Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 08 December 2021
Sec. Psychopathology

Cross-Lagged Analysis of COVID-19-Related Worry and Media Consumption in a Socioeconomically Disadvantaged Sample of Community Adults

  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States

The COVID-19 pandemic has led to an increase in an array of mental health problems. Previous research has shown that media exposure to stressful situations is often related to anxiety and stress. However, given that most existing work has used cross-sectional designs, less is known about the interplay of media exposure and worry as they unfold during sustained exposure to a collective stressor. The current study examined bidirectional associations between COVID-related worry and media consumption over a three-month period. Participants were 87 community adults, the majority of whom were recruited from communities heavily impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic. For three consecutive months, participants asked to indicate how much time they spent worrying and consuming news about the COVID-19 pandemic on a scale from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“most of the day”). Cross-lagged analyses revealed that Pandemic Worry at Month 1 predicted increases in Pandemic Media Consumption at Month 2, which in turn predicted increases in Pandemic Worry at Month 3. Findings suggest that media consumption may be a maladaptive coping strategy that has the iatrogenic effect of increasing worry. Clarifying the causal associations between anxiety-perpetuating processes and media consumption may have important clinical implications for understanding and treating mental health during health pandemics.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has led to an increase in an array of mental health problems (Liu and Liu, 2020). These rates may be even higher within low socioeconomic status populations who are already at an increased risk for mental health problems and less access to resources (Rozenfeld et al., 2020; Rudenstine et al., 2021). Anxiety is a particularly concerning potential outcome during a prolonged health crisis, given that it is associated with sustained, heightened hormonal stress responses that put individuals at risk for poor health outcomes (Nusslock and Miller, 2016). In efforts to better understand behaviors that influence mental health during large scale stressors, previous research has identified that increased media exposure may be a risk factor for developing crisis-related anxiety (McNaughton-Cassill, 2001; Busso et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2017). This is not surprising, given that media consumption, which broadly refers to the intake of information of news and/or information through various media outlets (e.g., television, cell phones, social media), has been associated with a host of poor outcomes, including persistent negative affect and sleep disruptions (Szabo and Hopkinson, 2007; LeBourgeois et al., 2017). Further, previous research shows that media consumption shapes individuals’ views about current events (Elsass et al., 2014), highlighting the importance of considering media consumption in relation to psychological functioning.

Existing data suggest that media consumption may be a relevant predictor of mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Media consumption has increased during the COVID-19 pandemic, with the big four news broadcasters in the United States witnessing a 19% spike in viewing during the week of March 16–22, 2020 (Liu and Liu, 2020). Consistent with past work, research is also showing that COVID-related media exposure is associated with increased mental distress (Riehm et al., 2020; Shabahang et al., 2020). These findings, combined with those from previous public health crises, suggest that exposure to stressor-related media may increase feelings of distress, anxiety, and hopelessness. Unsurprisingly, in one of their earliest reports, the World Health Organization recommended “minimizing watching, reading or listening to news about COVID-19 that causes you to feel anxious…” (Buheji et al., 2020; World Health Organization, 2020).

The tendency to consume media during times of crises may be explained by negative reinforcement models of anxiety. Specifically, an ongoing health crisis, such as the COVID-19 pandemic, introduces a great deal of uncertainty, such as the possibility of illness, loss of employment, and death (Wang et al., 2020). For example, Chen and Hong (2010) found that individuals who experience anxious emotions during a collective crisis might cope by seeking crisis-related information. As they attempt to ease negative emotions that arise in uncertain situations, information-seeking may, in part, provide individuals with a sense of control over the crisis or its impact. Indeed, studies have shown that being informed about the COVID-19 pandemic was associated with virus-related anxiety (Jungmann and Witthöft, 2020).

Staying informed about the COVID-19 pandemic may be one way to cope with pandemic-related worry. Worry, which can be broadly defined as intrusive and excessive concern about future events, is considered a related yet distinct construct from anxiety (e.g., Zebb and Beck, 1998; Segerstrom et al., 2000). Specifically, previous work has distinguished worry, a primarily cognitive manifestation, from anxiety which is characterized by physiological symptoms (Andrews et al., 2010). Thus, although a core feature of anxiety disorders, worry refers to a unique cognitive process in which individual engages in worry as an avoidance-based coping strategy to handle distress (e.g., Borkovec et al., 2004; Andrews et al., 2010; Newman et al., 2013). Persistent engagement in worry-based cognitions has been identified as a predictor of future anxiety disorders across various samples (e.g., Mathews, 1990; Esbjørn et al., 2015; Ryum et al., 2017).

