- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hung Hom, Hong Kong
Introduction: Effective adolescent learning programmes can positively influence adolescent development and curb risky behaviour. By immersing learners in an experience, experiential learning motivates learners to reflect on the experience to transform and create new skills, attitudes and ways of thinking. However, evidence of its effectiveness in learning programs facilitating positive youth development is still lacking. The objective of this study is to (a) identify the effect of adolescent learning programmes on prosocial behaviour, empathy and subjective well-being, (b) compare the effectiveness of experiential learning programmes and non-experiential learning programmes on improving these three outcomes, and (c) evaluating the effects of age on the outcomes of adolescent learning programmes.
Methods: This study was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Randomised controlled trials of learning programmes for typically developing adolescents aged 8–25 in the past 15 years were identified, and assessed for quality with the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDRO) scale. One thousand ninety-six records were screened with the inclusion and exclusion criteria, and 20 studies were adopted for this meta-analysis. The standardised mean difference and 95% confidence interval (CI) of the effect of experiential learning program on empathy, prosocial behaviour, and subjective well-being were examined. Sub-group analysis based on age was conducted to examine the effects of experiential learning on adolescents in different stages of life.
Results: Experiential learning programmes were more effective than non-experiential learning programmes in improving empathy [d = 0.65 (0.07, 1.23)] and subjective well-being [d = 0.46 (0.33, 0.59)]. The effect sizes of the three outcomes in non-experiential learning programmes were non-significant. Studies conducted on older adolescents had the most significant improvements in the three outcomes.
Conclusions: Results suggest the broader application of experiential learning in adolescent learning programmes for older adolescents in the future to promote positive youth development.
Introduction
Adolescence is a developmental period characterised by complex biological, psychological, social, emotional, sexual, and cognitive changes. This maturation offers an opportunity to foster a healthy and happy lifestyle as they transition to adulthood (Compas and Millstein, 1993). For example, their developing social cognition allows adolescents to grasp the complexities of social situations beyond simple rules, thus making room for new modes of prosocial behaviour with family members, peers, and the community (Fuligni, 2019). On the other hand, this period also leaves them vulnerable to developing problematic behaviours such as risky sexual behaviour, substance abuse, violence, addiction, and antisocial behaviours (Ciocanel et al., 2017). In recent years, though there is a drop in some adolescent risk behaviours, such as drug abuse, there is a worrying rise in internet and gaming addiction and risky sexual behaviour (Shek and Yu, 2011; Cheung and Cheung, 2019; Leung and Lin, 2019).
Adolescent prosociality, such as making small contributions to other members of society, promotes their personal social acceptance and integration for long-term functioning during adulthood (Fuligni, 2019). The transition of adolescents into thriving young adults raises educational and occupational prospects which enhances economic productivity (Fergusson et al., 2007). Addressing unique challenges during adolescence provides substantial personal benefits to health and well-being and also reduces economic costs for families and communities (Ciocanel et al., 2017). Positive youth development (PYD) is an ideology that aims to guide and empower youngsters by nurturing their self-efficacy, positive self-identity, psychosocial competence, and sense of belonging. Through providing adolescents with opportunities and resources to develop their strengths and form meaningful social networks, positive behaviour and the development of a prosocial and well-adjusted adult is promoted (Duncan et al., 2007). There are many examples of PYD programmes worldwide. Literature review concerning the outcomes of PYD programmes were done in the past with promising results (Catalano et al., 2004). Extensive research has been conducted by Shek and colleagues on the learning programme “Positive Adolescent Training through Holistic Social Programs” (PATHS) (Shek et al., 2010; Shek and Yu, 2011). Participants in PATHS displayed a lower level of substance abuse and delinquent behaviour than the control group (Shek and Yu, 2011) and also enabled development of positive interpersonal relationships, self-esteem, and sense of purpose (Shek et al., 2010). Other examples of PYD programmes include the Quantum Opportunities Program, Big Brother Big Sisters, Project K and the Summer Training and Education Program (Ciocanel et al., 2017).
In recent years, experiential learning (EXL) has been gaining popularity for its ability to engage students in active learning, translate classroom learning into real-world scenarios, and address community needs (Kruger et al., 2015). Currently, there is no single unanimous definition for the term “experiential learning” among researchers, but Kolb's Theory of Experiential Learning, which views learning as a process of creating knowledge through the transformation of experience (Kolb, 2014) is one of the most renowned theory supporting EXL due to his concrete theoretical base built upon the works of others. Theory of Experiential Learning states that for learning to occur, experience must first be grasped and then transformed through reflection and application. It involves four components that occur in a cyclical process: Concrete Experience (CE) is the opportunity for an experience, Reflective Observation (RO) makes sense of, breaks down, and transforms the experience through reflection, Abstract Conceptualisation (AC) is the formation of theoretical knowledge from which new behaviours and thinking emerge, and Active Experimentation (AE) is the practical application of new concepts (Kolb and Kolb, 2005; Alkan, 2016). This theory is built on the works of John Dewy, Kurt Lewin, Kurt Hahn, Jean Piaget, and so on (Dewey, 1986; James, 1990; Schein, 1996; Miettinen, 2000). While the works of Joplin (1981), Jarvis (2006) and Dean (1993) are alternative theories developed, Kolb's definition remains more inclusive of different modes of learning. For the purpose of this review, experiential learning programmes are thereby defined as learning programmes that include all four components of Kolb's learning cycle.
