2013; Koon–Magnin et al., 2016); hence, greater self-control is linked to much less Internet addiction among males than among females. However, the positive link between an impulsivity indicator and Internet addiction did not differ by gender. Future studies that are more fine-grained can determine whether other mechanisms account for these different gender moderation results by restraint versus impulsivity indicators.
These significant gender results suggest that a comprehensive theory of Internet addiction must include gender differences. Furthermore, future studies can test whether interventions to improve self-control are especially effective for reducing Internet addiction not only among students with impulsivity but also among girls with restraint.
Internet Addiction Measures
Internet addiction measures moderated the link between impulsivity and Internet addiction. The relationship was weaker among CIAS than other internet addiction measures. As China’s testing of Internet addiction was mostly restricted to CIAS, increasing the comparability of China’s results with other results worldwide entails more studies in China using GPIUS, IAT or other internet addiction measures rather than only CIAS.
Limitations and Future Studies
The current study had several limitations, including a limited and biased sample, cross-sectional data, few moderating factors, and languages of studies. As the participants in this study were adolescents or university studies, future studies can include younger students and older adult students. Also, the number of studies in some subgroups show obvious difference, which may affect the robustness of subgroup analysis, so those results need to be interpreted cautiously and future research can include more relevant studies. Furthermore, we used Eastern versus Western countries as a rough proxy for culture. However, culture differs both across countries within a region (Germany vs. France; China vs. Japan) and within countries (Korean Americans vs. Irish Americans). Hence, future studies can test for culture effects more rigorously by asking participants to respond to culture questions. As the cross-sectional data in these studies cannot determine causation, future studies can collect more longitudinal data, including intervention studies that test possible solutions. As the current study only tested three types of moderating factors (internet addiction measures, culture, age, and gender), future studies can test other moderating factors, such as family attributes and peer relations. Moreover, only studies published in English and Chinese were used. As artificial intelligence improves translation software (e.g., Google translate), future meta-analyses can include published studies in more languages. Finally, this study that covers many factors, so it is not possible to focus in depth on any one of the aspects studied, we will depth explore the relationship between specific people’s self-control and Internet addiction in the future and focusing on one of the variables in order to analyze it in greater depth.
Conclusion
Past studies of self-control and Internet addiction showed mixed results, so this meta-analysis synthesized 83 studies with 80,681 students to show that self-control was negatively linked to Internet addiction. Specifically, there was a positive link between impulsivity and Internet addiction, while there was a negative link between restraint and Internet addiction.
Furthermore, culture, age, gender, and Internet addiction measures moderated these links between self-control and Internet addiction. The negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger in East Asia than in Western Europe/North America. Compared with adolescents, university students showed a greater positive link between impulsivity indicators and Internet addiction. Also, the negative link between a restraint indicator and Internet addiction was stronger among males than among females. Lastly, the relationship was weaker among CIAS than other Internet addiction measures.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/supplementary material.
Author Contributions
SL and HL provided the idea, designed the study, wrote the manuscript, and contributed to data collection. PR and MC provided the idea, designed the study, wrote the manuscript, and contributed to data analysis. CW contributed to design the study, analysed the data, and revised the manuscript. All authors approval of the version to be published and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
References
Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol. Bull. 82, 463–496. doi: 10.1037/h0076860
Akın, A., Arslan, S., Arslan, N., Uysal, R., and Sahranç, Ü (2015). Self-control management and internet addiction. Int. Online J. Educ. Sci. 7, 95–100. doi: 10.15345/iojes.2015.03.016
Asendorpf, J. B. (2002). “Shyness and adaptation to the social world of university,” in Shyness, ed. W. R. Crozier (Routledge), 119–136. doi: 10.4324/9780203183427-13
Azer, S. A., and Azer, D. (2015). Group interaction in problem-based learning tutorials: a systematic review. Eur. J. Dental Educ. 19, 194–208. doi: 10.1111/eje.12121
Bai, Y., and Xu, Q. (2009). Research on the relationship between self-control ability and Internet overuse tendency of medical college students. J. Qiqihar Med. College 30, 192–194.
Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M., Dewall, C. N., and Oaten, M. (2010). Self-regulation and personality: how interventions increase regulatory success, and how depletion moderates the effects of traits on behavior. J. Personality 74, 1773–1802. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00428.x
Benda, B. (2013). Self-control, gender, and age: a survival analysis of recidivism among boot camp graduates in a 5-year follow-up. J. Off. Rehabil. 40, 115–132. doi: 10.1300/J076v40n03_06
Błachnio, A., and Przepiorka, A. (2016). Dysfunction of self-regulation and self-control in facebook addiction. Psychiatr. Quart. 87, 493–500. doi: 10.1007/s11126-015-9403-1
Blinka, L., Skarupová, K., Sevcíková, A., Wolfling, K., Müller, K. W., and Dreier, M. (2015). Excessive internet use in European adolescents: what determines differences in severity? Int. J. Public Health 60, 249–256. doi: 10.1007/s00038-014-0635-x
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synthesis Methods 1, 97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12
Bozoglan, B., Demirer, V., and Sahin, I. (2013). Loneliness, self−esteem, and life satisfaction as predictors of internet addiction: a cross−sectional study among Turkish university students. Scand. J. Psychol. 54, 313–319. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12049
Brenner, V. (1997). Psychology of computer use: XLVII. parameters of internet use, abuse and addiction: the first 90 days of the internet usage survey. Psychol. Rep. 80, 879–882. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.80.3.879
Cai, C., and Li, J. (2017). The relationship between outcome expectation, network control self-efficacy and internet addiction in college students. Technol. Wind 3, 168–169.
Cao, Y., Qi, J., Niu, Z., Li, C., Zhao, A., Yi, Y., et al. (2018). Study on the effect of self-control on shopping addiction in college students. Modern Med. Health Res. 19:160.
Caplan, S. E. (2002). Problematic internet use and psychosocial well-being: development of a theory-based cognitive–behavioral measurement instrument. Comput. Hum. Behav. 18, 553–575. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00004-3
Caplan, S. E. (2005). A social skill account of problematic internet use. J. Commun. 55, 721–736. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03019.x
Carver, C. S. (2005). Impulse and constraint: perspectives from personality psychology, convergence with theory in other areas, and potential for integration. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 312–333. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0904_2
Chak, K., and Leung, L. (2004). Shyness and locus of control as predictors of internet addiction and internet use. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7, 559–570. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.559
Charness, G., and Rustichini, A. (2011). Gender differences in cooperation with group membership. Games Econ. Behav. 72, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2010.07.006
Chen, S. H., Weng, L. J., Su, Y. J., Wu, H. M., and Yang, P. F. (2003). Development of a Chinese internet addiction scale and its psychometric study. Chin. J. Psychol. 45, 279–294.
Cheng, C., and Li, A. Y. L. (2014). Internet addiction prevalence and quality of (real) life: a meta-analysis of 31 nations across seven world regions. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 17, 755–760. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0317
Ching, H. K., and Tak, L. M. (2016). The structural model in parenting style, attachment style, self-regulation and self-esteem with smartphone addiction. IAFOR J. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 3, 85–103.
Choi, J. S., Park, S. M., Lee, J., Hwang, J. Y., Jung, H. Y., Choi, S. W., et al. (2013). Resting-state beta and gamma activity in internet addiction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 328–333. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.007
Choi, J. S., Park, S. M., Roh, M. S., Lee, J. Y., Park, C. B., Hwang, J. Y., et al. (2014). Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in internet addiction. Psychiatry Res. 215, 424–428. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.001
Chou, C., Condron, L., and Belland, J. C. (2005). A review of the research on internet addiction. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 17, 363–388. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-8138-1
Chou, T. J., and Ting, C. C. (2003). The role of flow experience in cyber-game addiction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 6, 663–675. doi: 10.1089/109493103322725469
Churchwell, J. C., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2013). Age-related changes in insula cortical thickness and impulsivity: significance for emotional development and decision-making. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.001
Cook, D. A., and Reed, D. A. (2015). Appraising the quality of medical education research methods: the medical education research study quality instrument and the Newcastle–Ottawa scale-education. Acad. Med. 90, 1067–1076. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000786
Davis, R. A., Flett, G. L., and Besser, A. (2002). Validation of a new scale for measuring problematic internet use: implications for pre-employment screening. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 5, 331–345. doi: 10.1089/109493102760275581
DeLisi, M. (2014). “Low self-control is a brain-based disorder,” in The Nurture Versus Biosocial Debate in Criminology: On the Origins of Criminal Behavior and Criminality, eds K. M. Beaver, J. C. Barnes, and B. B. Boutwell (SAGE Publications), 172–184. doi: 10.4135/9781483349114.n11
Demetrovics, Z., Király, O., Koronczai, B., Griffiths, M. D., Nagygyörgy, K., Elekes, Z., et al. (2016). Psychometric properties of the problematic internet use questionnaire short-form (PIUQ-SF-6) in a nationally representative sample of adolescents. PLoS One 11:1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159409
Dewitte, S., and Cremer, D. D. (2001). Self-control and cooperation: different concepts, similar decisions? A question of the right perspective. J. Psychol. 135, 133–153. doi: 10.1080/00223980109603686
Du, L. (2013). The relationship between network self-control ability and pathological internet use in college students. Chin. J. School Health 8, 990–991.