Increased media exposure during the pandemic may represent a potential coping strategy to handle pandemic-related distress. However, this strategy may “back-fire” and actually induce more worries (Witthöft et al., 2018). Indeed, some research has shown that media-related rumination is linked to negative outcomes such as psychological distress and onset of symptoms of trauma disorders (Jenness et al., 2016; Parris et al., 2020). In one three-year longitudinal study, Thompson et al. (2019) found that media exposure to the 2013 Boston marathon bombings positively predicted distress and worry about future negative events. Further, conceptual models of anxiety indicate that although worry may represent a coping strategy in the short-term, worry is associated with increases in negative affect and may sustain distress in the long term (Newman et al., 2013). Taken together, these patterns of findings suggest that while a degree of media consumption during a health pandemic may initially alleviate some pandemic-related distress, an overconsumption of media may inadvertently increase worry through mechanisms of overexposure and rumination.

One limitation of the previous work is the use of cross-sectional designs, which limits the ability to examine these associations over time, rendering impossible the potential to make causal interpretations about the relationship between media exposure and anxiety. One exception is a study by Schmidt et al. (2021) who examined COVID-19 media exposure and COVID-related worry across 21days and found that media exposure predicted next day worry which in turn predicted next day media exposure. These findings provide preliminary evidence that a reciprocal relationship between worry and news consumption exists. However, the extent to which news consumptions and anxiety transact across larger periods of time remains relatively unknown, which is problematic given the sustained nature of the COVID-19 pandemic. To address this gap in the literature, the current study examined bidirectional associations between the degree of COVID-related worry and media consumption over a three-month period in a sample of socio-economically diverse community adults living in the United States. Based on previous work (e.g., Schmidt et al., 2021), we hypothesize that higher levels of COVID-related worry will predict increases in COVID-related media exposure, and that increases in media exposure will be associated with increases in COVID-related worry over time.

Materials and Methods

Participants

All participants had previously completed a research study in our laboratory and were invited to complete a virtual follow-up study seeking to examine impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health. For the initial study, participants were recruited from the community using flyers and online advertisements (e.g., Facebook, Craigslist) and were eligible to participate if they were between the ages of 18–55years old and were able to communicate fluently in English. The first 90 people who responded to this request were enrolled in the study.

Participants for the present study were 87 socioeconomically diverse community adults (M/SDage=33.8/9.38years old; 51.2% female), with 55.3% of the sample identifying as Non-Caucasian/White, 23.0% as Black/African-American, 12.6% Asian, and 9.2% reporting some other race. Approximately 17.2% of the sample identified as Hispanic. About half of the participants (53.5%) were working full-time at the time of the study, followed by 16.3% were working part-time, 15.1% were unemployed, 10.5% were students, with the remainder indicating some other employment status (e.g., retired, receiving disability). The median household income fell between the range of $25,000–$49,999. The majority of participants came from communities with COVID-19 positivity and unemployment rates higher than the national average at the time of assessment (Cerron, 2020).

Measures

All procedures were approved by the University Institutional Review Board (Protocol #‘s: 1073423–17, 1,361,164–1, 1,590,453–6) and followed the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Interested participants provided written consent prior to data collection. For three consecutive months (9/20–11/20), participants completed an online assessment, in which they were asked to indicate: (1) “how much time they spent reading or watching news related to the coronavirus” and (2) “how much time they spent worrying about the COVID-19 pandemic” on a scale from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“most of the day”). The data that support the findings of this study can be made available from the corresponding author upon request.

Data Analysis

Repeated measures GLM were conducted to examine temporal trends in Pandemic Worry and News Consumption across the three time points. These analyses were conducted using SPSS (v.26). To test the central hypothesis, cross-lagged analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.0 (Muthén and Muthén, 2017). Specifically, autoregressive and cross-lagged pathways among constructs over time were specified. Missingness at each timepoint was as follows: Month 1 (.=3; 3.4%), Month 2 (.=7; 8.0%), and Month 3 (.=6; 6.9%). In order to include all participants with at least partial data, missing data were estimated using maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors (MLR). Standardized results are reported below.