EXL is an effective form of learning as it engages the whole being through connecting the senses, intellect, and feelings during the learning process, which improves retention of information (Kolb and Kolb, 2005). Literature also points to EXL's effectiveness in improving the development of critical thinking (Lisko and O'dell, 2010) and personal insight (Burch et al., 2016). Through action-reflection and experience-abstraction, experiential learning is a process that facilitates learners to transform and create knowledge, skills, attitudes, and ways of thinking; thus, accommodate to various learning styles of individual learners by integrating the experience, perception, cognition, and behaviour (Lewis and Williams, 1994; Kolb and Kolb, 2009; Kolb, 2014). There is increasing inclusion of EXL concepts in learning programmes. For example, the Youth ImpACT Award is an example of an EXL programme that aims to promote PYD. This programme fosters participants' awareness of social issues through community engagement activities, whereby participants meet with community dwellers, explore the problems they face, and devise solutions using design thinking and innovative skills (Youth Impact Award, 2020).
Although literature asserts EXL's effectiveness in learning and skill development (Biers et al., 2006), there is a paucity of literature that specifically evaluates whether EXL programmes enhance PYD outcomes in adolescents. Moreover, what essentially constitutes an effective PYD programme is still obscure since there is no consensus about what components should be present in terms of programme design and targeted outcomes. For instance, the means of delivery, engagement methods, contexts, participants, and formats of the programmes (Brooks-Gunn and Roth, 2014; Tolan, 2016; Curran and Wexler, 2017).
Empathy, prosocial behaviour, and subjective well-being are key factors that influence healthy adolescent development and cultivate a harmonious society (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987; Silke et al., 2018). A wealth of literature suggests that empathy and prosocial behaviour play a key role in healthy social functioning and are particularly relevant in enhancing connectedness, cooperation, and understanding among people (Silke et al., 2018). For well-being, research shows that it is a reliable predictor of health and long-term positive adjustment (Gómez-López et al., 2019). As evidence suggests prosocial behaviour, empathy, and well-being are salient factors in facilitating PYD, they were chosen as the outcomes in this review. Prosocial behaviour is an umbrella term used to describe actions performed to enhance the welfare of others (Weinstein and Ryan, 2010; Spinrad and Eisenberg, 2017). It includes activities such as sharing, helping, caregiving, donating, volunteering, and acts of kindness. Empathy is the ability to apprehend others' emotional state (cognitive empathy) or the ability to share emotional experiences of others (affective empathy) (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987; Silke et al., 2018). Well-being is a multidimensional construct that includes psychological states such as positive affect or happiness, low negative affect or depression, and life-satisfaction (Curry et al., 2018). It also includes eudaimonic factors such as positive life functioning, self-actualisation, self-esteem, quality relationships, and purpose in life (Dodge et al., 2012; Moreira et al., 2015). Well-being will be referred to as “subjective well-being” in this review since all well-being outcomes were self-reported. The terminology “adolescent” traditionally refers to people under 19 years old (Curtis, 2015); however, recent literature suggests that the delayed transition to life stages such as education completion, career attainment, marriage, and parenthood expanded the length of adolescence into the mid-twenties (Mandarino, 2014; Sawyer et al., 2018). To increase the inclusiveness of this review, adolescents between the age of 8 to 25 will be explored.
The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to synthesise and categorise randomised controlled trials of adolescent learning programmes published between 2005 and 2020 into EXL programmes (those that incorporated the four elements of Kolb's Theory of Experiential Learning:) and non-experiential learning (NEXL) programmes (those that did not incorporate all four elements) and compare their effects on three PYD outcomes: prosocial behaviour, empathy, and subjective well-being. The time frame was selected as a comprehensive review on PYD programs was done on the years before (Catalano et al., 2004). The research objectives are (a) identifying the effect of adolescent EXL and NEXL programmes on prosocial behaviour, empathy and subjective well-being, (b) comparing the effectiveness of EXL and NEXL programmes on improving these three outcomes, and (c) evaluating the effects of age on the outcomes of adolescent learning programmes.
Methods
Guidelines
This study was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., 2020). This guideline has been widely accepted and endorsed in the academic community to facilitate accurate reporting in systematic reviews (Page and Moher, 2017). It serve as a standardised method to provide transparency to the full process behind the study, thus making this review replicable and hold it up to an internationally approved standard. The PRISMA 2020 checklist is included in the Supplementary Material.