Duckworth, A. L., and Steinberg, L. (2015). Unpacking self−control. Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 32–37. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12107
Dvorak, R. D., and Simons, J. S. (2009). Moderation of resource depletion in the self-control strength model: differing effects of two modes of self-control. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 572–583. doi: 10.1177/0146167208330855
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J. 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Feng, J., Zeng, S., Sun, Y., and Kenichi, K. (2014). A study on the relationship between undergraduates’ internet addiction disorder, online game experience, online time and sleep quality. World J. Sleep Med. 4, 193–197.
Fırat, M. (2017). Relationship between self-control and facebook use: case of CEIT students. Educ. Sci. Theory Practice 17, 1179–1201.
Gámez, M., Calvete, E., Orue, I., and Las Hayas, C. (2015). Problematic internet use and problematic alcohol use from the cognitive–behavioral model: a longitudinal study among adolescents. Addictive Behav. 40, 109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.09.009
Gao, Y., and Zhao, L. (2009). On upgrading college students ‘mental resilience and measures of handling network addiction. J. Jilin Province Econ. Manage. Cadre College 23, 115–117.
Ge, L., Ge, X., and Zhao, J. (2010). Correlation between locus of control and internet addiction in college students. China J. Health Psychol. 11, 1386–1387.
Geng, J., Han, L., Gao, F., Zou, M., and Huang, C. (2018). Internet addiction and procrastination among Chinese young adults: a moderated mediation model. Computers in Human Behavior 84, 320–333. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.013
Gignac, G. E., and Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 102, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069
Gokçearslan, S., Mumcu, F., Haçlaman, T., and Çevik, Y. D. (2016). Modelling smartphone addiction: the role of smartphone usage, self-regulation, general self-efficacy and cyberloafing in university students. Comput. Hum. Behav. 63, 639–649. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.091
He, C., Xia, M., Jiang, G., and Wei, H. (2012). Mediation role of self-control between internet game addiction and self-esteem. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 20, 58–60.
Henderson, A. T., and Mapp, K. L. (2002). A new wave of evidence: the impact of school, family and community connections on student achievement. Natl. Center Family Commun. Connect. Schools 97, 13–14.
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., and Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 162–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.x
Hofstede, G. J., Jonker, C. M., and Verwaart, T. (2008). “Long-term orientation in trade,” in Complexity and Artificial Markets, eds F. Hauser and K. Schredelseker (Springer), 107–119. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-70556-7_9
Hou, Z., Zhang, Q., and Yang, G. (2013). Effects of different addiction states on activities, personalities and self-control ability among undergraduate internet user. J. Zhejiang Univ. 40, 106–111.
Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Marin-Martinez, F., and Botella, J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol. Methods 11, 193–206.
Hur, M. H. (2006). Demographic, habitual, and socioeconomic determinants of Internet addiction disorder: an empirical study of Korean teenagers. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 514–525. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.514
Iftikhar, M., and Tariq, S. (2014). Self-control, narcissistic tendencies and internet addiction among adolescents. J. Arts Soc. Sci. 4, 1–17.
Ismail, A. B., and Zawahreh, N. (2017). Self-control and its relationship with the internet addiction among a sample of najran university students. J. Educ. Hum. Dev. 6, 168–174.
Jabutay, F., and Kanthawongs, P. (2016). “An empirical study on the impact of self–regulation and compulsivity towards smartphone addiction of university students,” in Proceedings of 2016 International Conference on Cognition and Exploratory Learning in the Digital Age, (International Association for Development of the Information Society), 28–30.