Results

Preliminary Results

Average trends in these variables over time are depicted in Figure 1. Preliminary analyses indicated that, on average, participants reported less Pandemic Media Consumption (F=18.89, p<0.001) and Pandemic Worry (F=8.05, p=0.006) over the three month period. No significant associations between income or pandemic-related Media Consumption and Worry were observed at any time point (.’s>0.05). These results indicate that the extent to which respondents worried and consumed news related to the COVID-19 pandemic varied across the three timepoints, allowing for the analysis on potential causal effect of these variables on each other over time.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trends in pandemic-related media consumption and worry across three-month period. Graphs depict means and standard deviations at each time point.

Longitudinal Associations

Results of the cross-lagged analyses are depicted in Figure 2. Analysis of the cross-lagged relationships revealed the data partially supported our hypothesis. Specifically, Pandemic Worry at Month 1 positively predicted Pandemic Media Consumption at Month 2, indicating higher levels of worry was associated with an increase in the amount of time individuals spent consuming news related to the pandemic over and above the influence of media consumption at Month 1 on Month 2. Further, the cross-lagged path from Pandemic Media Consumption at Month 2 to Pandemic Worry at Month 3 was significant, indicating greater amount of time spent watching or reading news related to the pandemic was associated with an increase in worry about COVID-19 over and above the influence of worry at Month 2 on Month 3. Unexpectedly, the cross-lagged paths from Pandemic Media Consumption at Month 1 to Pandemic Worry at Month 2 and from Pandemic Worry at Month 2 to Media Consumption at Month 3 were not significant. The overall model explained 29.7% variance in Pandemic Media Consumption and 54.1% of variance in Pandemic Worry at Month 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Lagged associations between pandemic-related media consumption and worry over three-month period. *.<0.05. Standardized coefficients are shown. Bolded pathways represent significant cross-lagged associations. Rmedia2=0.30. Rworry2=0.54.

Parsing Trait and State Pandemic Worry

We conducted secondary analyses to explore whether the findings for Pandemic Worry could be accounted for by individual differences in trait-worry as assessed with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire total score (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990). First, bivariate correlations showed that trait worry was not associated with Pandemic Worry at any timepoint (.’s=−0.14-0.12, p’s>0.05), suggesting that these variables are assessing two distinct constructs. Next, to test whether the cross-lagged effects of Pandemic Worry were driven by trait-levels of anxiety, we reconducted the cross-lagged analysis including trait worry as a covariate at each time point. Results revealed that all significant pathways remained significant, and no new findings emerged. Taken together, these results suggest that the observed findings cannot solely be accounted for by trait-level factors and instead reflect fluctuations in state COVID-related worry over time.

Discussion

The current study sought to examine how pandemic-related media exposure and worry mutually influence each other over a three-month period. Results revealed a bidirectional association between worry and media consumption related to COVID-19, such that the degree of pandemic-related worry at baseline was associated with increases in consumption of pandemic-related media a month later. Further, media consumption at month 2 was associated with more pandemic-related worry at month 3, over and above pandemic-related worry at the month 2 assessment.

Past research during other public health crises has shown that consuming stressor-related media leads to heightened stress response and increases in anxiety and other mental health symptoms (Busso et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2017). Studies during the COVID-19 pandemic thus far have reported similar findings across various samples (e.g., Garfin et al., 2020; Liu and Liu, 2020). While these findings are informative, the temporal associations between these processes as they unfold over time are still unknown. To our knowledge, only one other study has examined these links over time and found support of cyclical daily associations between pandemic-related news exposure and worry (Schmidt et al., 2021). Given the longevity and sustained nature of the current pandemic, the current study extends this research by showing that the bidirectional relationships between media consumption and worry persist across larger intervals of time.