Identification of Studies
To identify suitable experimental studies, searches were conducted in 2020 on scientific databases: Wiley Online Library, ScienceDirect, PsycINFO, Taylor and Francis Online, SAGE Journals, and Springer LINK. A search was also conducted using Google Scholar. The search terms for interventions and outcomes were as follows.
This search identified 985 articles, screened and categorised by four reviewers working independently unless a discussion is warranted. To this, we added 111 articles by following references in journal articles and checking systematic reviews. This initial set of 1,096 articles was screened by reading the titles and abstracts, and 906 articles were excluded. These articles had unrelated interventions or did not measure target outcomes. The remaining 190 potentially eligible articles were then assessed by full-text screening. Within, 83 articles were excluded for not meeting the inclusion criteria. The remaining 107 articles were read in full and rated according to the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) Scale. Articles with a PEDro score of less than six were excluded. Articles that did not provide data such as mean, SD, and sample size, and convertable statistics to Cohen's d were also excluded (Bird, 2014; Nelson et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2018). By the end, 20 articles were left for the final systematic review. All four reviewers reviewed the full texts to ensure its eligibility. Please see Figure 1 for the flow diagram that summarises the literature search process.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Studies were included in the review if they met the following criteria:
(a) Articles must focus on delivering learning programmes for adolescents between 8 and 25 years of age (or a mean sample age within this range).
(b) Articles must focus on intervention outcomes of empathy, prosocial behaviour, or subjective well-being.
(c) Articles must focus on the assessment of typically developing adolescents from non-clinical samples.
(d) Articles must report on randomised controlled trials, be peer-reviewed, written in the English language, and published in or after 2005.
(e) Articles failing to meet the above criteria (a–d) are included if effects for relevant comparison groups or sub-sample analyses were reported separately.
Appraisal of Study Quality and Data Extraction
Potentially eligible articles were scored individually by four reviewers with the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) Scale (Physiotherapy Evidence Database, 2020a). PEDro is designed to rate the validity of randomised controlled trials through critical appraisal (Moseley et al., 2019). The reviewers collaborated to assess and score the articles according to PEDro criteria. The items include (1) specified eligibility criteria, (2) randomisation of subjects, (3) concealed allocation, (4) similarity at baseline, blinding of subjects, (5) blinding of therapists, (6) blinding of assessors, (7) measures of at least one key outcome obtained from more than 85% of the subjects initially allocated to groups, (8) intention to treat, (9) results of between-group statistical comparisons reported for at least one key outcome, (10) and (11) point measures and measures of variability for at least one key outcome provided. Item 1 (specified eligibility criteria) is not used to calculate the PEDro score. For items 2–11, one point is given for each item when a criterion is clearly satisfied. The possible range of score is 0–10. Only papers with a score of six or above were included in the final systematic review as the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (2020b) states that scores between six and ten are moderate to high quality. All studies were also randomised controlled trials (RCT) because this type of experiment reduces bias and allows for a better examination of cause and effect due to having a control group and random subject allocation (National Institute for Health Care Excellence, 2020).
Data Analysis
Standardised mean differences (Cohen's d) were computed from reported inferential or descriptive statistics using the Campbell Collaboration Standardised mean difference Calculator (Wilson, 2001) or extracted from articles when provided. Standardised mean differences were coded as positive values to suggest an increase in prosocial behaviour, empathy, or subjective well-being. Standardised mean differences were reverse coded for the well-being outcomes: depression, depressive symptoms and anxiety. The value of Cohen's d (later displayed as d) 0.20 indicates a small, 0.50 a medium, and 0.80 a large standardised mean difference (Cohen, 1988). When a study had multiple measures for the same outcome, an overall standardised mean difference was calculated by pooling the individual standardised mean differences. The pooled standardised mean differences were calculated with the calculator Meta Essentials (Suurmond et al., 2017). For studies with multiple time-points, we calculated the outcome closest to the time of intervention.
Results
Study Quality Assessment
The average PEDro score of articles in this review is 6.65 which reflects of moderate-to-high quality articles. The highest score of 8 was obtained in four studies, a score of 7 was obtained in five studies, and a score of 6 was obtained in eleven studies. Please refer to Table 1 for individual scores for each article.
Study Characteristics
Design
Presented in Table 2 are the characteristics of the 20 studies included in this review. There were ten studies on EXL and ten studies on NEXL interventions. The sample size ranged from 43 to 4,516. All ten of the EXL programmes were carried out in developed countries, such as the United States, Canada, the United Kingdom, and Greece. Eight NEXL programmes were carried out in developed western countries such as the United States, Canada, and the United Kingdom, while the remaining two were conducted in Tanzania and Jordan. Seven studies compared the intervention group with a control group using a standard school curriculum, twelve used an alternative intervention, and three used no intervention or wait-list as the control. Please see Appendix A for more details of the studies.