Jensen, L., and Konradsen, F. (2018). A review of the use of virtual reality head-mounted displays in education and training. Educ. Inf. Technol. 23, 1515–1529. doi: 10.1007/s10639-017-9676-0
Jeong, S. H., Kim, H. J., Yum, J. Y., and Hwang, Y. (2016). What type of content are smartphone users addicted to? SNS vs. games. Comput. Hum. Behav. 54, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.035
Jiang, Y., Wang, H., Jiang, H., and Liu, Y. (2018). The influence of neuroticism on the excessive use of mobile social networks in adolescents: the dual mediating effects of impulsive and interpersonal disturbance. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 16, 130–140.
Jiang, Z., Zhao, X., and Li, C. (2017). Self–control predicts attentional bias assessed by online shopping–related stroop in high online shopping addiction tendency college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry 75, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.02.007
Kasen, S., Cohen, P., and Chen, H. (2011). Developmental course of impulsivity and capability from age 10 to age 25 as related to trajectory of suicide attempt in a community cohort. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 41, 180–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00017.x
Khang, H., Kim, J. K., and Kim, Y. (2013). Self–traits and motivations as antecedents of digital media flow and addiction: the internet, mobile phones, and video games. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2416–2424. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.027
Kim, E. J., Namkoong, K., Ku, T., and Kim, S. J. (2008). The relationship between online game addiction and aggression, self–control and narcissistic personality traits. European Psychiatry 23, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.10.010
Kim, J., Hong, H., Lee, J., and Hyun, M. H. (2017). Effects of time perspective and self–control on procrastination and internet addiction. J. Behav. Addict. 6, 229–236. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.017
Kim, Y. J., Roh, D., Lee, S. K., Canan, F., and Potenza, M. N. (2018). Factors statistically predicting at–risk/problematic Internet use in a sample of young adolescent boys and girls in South Korea. Front. Psychiatry 9:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00351
Koon–Magnin, S., Bowers, D., Langhinrichsen–Rohling, J., and Arata, C. (2016). Social learning, self–control, gender, and variety of violent delinquency. Deviant Behavior 37, 824–836. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2016.1147798
Lam, L. T., and Peng, Z. W. (2010). Effect of pathological use of the internet on adolescent mental health: a prospective study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolescent Med. 164, 901–906. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.159
Larose, R., and Eastin, M. S. (2010). Is online buying out of control? Electronic commerce and consumer self–regulation. J. Broad. Electr. Media 46, 549–564. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4604_4
Larose, R., Mastro, D., and Eastin, M. S. (2001). Understanding internet usage, a social-cognitive approach to uses and gratifications. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 19, 395–413. doi: 10.1177/089443930101900401
Larose, R., Lin, C. A., and Eastin, M. S. (2003). Unregulated internet usage: addiction, habit, or deficient self–regulation? Media Psychol. 5, 225–253.
Larose, S., and Boivin, M. (1998). Attachment to parents, social support expectations, and socioemotional adjustment during the high school–college transition. J. Res. Adolescence 8, 1–27. doi: 10.1207/s15327795jra0801_1
Lee, J., and Cho, B. (2015). Effects of self–control and school adjustment on smartphone addiction among elementary school students. Int. J. Contents 11, 1–6. doi: 10.5392/IJoC.2015.11.3.001
Lei, H., Jing, G., Min, J., Feng, G., and Hua, Y. (2017). Relationship between shyness and mobile phone addiction in Chinese young adults: mediating roles of self-control and attachment anxiety. Comput. Hum. Behav. 76, 363–371. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.036
Lei, H., Chiu, M. M., Cui, Y., Zhou, W., and Li, S. (2018). Parenting style and aggression: a meta–analysis of mainland Chinese children and youth. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 94, 446–455. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.07.033
Lei, H., Mao, W., Cheong, C. M., Wen, Y., Cui, Y., and Cai, Z. (2020). The relationship between self-esteem and cyberbullying: a meta-analysis of children and youth students. Curr. Psychol. 3, 830–842. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00407-6
Leung, L. (2014). Predicting internet risks: a longitudinal panel study of gratifications–sought, internet addiction symptoms, and social media use among children and adolescents. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. Open Access J. 2, 424–439. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2014.902316
Li, B., Shi, Z., Wang, S., Zheng, K., and Zhao, X. (2018). The correlation between college students’ degree of wechat use and their loneliness: the mediating role of self–control. Psychol. Techn. Applic. 6, 51–57.