Findings suggest that consuming pandemic-related news may represent a maladaptive strategy to cope with the ongoing pandemic stress, which has the iatrogenic effect of increasing worry. Previous research suggests that individuals are more likely to watch stressor-related media to cope with ongoing stress during times of crises (Liu and Liu, 2020). For example, some research has found that anxious individuals may be more likely to cope with widely broadcasted terrorist attacks by seeking out information on social media (Jin et al., 2016). The current findings are in line with these previous studies and suggest that worry may induce greater news consumption during the COVID-19 pandemic. Given that worry has been identified as a risk factor for the development of anxiety disorders (e.g., Mathews, 1990; Esbjørn et al., 2015; Ryum et al., 2017), these findings suggest that elevated engagement in worry during the COVID-19 pandemic may lead to increases in anxiety-related psychopathology.

One important consideration is that some degree of worry and media consumption may be adaptive in the context of global crises, like the COVID-19 pandemic. Indeed, one study found that flu-related fear mediated the relationship between media exposure and engagement in preventative measures during the 2009 H1N1 outbreak (Zhang et al., 2015). In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, some degree of exposure to pandemic-related media may be necessary to remain up to date on changes in health protocols and policies, which are critical to “stop the spread” of the virus. However, perseveration on the topic through media consumption is problematic for emotional well-being. This would be consistent with research characterizing persistent worry as a maladaptive, avoidance-based coping strategy to handle distress (e.g., Borkovec et al., 2004; Andrews et al., 2010; Newman et al., 2013). Thus, the public should be cautioned against over-exposure to media related to the pandemic, because it can actually lead to poorer emotional health, which may compromise immune system function (Nusslock and Miller, 2016), which is particularly detrimental during a pandemic.

The current study has several strengths, including a three-time point longitudinal design which allowed for temporal causality to be inferred, the inclusion of a socioeconomically disadvantaged sample of community adults, and a timely question that furthers understanding of mental health during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. However, the findings should be interpreted in light of the limitations of the study design. First, like other studies on the impact of the pandemic on mental health (e.g., Schmidt et al., 2021), the measures used to assess worry and media consumption during the COVID-19 pandemic in the current study consisted of single items, which limited our ability to evaluate the psychometric properties of these indices. The rationale for using single items was to reduce burden on participants. Given the limitations inherent in using single-item assessments, we purposefully repeated the measurement over multiple timepoints to provide some index of the reliability of the relationships under investigation. Still, additional research is required to ascertain the construct validity and reliability of the measures used. Second, data collection occurred between October and December 2020, which precludes our understanding of how these processes may have manifested earlier in the pandemic. Third, although previous studies with similar sample sizes have conducted CLPMs (e.g., Juengst et al., 2017), our sample size may have limited our power to detect important effects. Future research should aim to examine these associations in larger samples, particularly among individuals drawn from highly impacted communities, to replicate the findings. Findings could be bolstered by continued investigations into the interplay between media consumption and anxiety-based processes that may continue during, and perhaps, after the COVID-19 pandemic. Similarly, implications of the current are limited to individuals with similar characteristics to the sample used in this study. Generalizability of the findings would be bolstered if future studies examined the associations between media exposure and psychological distress using nationally representative samples, as recently done by other studies examining mental health trajectories in large, national samples (Li and Wang, 2020; Gong et al., 2021). Future research should also aim to examine trends in pandemic-related worry and media consumption to further understand the dynamic nature of these constructs over time. Such analyses are necessary to further elucidate the complex associations between media consumption and anxiety processes as they unfold during collective stressors.

Given the sustained nature of the COVID-19 pandemic, research is needed to identify proximal processes that are contributing to the spike in mental health problems. The goal of the current study was to examine the longitudinal associations between COVID-related news consumption and worry during a three-month period of the COVID-19 pandemic. Despite potential large-scale, public health benefits of media coverage during a pandemic, results revealed that individuals with COVID-related worry engaged in more future news coverage, which then increased their worry. Findings suggest that news consumption, which has increased during the COVID-19 pandemic, may be a useful intervention target during mental health treatment.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board, University of Delaware. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

NB contributed to conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, project administration, roles/writing – original draft, and writing – review and editing. SG contributed to roles/writing – original draft, and writing – review and editing. NS contributed to data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, and writing – review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported in part by the National Institute of Mental Health awarded to NB [1F31MH120936-01A1]. The Institute had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andrews, G., Hobbs, M. J., Borkovec, T. D., Beesdo, K., Craske, M. G., Heimberg, R. G., et al. (2010). Generalized worry disorder: a review of DSM-IV generalized anxiety disorder and options for DSM-V. Depress. Anxiety 27, 134–147. doi: 10.1002/da.20658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O. M., and Behar, E. (2004). “Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder,” in Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Advances in Research and Practice. eds. R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, and D. S. Mennin (New York: The Guilford Press), 77–108.