Participants
The total number of randomised participants was 10,761. There were 2,351 participants in EXL programmes (intervention n = 1,241; control n = 1,110) and 8,410 participants in NEXL programmes (intervention n = 4,363; control n = 4,047). The subjects were categorised into three age groups: primary school-age (ages 8–12), secondary school-age (ages 12–18), and university-age (ages 18–25). The EXL sample consisted of five studies with primary school-age subjects, one with secondary school-age subjects, and four with university-age subjects. The NEXL sample consisted of five studies with primary school-age subjects, four with secondary school-age subjects, and one with university-age subjects. The average proportion of male subjects included is 44.58% in EXL programmes and 49.17% in NEXL programmes. Participants from eighteen studies were recruited from educational institutions, while participants from two studies were recruited from the community.
Intervention
Programmes were categorised into EXL and NEXL programmes by reviewers based on the criteria as described in the introduction. Five of the ten EXL programmes reported they were based on the Theory of Experiential Learning, while the other five were categorised by reviewers. Both types of programmes varied in programme duration, structure, and number and demographic of participants. The activities in the EXL programmes vary widely. For example, they are based on community service (Deane et al., 2017), simulation of physical disabilities (Henry et al., 2011) and classroom games and discussion sessions about understanding one's emotions and social skills (O'Hare et al., 2015). The activities in NEXL programmes are more similar in nature, as most are classroom games and discussions (Horowitz et al., 2007; Humphrey et al., 2016; Connolly et al., 2018). Please see Appendix A for more information on the learning programmes.
Outcome Measures
Outcome measures related to prosocial behaviour, empathy, and subjective well-being were selected to be included in the systematic review. A majority of studies used self-reported measures while some used third-party measures, such as the Teacher Observation of Classroom Adaptation–Checklist (TOCA-C) (Samuels et al., 2016) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (Prosocial Behaviour Subscale) (Humphrey et al., 2016).
Self-reported measures and third-party measures were used to measure prosocial behaviour. The Prosocial Subscale of Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used in four studies. Self-reported and third-party measures were used to measure empathy. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was used in two studies, while all other measures were different in each study. All measures on subjective well-being were self-reported. There were three measures for quality of life and life satisfaction, six measures for self-esteem and self-efficacy, and ten measures for mood and affect related items. Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) and the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) were each used in two studies. In total, 14 articles used self-reported measures only, 2 used third-party measures only, and 4 used both self-reported and third-party measures.
Intervention Effects
Six separate main effects analyses were conducted for EXL and NEXL programmes with respect to the three outcome categories. Please refer to Figures 2, 3 for the forest plots.
Figure 2. Forest plot showing the standardised mean difference of experiential learning interventions.
Figure 3. Forest plot showing the standardised mean difference of non-experiential learning interventions.
Experiential Learning Programmes
Empathy
Based on the empathy outcome measures reported in four studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.65, 95% CI (0.07, 1.23). This represents a moderately large effect concerning the observed increase in empathy outcomes associated with the participation in EXL programmes.
Prosocial Behaviour
Based on the prosocial behaviour outcome measures reported in four studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.14, 95% CI (−0.36, 0.63). This represents a non-significant effect concerning the observed increase in prosocial behaviour outcomes associated with the participation in EXL programmes.
Subjective Well-being
Based on the subjective well-being outcome measures reported in five studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.46, 95% CI (0.33, 0.59). This represents a moderate effect concerning the observed increase in subjective well-being outcomes associated with the participation in EXL programmes.
Non-experiential Learning Programmes
Empathy
Based on the empathy outcome measures reported in four studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.02, 95% CI (−0.3, 0.34). This represents non-significant effect concerning the empathy outcomes associated with the participation in NEXL programmes.
Prosocial Behaviour
Based on the prosocial behaviour outcome measures reported in six studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.06, 95% CI (−0.06, 0.19). This represents a non-significant effect concerning the observed increase in prosocial behaviour outcomes associated with the participation in NEXL programmes.
Subjective Well-being
Based on the subjective well-being outcome measures reported in seven studies, the overall standardised mean difference was d = 0.11, 95% CI (−0.04, 0.26). This represents a non-significant effect concerning the observed increase in subjective well-being outcomes associated with the participation in NEXL programmes.
Sub-group Analysis on Age
Main effects analyses were conducted for primary school-age, secondary school-age, and university-age participants with respect to the three outcome categories. Results indicate that all effects are non-significant, except for that of participants in the university-age range for empathy which d = 0.52, 95% CI (0.07, 0.97). Please refer to Table 3 for the results.
Sub-group Analysis on Control
Main effects analyses were conducted for controlled conditions with respect to the three outcome categories. Please refer to Table 4 for the results.
Discussion
Effectiveness of EXL and NEXL Programmes
Results demonstrated that EXL programmes were effective in improving adolescents' empathy, prosocial behaviour, and subjective well-being. The following section suggests possible reasons for this based on information provided by the studies and literature on EXL.