Li, C., Dang, J., Zhang, X., Zhang, Q., and Guo, J. (2014). Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: the effect of parental behavior and self–control. Comput. Hum. Behav. 41, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.001
Li, D., Li, X., Wang, Y., Zhao, L., Bao, Z., and Wen, F. (2013). School connectedness and problematic internet use in adolescents: a moderated mediation model of deviant peer affiliation and self–control. J. Abnormal Child Psychol. 41, 1231–1242. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9761-9
Li, J., and Wang, W. (2013). The characteristics of internet addiction among high school freshmen in Chongqing and its relationship with impulsiveness. China J. Health Psychol. 21, 420–422.
Li, X., Li, D., and Newman, J. (2013). Parental behavioral and psychological control and problematic internet use among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of self–control. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 16, 442–447. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0293
Li, X., Xin, T., Zhang, Y., Du, Y., Liu, Y., and Jiang, Y. (2016). Boredom proneness and mobile phone addiction: mediating of self–control. Chin. J. School Health 37, 1487–1490.
Li, Y., Jin, L., and Zhang, S. (2016). Research on the relationship between self–control ability and mobile phone dependence among university students. Chin. J. Health Educ. 32, 775–778+802.
Liang, H., Wang, L., and Fan, C. (2016). Relationship between the due self–control system and internet addiction among adolescents. Chin. General Practice 19, 1076–1080.
Lin, S., and Tsai, C. C. (2002). Sensation seeking and internet dependence of Taiwan high school adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 18, 411–426. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(01)00056-5
Lipsey, M. W., and Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Liu, H., and Yang, N. (2012). Correlation study on internet addiction, time management disposition and anxiety form among undergraduates. Soc. Psychol. Sci. 141, 92–95.
Lu, X. (2016). The current situation of network dependence and the influence of self–control on students—based on the empirical study of Sino–foreign cooperative education in Jiangsu province. J. Jiangsu Second Normal Univ. 9, 30–36.
Maloney, P. W., Grawitch, M. J., and Barber, L. K. (2012). The multi-factor structure of the brief self-control scale: discriminant validity of restraint and impulsivity. J. Res. Personal. 46, 111–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.10.001
Mattanah, J. F., Ayers, J. F., Brand, B. L., Brooks, L. J., Quimby, J. L., and McNary, S. W. (2010). A social support intervention to ease the college transition: exploring main effects and moderators. J. College Stud. Dev. 51, 93–108. doi: 10.1353/csd.0.0116
Mehroof, M., and Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: the role of sensation seeking, self–control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 13, 313–316. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0229
Mei, S., and Chai, J. (2013). Teenagers’ access to the Internet with cell phones and its relationship with their subjective well–being and self–control. Chin. J. Special Educ. 159, 80–85.
Mei, S., Chai, J., and Guo, J. (2015). Subjective well–being and internet addiction of adolescents: mediating roles of self–esteem and self–control. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 31, 603–609.
Mesgarani, M., Shafiee, S., Ahmadi, E., and Zare, F. (2013). Studying the relationship between internet addiction and emotional intelligence, sensation seeking and metacognition among those who referred to cafes. Int. Res. J. Appl. Basic Sci. 4, 889–893.
Metcalfe, J., and Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev. 106, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3
Mottram, A. J., and Fleming, M. J. (2009). Extraversion, impulsivity, and online group membership as predictors of problematic internet use. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 319–321. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0170
Nie, Y., Dou, K., and Wang, Y. (2013). Impulsive and internet addition disorder: a mediator role of self–control. J. Ningbo Univ. 3, 7–12.
Ningtyas, D. (2012). Hubungan antara self control dengan internet addiction pada mahasiswa program studi piskologi universitas sebelas maret surakarta. J. Ilmiah Psikologi Candrajiwa 1, 25–30.