Google Scholar

Buheji, M., Jahrami, H., and Dhahi, A. S. (2020). Minimising stress exposure During pandemics similar to COVID-19. Int. J. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 10, 9–16. doi: 10.5923/j.ijpbs.20201001.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busso, D. S., McLaughlin, K. A., and Sheridan, M. A. (2014). Media exposure and sympathetic nervous system reactivity predict PTSD symptoms after the Boston Marathon bombings. Depress. Anxiety 31, 551–558. doi: 10.1002/da.22282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerron, C. (2020). Monthly Labor Review: September 2020. Delaware Department of Labor. https://labor.delaware.gov/divisions/oolmi/monthly-labor-review/ (Accessed January 12, 2020).

Google Scholar

Chen, C. Y., and Hong, R. Y. (2010). Intolerance of uncertainty moderates the relation between negative life events and anxiety. Pers. Individ. Differ. 49, 49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsass, H. J., Schlidkraut, J., and Stafford, M. C. (2014). Breaking news of social problems: examining media consumption and student beliefs about school shootings. Criminol. Crim. Just. L Soc. 15:31.

Google Scholar

Esbjørn, B. H., Lønfeldt, N. N., Nielsen, S. K., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Sømhovd, M. J., and Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2015). Meta-worry, worry, and anxiety in children and adolescents: relationships and interactions. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 44, 145–156. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.873980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfin, D. R., Silver, R. C., and Holman, E. A. (2020). The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 39, 355–357. doi: 10.1037/hea0000875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, S., Li, L. Z., and Wang, S. (2021). Youth mental health before and after the control of the coronavirus disease 2019: A nationally representative cohort study of Chinese college students. J. Affect. Disord. Rep. 3. doi: 10.1016/j.jadr.2020.100066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y., Fraustino, J. D., and Liu, B. F. (2016). The scared, the outraged, and the anxious: how crisis emotions, involvement, and demographics predict publics’ conative coping. Int. J. Strateg. Commun. 10, 289–308. doi: 10.1080/1553118X.2016.1160401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenness, J. L., Jager‐Hyman, S., Heleniak, C., Beck, A. T., Sheridan, M. A., McLaughlin, K. A., et al. (2016). Catastrophizing, rumination, and reappraisal prospectively predict adolescent PTSD symptom onset following a terrorist attack. Depress. Anxiety, 33, 1039–1047. doi: 10.1002/da.22548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juengst, S. B., Myrga, J. M., Fann, J. R., and Wagner, A. K. (2017). Cross-lagged panel analysis of depression and Behavioral dysfunction in the first year After moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 29, 260–266. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16100217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jungmann, S. M., and Witthöft, M. (2020). Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: Which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J. Anxiety Disord. 73:102239. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeBourgeois, M. K., Hale, L., Chang, A. M., Akacem, L. D., Montgomery-Downs, H. E., and Buxton, O. M. (2017). Digital media and sleep in childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics 140, S92–S96. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C., and Liu, Y. (2020). Media exposure and anxiety during COVID-19: The mediation effect of media vicarious traumatization. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health, 17:4720. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17134720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L. Z., and Wang, S. (2020). Prevalence and predictors of general psychiatric disorders and loneliness during COVID-19 in the United Kingdom. Psychiatry Res. 291, doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 455–468. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90132-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNaughton-cassill, M. E. (2001). The news media and psychological distress. Anxiety Stress Coping 14, 193–211. doi: 10.1080/10615800108248354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L., and Muthén, B. (2017). Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Newman, M. G., Llera, S. J., Erickson, T. M., Przeworski, A., and Castonguay, L. G. (2013). Worry and generalized anxiety disorder: a review and theoretical synthesis of evidence on nature, etiology, mechanisms, and treatment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 275–297. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nusslock, R., and Miller, G. E. (2016). Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health Across the lifespan: A Neuroimmune network hypothesis. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, L., Lannin, D. G., Hynes, K., and Yazedjian, A. (2020). Exploring social media rumination: associations with bullying, cyberbullying, and distress. J. Interpers. Violence, (2020). doi: 10.1177/0886260520946826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riehm, K. E., Holingue, C., Kalb, L. G., Bennett, D., Kapteyn, A., Jiang, Q., et al. (2020). Associations Between media exposure and mental distress Among U.S. adults at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. J. Prev. Med. 59, 630–638. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2020.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozenfeld, Y., Beam, J., Maier, H., Haggerson, W., Boudreau, K., Carlson, J., et al. (2020). A model of disparities: risk factors associated with COVID-19 infection. Int. J. Equity Health 19:126. doi: 10.1186/s12939-020-01242-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudenstine, S., McNeal, K., Schulder, T., Ettman, C. K., Hernandez, M., Gvozdieva, K., et al. (2021). Depression and anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic in an urban, low-income public university sample. J. Trauma. Stress. 34, 12–22. doi: 10.1002/jts.22600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryum, T., Kennair, L. E. O., Hjemdal, O., Hagen, R., Halvorsen, J. Ø., and Solem, S. (2017). Worry and metacognitions as predictors of anxiety symptoms: a prospective study. Front. Psychol. 8, 924. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, A., Brose, A., Kramer, A. C., Schmiedek, F., Witthöft, M., Neubauer, A. B., et al. (2021). Dynamic relations among COVID-19-related media exposure and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Health. 1–15. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2021.1912345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C., Alden, L. E., and Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cogn. Ther. Res. 24, 671–688. doi: 10.1023/A:1005587311498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabahang, R., Aruguete, M., and McCutcheon, L. (2020). Online health information utilization and online news exposure as predictor of COVID19 anxiety. N. Am. J. Psychol. 22, 469–482.