Kolb's Theory of Experiential Learning is constructed based on the works of leading psychologists Kurt Lewin, William James, Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky, and others who shaped the field of human development and learning (Kolb, 2014). Components in the Theory of Experiential Learning are also explicitly structured to aid the transformation of experience into new ways of thinking and behaviour. This model of learning is likely a decisive factor for programme effectiveness because the four components of learning (Concrete Experience, Reflective Observation, Abstract Conceptualisation, Active Experimentation) helped participants assimilate and comprehend new knowledge. On the other hand, NEXL programmes do not fit Kolb's framework. Still, most NEXL programmes are constructed based on other well-established theories, such as Social-Emotional Learning and Cognitive-Behavioural Theory.
One major difference noted between the two types of learning programme was the amount of facilitation in reflection. With regards to the Reflective Observation component of Kolb's Theory of Experiential Learning, all EXL programmes had teachers, instructors, mentors or group members facilitating participants' reflection of new experiences while all NEXL programmes did not. EXL programmes that had a facilitator guide members reflect on their experiences systematically produced the most significant outcomes in empathy (Lakin and Mahoney, 2006; Daeppen et al., 2012). Many EXL programmes conducted Reflective Observation through verbal discussions of feelings, thoughts, and experiences, such as what participants learnt and how they think or felt they performed in an activity (Lakin and Mahoney, 2006; Bosse et al., 2012; Daeppen et al., 2012; Cooke et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013; O'Hare et al., 2015; Samuels et al., 2016). Some programmes crafted opportunities for self-reflection during the activity (Deane et al., 2017), two programmes used written methods (Lakin and Mahoney, 2006; Karasimopoulou et al., 2012), and one programme used open-ended questions to guide participants' reflection (Henry et al., 2011).
In contrast, instructors in NEXL programmes did not guide their participants to reflect. Two factors, namely the mode of education and the large participant size, may have contributed to the lack of reflection. In half of the NEXL programmes, didactic lectures or classroom-based teaching was the prime mode of education (Horowitz et al., 2007; Stallard et al., 2014; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015; Humphrey et al., 2016; Berger et al., 2018), so the participants adopted a relatively passive mode of learning. On the other hand, half of the NEXL interventions were conducted as a class, while more than half of the EXL programmes were conducted in small groups. Whole classroom interventions may pose difficulties for teachers to guide students' reflection because of limited time and resources. Overall, results suggest that reflection is essential in improving programme outcomes.
In terms of Active Experimentation, the setting in which experimentation and application of new concepts was conducted differed between the two types of programmes. Participants in EXL programmes engaged with groups and individuals in the community (Lakin and Mahoney, 2006; Henry et al., 2011; Bosse et al., 2012; Daeppen et al., 2012; Karasimopoulou et al., 2012; Deane et al., 2017). Results show that EXL programmes that used service-learning events, community service projects, simulation, role-play, or adventure programmes to apply new concepts and skills were the most effective in increasing empathy, prosocial behaviour and subjective well-being outcomes.
In contrast, most NEXL programmes either lacked the Active Experimentation component, or if they did, limited participants' experimentation to the classroom without engaging the real-world (Horowitz et al., 2007; Stallard et al., 2014; Humphrey et al., 2016; Berger et al., 2018). This may limit effective learning as it prevents newly learnt concepts from being reconstructed in a meaningful and authentic context for deeper reflections (Jeyaraj, 2019). Ultimately, participants in most NEXL programmes had no significant improvements in prosocial behaviour and empathy. Notably, two NEXL programmes that provided community-immersive activities demonstrated significant improvements in empathy (Ferri et al., 2019), prosocial behaviour (Connolly et al., 2018) and subjective well-being outcomes (Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015). Overall, these results suggest that the opportunity for participants to apply concepts beyond the classroom may yield better programme outcomes. This is consistent with existing research as interacting with others facilitates the development of empathetic attitudes and emotional sensitivity (Carlo et al., 2015; Spinrad and Eisenberg, 2017).
The flexibility of programmes for participants to take ownership of activities differed significantly between NEXL and EXL programmes. Most EXL programmes were flexible, and teachers imposed few restrictions. Emphasis was placed on the learner's self-motivation to initiate, plan and implement a course of action to achieve goals (Lakin and Mahoney, 2006; Bosse et al., 2012; Daeppen et al., 2012; Karasimopoulou et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013,?; Samuels et al., 2016; Deane et al., 2017). Participants of these programmes had marked improvements in empathy, prosocial behaviour, and subjective well-being.
The NEXL programmes were often highly structured and teachers were required to follow programme guidelines to ensure programme fidelity (Horowitz et al., 2007; Stallard et al., 2014; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015; Humphrey et al., 2016; Berger et al., 2018; Connolly et al., 2018). The authoritarian role of teachers and overreliance on traditional didactic teaching methods may have halted participants' self-discovery and engagement, leading to poor outcomes in NEXL programmes. Notably, an NEXL intervention that enabled participants to negotiate activities with their mentors had significant intervention effects on participants' subjective well-being (Morton and Montgomery, 2012). Hence, flexible programmes that provide opportunities for participants to take control of their learning may yield better outcomes.