Öğütçü, G., Çırakoğlu, O. C., and Cula, S. (2016). Information security in the world of digital natives: how internet addiction, sensation seeking and information security behaviors are related. Int. J. Manage. Appl. Sci. 83, 79–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cose.2015.10.002
Olds, T., Wake, M., Patton, G., Ridley, K., Waters, E., Williams, J., et al. (2009). How do school–day activity patterns differ with age and gender across adolescence? J. Adolescent Health 44, 64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.05.003
Özdemir, Y., Kuzucu, Y., and Ak, S. (2014). Depression, loneliness and internet addiction: how important is low self–control? Comput. Hum. Behav. 34, 284–290. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.009
Park, J. A., Park, M. H., Shin, J. H., Li, B., Rolfe, D. T., Yoo, J. Y., et al. (2016). Effect of sports participation on Internet addiction mediated by self–control: a case of korean adolescents. Kasetsart J. Soc. Sci. 37, 164–169. doi: 10.1016/j.kjss.2016.08.003
Park, S., Kang, M., and Kim, E. (2014). Social relationship on problematic internet use (PIU) among adolescents in South Korea: a moderated mediation model of self–esteem and self–control. Comput. Hum. Behav. 38, 349–357. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.005
Paul, E. L., and Brier, S. (2001). Friendsickness in the transition to college: precollege predictors and college adjustment correlates. J. Counseling Dev. 79, 77–89. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2001.tb01946.x
Peluso, T., Ricciardelli, L. A., and Williams, R. J. (1999). Self–control in relation to problem drinking and symptoms of disordered eating. Addict. Behav. 24, 439–442. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(98)00056-2
Ponton, M. K., and Rhea, N. E. (2006). Autonomous learning from a social cognitive perspective. New Horizons Adult Educ. Hum. Resour. Dev. 20, 38–49.
Rahmani, S., and Lavasani, M. G. (2011). The relationship between internet dependency with sensation seeking and personality. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 30, 272–277. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.054
Romano, M., Osborne, L. A., Truzoli, R., and Reed, P. (2013). Differential psychological impact of internet exposure on internet addicts. PLos One 8:1–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055162
Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., and Carrier, L. M. (2014). “Social networking is addictive and can lead to psychological disorders,” in Are Social Networking Sites Harmful?, ed. N. Berlatsky (Greenhaven Press), 51–58.
Rudman, L. A., and Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic in–group bias: why do women like women more than men like men? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 87:494. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494
Shaw, M., and Black, D. W. (2008). Internet addiction. CNS Drugs 22, 353–365. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200822050-00001
Shek, D. T. L., and Lu, Y. (2012). Internet addiction phenomenon in early adolescents in Hong Kong. Sci. World J. 2012:104304. doi: 10.1100/2012/104304
Son, D. T., Yasuoka, J., Poudel, K. C., Otsuka, K., and Jimba, M. (2013). Massively multiplayer online role–playing games (MMORPG): association between its addiction, self–control and mental disorders among young people in Vietnam. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 59, 570–577. doi: 10.1177/0020764012445861
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., and Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self–report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 44:1764. doi: 10.1037/a0012955
Strack, F., and Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
Sun, G., Yu, Y., Luo, Z., Li, Y., and Zhao, X. (2011). Research on middle school students’ mobile phone internet addition and self–control over internet use. China J. Health Psychol. 9, 1078–1080.
Sun, X., Zhao, X., Zhou, Z., Chen, W., and Bao, N. (2015). Mediation role of self–control in internet use between time management disposition and pathological internet use. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 13, 410–413.
Takao, M., Takahashi, S., and Kitamura, M. (2009). Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 501–507. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0022
Tang, Y., Zou, J., Li, M., Liang, J., and Liu, W. (2015). Subjective well–being and mobile phone dependence among vocational college students: the mediating role of self–esteem and self–control. Chin. J. School Doctor 721–725.
Tao, Y. (2016). Analysis of the influence of self–control on adolescent internet addiction. New West 125–126.
Tao, Y., and Li, C. (2009). Research on the mediating effect of self–control on internet addiction disorder and parental rearing style. China J. Health Psychol. 17, 1444–1447.