Google Scholar

Szabo, A., and Hopkinson, K. L. (2007). Negative psychological effects of watching the news in the television: relaxation or another intervention may be needed to buffer them! Int. J. Behav. Med. 14, 57–62. doi: 10.1007/BF03004169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. R., Garfin, D. R., Holman, E. A., and Silver, R. C. (2017). Distress, worry, and functioning following a Global Health crisis: A National Study of Americans’ responses to Ebola. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 5, 513–521. doi: 10.1177/2167702617692030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. R., Jones, N. M., Holman, E. A., and Silver, R. C. (2019). Media exposure to mass violence events can fuel a cycle of distress. Sci. Adv. 5. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav3502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., et al. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witthöft, M., Freitag, I., Nußbaum, C., Bräscher, A. K., Jasper, F., Bailer, J., et al. (2018). On the origin of worries about modern health hazards: experimental evidence for a conjoint influence of media reports and personality traits. Psychol. Health 33, 361–380. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1357814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2020). Mental health and psychosocial considerations during the COVID-19 outbreak, 18 March 2020. Institutional Repository of Information Sharing. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/331490 (Accessed January 12, 2020).

Google Scholar

Zebb, B. J., and Beck, J. G. (1998). Worry versus anxiety: is there really a difference? Behav. Modif. 22, 45–61. doi: 10.1177/01454455980221003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Kong, Y., and Chang, H. (2015). Media use and health behavior in H1N1 flu crisis: The mediating role of perceived knowledge and fear. Atl. J. Commun. 23, 67–80. doi: 10.1080/15456870.2015.1013101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stress, anxiety, coping, COVID-19, worry

Citation: Bounoua N, Goodling S and Sadeh N (2021) Cross-Lagged Analysis of COVID-19-Related Worry and Media Consumption in a Socioeconomically Disadvantaged Sample of Community Adults. Front. Psychol. 12:728629. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.728629

Received: 21 June 2021; Accepted: 12 November 2021;
Published: 08 December 2021.

Edited by:

Alessandra Maria Passarotti, University of Illinois at Chicago, United States

Reviewed by:

Siamak Khodarahimi, Islamic Azad University, Iran
Zixin Lambert Li, Stanford University, United States
Yunier Broche-Pérez, Universidad Central Marta Abreu de Las Villas, Cuba

Copyright © 2021 Bounoua, Goodling and Sadeh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nadia Bounoua, nbounoua@udel.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.