It is worth noting that despite the relative ineffectiveness of NEXL programmes in improving empathy, prosocial behaviour and subjective well-being, it cannot be overlooked that out of the three outcomes, subjective well-being improved the most, albeit an less than small standardised mean difference (d = 0.11). One possible explanation is that most NEXL programmes aimed to teach adolescents how to regulate and understand their emotions, manage their behaviours, and reduce anxiety or depression (Horowitz et al., 2007; Stallard et al., 2014; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015; Humphrey et al., 2016; Berger et al., 2018). These programmes were more self-oriented, focusing on understanding one's feelings and behaviours rather than helping others. Thus, improvement in subjective well-being was greater than empathy and prosocial behaviour in NEXL programmes.
Effects of Age
Results show that university-age subjects benefitted most from the learning programmes, with a higher overall standardised mean difference than that of other age groups in all three outcomes (empathy, d = 0.52; prosocial behaviour, d = 0.09; and well-being, d = 0.46).
One reason university-age adolescents had the most profound improvements after attending learning programmes may be that they are more cognitively developed. Major growth spurts in the brain during puberty cause the emergence of new neuronal pathways, pruning of existing neural networks, and significant development of the prefrontal cortex. Based on Piaget's Theory of Cognitive Development, these neuronal changes enable teens to perform formal operational skills such as abstract thinking and hypothetico-deductive reasoning, thus improving their ability to think critically about abstract concepts such as morality and free will (Weiten, 2013; Boyd and Bee, 2015). Moreover, increasingly challenging cognitive tasks demanded by higher education further develop metacognitive skills, facilitating conscious control of thought and reflective thinking (Boyd and Bee, 2015).
In contrast, primary school-age adolescents are closer to the concrete operational stage. In this stage, children can think logically, but can only apply logic to tangible objects and events (Weiten, 2013). They have difficulty thinking hypothetically and understanding things they cannot see or have not experienced (Boyd and Bee, 2015). The development of empathy and prosocial behaviour is highly linked to age. As children age and their theory of mind develops, their ability to self-regulate and differentiate themselves from others improves. This increases their ability to empathise with others and respond prosocially (Carlo et al., 2015). The results of this review are consistent with existing research as empathy and prosocial outcomes were the greatest for university-age, moderate for secondary school-age, and lowest for primary school-age participants.
Limitations and Implications
There are several limitations associated with this review. Firstly, as the interest of this review was to collect RCTs to ensure intervention reliability, the number of articles synthesised was limited. Only ten articles each were collated for NEXL and EXL, which may not be adequate to draw reliable conclusions on the overall effectiveness of these programmes. Secondly, the use of the PEDro scale to evaluate studies for internal validity may be too stringent. Since it is difficult to blind the administers, assessors and participants of the programs, many potential studies were screened out as a result of their low PEDro score. Thirdly, many factors contribute to the effectiveness of learning programmes beyond EXL or NEXL such as its activities, method of implementation, quality, and duration. Previous research also attests that participant characteristics such as age, gender, personality, experiences, and relationship with parents and peers also influence their empathetic responding, prosocial behaviour, and well-being (Lai et al., 2015; Silke et al., 2018). Hence, it would be inapt to attribute changes in outcomes to NEXL or EXL alone. Fourthly, this review looked at the immediate effects of learning programmes on the three outcomes, it is not clear what the long-term effects of these interventions may be.
The results of this review suggest that EXL programmes have a higher potential to improve adolescent empathy, prosocial behaviour and well-being than NEXL programmes. This encourages the broader application of EXL in learning programmes to enhance Positive Youth Development outcomes. Future learning programmes may incorporate components such as facilitated individual or small-group reflections, active experimentation in various settings, and flexibility on the learning programme along with promotion of self-motivation to potentially increase the effectiveness of the programmes to promote PYD outcomes. However, since most EXL interventions in this review were conducted on older adolescents, further RCTs of EXL programmes for adolescents between ages 12-18 is recommended to confirm the effects of EXL programmes. Furthermore, only three studies measured all three outcomes. More RCTs of learning programmes that measure empathy, prosocial behaviour and subjective well-being is recommended.