Teng, Z., Griffiths, M. D., Nie, Q., Xiang, G., and Guo, C. (2020). Parent–adolescent attachment and peer attachment associated with internet gaming disorder: a longitudinal study of first-year undergraduate students. J. Behav. Addic. 9, 116–128. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00011
Teng, Z., Li, Y., and Liu, Y. (2014). Online gaming, internet addiction, and aggression in chinese male students: the mediating role of low self–control. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 6, 88–97. doi: 10.5539/ijps.v6n2p89
Throuvala, M. A., Janikian, M., Griffiths, M. D., Rennoldson, M., and Kuss, D. J. (2019). The role of family and personality traits in internet gaming disorder: a mediation model combining cognitive and attachment perspectives. J. Behav. Addic. 8, 48–62. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.05
Tsitsika, A., Janikian, M., Schoenmakers, T. M., Tzavela, E. C., Olafsson, K., Wójcik, S., et al. (2014). Internet addictive behavior in adolescence: a cross–sectional study in seven European countries. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 17, 528–535. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0382
Van Deursen, A. J. A. M., Bolle, C. L., Hegner, S. M., and Kommers, P. A. M. (2015). Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior. Comput. Hum. Behav. 45, 411–420. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.039
Wan, J., Liu, L., and Fang, X. (2015). On the relationship between college students’ psychological needs or self–efficacy and internet addiction. Chin. J. Special Educ. 3, 86–91.
Wang, J., You, Y., Huang, J., Chen, D., and Zhuang, H. (2012). The relationship between internet addiction and ego–identity, self–control ability of college students. J. Psychiatry 25, 350–352.
Wang, L., Tao, T., Fan, C., Gao, W., and Wei, C. (2017). The association between internet addiction and both impulsivity and effortful control and its variation with age. Addic. Res. Theory 25, 83–90. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2016.1206082
Wang, Y. (2009). Relationship between college students’ self –regulation and the tendency of internet addiction. Educ. Teach. Res. 23, 37–39.
Wills, T. A., Resko, J. A., Ainette, M. G., and Mendoza, D. (2004). Role of parent support and peer support in adolescent substance use: a test of mediated effects. Psychol. Addic. Behav. 18:122. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.2.122
Wu, Y., An, Y., and Hou, S. (2009). A study on the relationship between IAD and locus of control among university students. J. Shanxi Agric. Univ. 8, 329–332.
Yao, M. Z., He, J., Ko, D. M., and Pang, K. (2014). The influence of personality, parental behaviors, and self-esteem on Internet addiction: a study of Chinese college students. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 17, 104–110. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0710
Young, K. S. (1998a). Caught in the net: how to recognize the signs of internet addictionand a winning strategy for recovery. Assessment 21, 713–722.
Young, K. S. (1998b). Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychol. Behav. 1, 237–244.
Young, K. S. (2004). Internet addiction: a new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. Am. Behav. Sci. 48, 402–415. doi: 10.1177/0002764204270278
Yun, I., Kim, S. G., and Kwon, S. (2016). Low self–control among south Korean adolescents: a test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s generality hypothesis. Int. J. Off. Ther. Comparat. Criminol. 60, 1185–1208. doi: 10.1177/0306624X15574683
Zhang, X., Zhang, Y., and Wu, M. (2017). Research on the relationship between self–control and cell phone dependence in college students. J. Lanzhou Instit. Educ. 9, 158–160.
Zhang, Y., Zhang, W., and Wu, L. (2013). Relationship between mobile phone internet addiction and impulsivity among medical college students. China J. Health Psychol. 21, 284–287.
Zhou, E., and Zhou, H. (2017). An empirical study of college student’s subjective well–being, self–control and internet addiction. J. Gradual School Chin. Acad. Soc. Sci. 19–26.
Zhou, H. (2017). Study on vocational college students cell phone addiction and self–control. J. Lanzhou Petrochem. Polytechnic 3, 52–56.
Zhou, X., and Wang, X. (2017). Relationship between we chat addiction and self–control in college students. China J. Health Psychol. 8, 1254–1257.
Keywords: self-control, meta-analysis, internet addiction, moderator analysis, students
Citation: Li S, Ren P, Chiu MM, Wang C and Lei H (2021) The Relationship Between Self-Control and Internet Addiction Among Students: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:735755. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.735755
Received: 12 July 2021; Accepted: 07 October 2021;
Published: 24 November 2021.
Edited by:
Bojana M. Dinic, University of Novi Sad, SerbiaReviewed by:
Darko Hinic, University of Kragujevac, SerbiaMagdalena Ramos Navas-Parejo, University of Granada, Spain
Daniela Šincek, Josip Juraj Strossmayer University of Osijek, Croatia
Copyright © 2021 Li, Ren, Chiu, Wang and Lei. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hao Lei, bGVpaGFvODk5OEAxNjMuY29t