Conclusion
Adolescence is a critical time for implementing learning programmes that promote PYD. Moreover, the use of such learning programmes may reduce risky behaviours in youths that lead to adverse physical and psychosocial effects that carry on into adulthood. The results of this review support the use of EXL programmes in schools and communities to develop empathy and subjective well-being. More RCTs on EXL programmes for adolescents are needed to deepen our understanding of how they can help adolescents thrive.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Author Contributions
FL is responsible for supervising the process of paper writing. All authors contributed to this article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by Collaborative Research Fund by The Boys' and Girls' Clubs Association of Hong Kong, under the project Youth ImpACT Award: The Development of an Innovative Framework to Enhance Social Competency of Hong Kong Secondary Students (Project ID: P0031095, Program Code: ZH3M).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We appreciate the general support from the Collaborative Research Fund by The Boys' and Girls' Clubs Association of Hong Kong.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.709699/full#supplementary-material
References
Alkan, F. (2016). Experiential learning: Its effects on achievement and scientific process skills. J. Turk. Sci. Educ. 13, 15–26. doi: 10.12973/tused.10164a
*Berger, R., Benatov, J., Cuadros, R., VanNattan, J., and Gelkopf, M. (2018). Enhancing resiliency and promoting prosocial behavior among Tanzanian primary-school students: a school-based intervention. Transcultural Psychiatry 55, 821–845. doi: 10.1177/1363461518793749
Biers, K., Jensen, C., and Serfustini, E. (2006). Experiential learning: a process for teaching youth entrepreneurship. J. Youth Dev. 1, 58–64. doi: 10.5195/JYD.2006.386
Bird, J. M. (2014). Evaluating the effects of a strengths-based, professional development intervention on adolescents' academic, social, and emotional outcomes [Unpublished doctoral thesis]. University, of South Caroline, United States. Retrieved from: http://scholarcommons.sc.edu/etd/2790/utm_source=scholarcommons.sc.edu%2Fetd%2F2790&utm_medium=PDF&utm_campaign=PDFCoverPages (accessed December 1, 2019).
*Bosse, H. M., Schultz, J. H., Nickel, M., Lutz, T., Möltner, A., Jünger, J., et al. (2012). The effect of using standardised patients or peer role play on ratings of undergraduate communication training: a randomized controlled trial. Patient Educ. Counsel. 87, 300–306. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.10.007
Boyd, B., and Bee, H. L. (2015). Lifespan Development. 7th Edn. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Education.
Brooks-Gunn, J., and Roth, J. (2014). Invited commentary: promotion and prevention in youth development: two sides of the same coin? J Youth Adolesc. 43, 1004–1007. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0122-y
Burch, G., Giambatista, R. C., Batchelor, J., Hoover, J. D., Burch, J., Heller, N., et al. (2016). Do experiential learning pedagogies effect student learning? A meta-analysis of 40 years of research. Acad. Manage. Proc. 2016:16838. doi: 10.5465/ambpp.2016.127
Carlo, G., Padilla-Walker, L. M., and Nielson, M. G. (2015). Longitudinal bidirectional relations between adolescents' sympathy and prosocial behavior. Dev. Psychol. 51:1771. doi: 10.1037/dev0000056
Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A., Lonczak, H. S., and Hawkins, J. D. (2004). Positive youth development in the United States: research findings on evaluations of positive youth development programs. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 591, 98–124. doi: 10.1177/0002716203260102
Cheung, Y. W., and Cheung, N. W. (2019). Adolescent drug abuse in Hong Kong: prevalence, psychosocial correlates, and prevention. J. Adolesc. Health 64, S28–S33. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.09.016
Ciocanel, O., Power, K., Eriksen, A., and Gillings, K. (2017). Effectiveness of positive youth development interventions: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 483–504. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0555-6
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge Academic.
Compas, B. E., and Millstein, S. (1993). “Promoting positive mental health during adolescence,” in Promoting the Health of Adolescents: New Directions for the Twenty-First Century, eds S. G. Millstein, A. C. Petersen, and E. O. Nightingale (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 159–179.
*Connolly, P., Miller, S., Kee, F., Sloan, S., Gildea, A., McIntosh, E., et al. (2018). A cluster randomized controlled trial and evaluation and cost-effectiveness analysis of the Roots of Empathy schools-based programme for improving social and emotional well-being outcomes among 8-to 9-year-olds in Northern Ireland. Public Health Res. 6, 1–108. doi: 10.3310/phr06040
*Cooke, P. A., Tully, M. A., Cupples, M. E., Gilliland, A. E., and Gormley, G. J. (2013). A randomized control trial of experiential learning to promote physical activity. Educ. Primary Care 24, 427–435. doi: 10.1080/14739879.2013.11494213
Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., Hightower, A. D., Wyman, P. A., Parker, G. R., and Lotyczewski, B. S. (1991). Toward the development of a measure of perceived self-efficacy in children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 20, 169–178.
Curran, T., and Wexler, L. (2017). School-based positive youth development: a systematic review of the literature. J. School Health 87, 71–80. doi: 10.1111/josh.12467
Curry, O. S., Rowland, L. A., Van Lissa, C. J., Zlotowitz, S., McAlaney, J., and Whitehouse, H. (2018). Happy to help? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 76, 320–329 doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.02.014
*Daeppen, J. B., Fortini, C., Bertholet, N., Bonvin, R., Berney, A., Michaud, P. A., et al. (2012). Training medical students to conduct motivational interviewing: a randomized controlled trial. Patient Educ. Counsel. 87, 313–318. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.12.005
Dean, G. J. (1993). “Developing experiential learning activities for adult learners,” in American Association for Adult and Continuing Education National Conference, Dallas, TX, 18.
*Deane, K. L., Harr,é, N., Moore, J., and Courtney, M. G. (2017). The impact of the Project K youth development program on self-efficacy: a randomized controlled trial. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 516–537. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0463-9
Dodge, R., Daly, A. P., Huyton, J., and Sanders, L. D. (2012). The challenge of defining wellbeing. Int. J. Wellbeing 2, 222–235. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v2i3.4
Duncan, P. M., Garcia, A. C., Frankowski, B. L., Carey, P. A., Kallock, E. A., Dixon, R. D., et al. (2007). Inspiring healthy adolescent choices: a rationale for and guide to strength promotion in primary care. J. Adolesc. Health 41, 525–535. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.05.024
Eisenberg, N., and Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 101:91. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., and Ridder, E. M. (2007). Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 88, S14–S26. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.011
*Ferri, P., Rovesti, S., Padula, M. S., D'Amico, R., and Di Lorenzo, R. (2019). Effect of expert-patient teaching on empathy in nursing students: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manage. 12:457. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S208427
Fuligni, A. J. (2019). The need to contribute during adolescence. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 331–343. doi: 10.1177/1745691618805437
Gómez-López, M., Viejo, C., and Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2019). Psychological well-being during adolescence: stability and association with romantic relationships. Front. Psychol. 10:1772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01772
Guo, Q., Wu, R., and Li, X. (2018). Beneficial effects of pro-social behaviour on physical well-being in Chinese samples. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 21, 22–31. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12193
*Henry, B. W., Ozier, A. D., and Johnson, A. (2011). Empathetic responses and attitudes about older adults: how experience with the aging game measures up. Educ. Gerontol. 37, 924–941. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2010.495540
*Herrera, C., Grossman, J. B., Kauh, T. J., and McMaken, J. (2011). Mentoring in schools: an impact study of Big Brothers Big Sisters school-based mentoring. Child Dev. 82, 346–361. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01559.x
*Horowitz, J. L., Garber, J., Ciesla, J. A., Young, J. F., and Mufson, L. (2007). Prevention of depressive symptoms in adolescents: a randomized trial of cognitive-behavioral and interpersonal prevention programs. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75:693. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.693
*Humphrey, N., Barlow, A., Wigelsworth, M., Lendrum, A., Pert, K., Joyce, C., et al. (2016). A cluster randomized controlled trial of the promoting alternative thinking strategies (PATHS) curriculum. J. School Psychol. 58, 73–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2016.07.002
James, T. (1990). Kurt Hahn and the aims of education. J. Exp. Educ. 13, 6–13. doi: 10.1177/105382599001300101
Jarvis, P. (2006). Towards a Comprehensive Theory of Human Learning. Lifelong Learning and the Learning Society. London: Routledge.
Jeyaraj, J. S. (2019). Traditional learning vs. experiential learning. Am. Coll. J. English Lang. Liter. 9, 43–46. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3486129
Joplin, L. (1981). On defining experiential education. J. Exp. Educ. 4, 17–20. doi: 10.1177/105382598100400104
*Karasimopoulou, S., Derri, V., and Zervoudaki, E. (2012). Children's perceptions about their health-related quality of life: effects of a health education–social skills program. Health Educ. Res. 27, 780–793. doi: 10.1093/her/cys089
Kolb, A. Y., and Kolb, D. A. (2005). Learning styles and learning spaces: enhancing experiential learning in higher education. Acad. Manage. Learn. Educ. 4, 193–212. doi: 10.5465/amle.2005.17268566
Kolb, A. Y., and Kolb, D. A. (2009). Experiential learning theory: a dynamic, holistic approach to management learning, education and development. SAGE Handbook Manage. Learn. Educ. Dev. 42:68. doi: 10.4135/9780857021038.n3
Kolb, D. A. (2014). Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: FT Press.
*Kolić-Vehovec, S., Smojver-AŽić, S., Martinac Dorči,ć, T., and Rončević Zubković, B. (2020). Evaluation of serious game for changing students' behaviour in bullying situation. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 36, 323–334. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12402
Kruger, J. S., Kruger, D. J., and Suzuki, R. (2015). Assessing the effectiveness of experiential learning in a student-run free clinic. Pedagogy Health Promot. 1, 91–94. doi: 10.1177/2373379915575530
Lai, F., Siu, A., and Shek, D. (2015). Individual and social predictors of prosocial behavior among Chinese Adolescents in Hong Kong. Front. Pediatr. 3:39. doi: 10.3389/fped.2015.00039
*Lakin, R., and Mahoney, A. (2006). Empowering youth to change their world: Identifying key components of a community service program to promote positive development. J. School Psychol. 44, 513–531. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.06.001
Leung, H., and Lin, L. (2019). Adolescent sexual risk behavior in Hong Kong: prevalence, protective factors, and sex education programs. J. Adolesc. Health 64, S52–S58. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.12.007
Lewis, L. H., and Williams, C. J. (1994). Experiential learning: past and present. New Direct. Adult Continuing Educ. 1994, 5–16. doi: 10.1002/ace.36719946203
*Li, W. H., Chung, J. O., and Ho, E. K. (2013). Effectiveness of an adventure-based training programme in promoting the psychological well-being of primary schoolchildren. J. Health Psychol. 18, 1478–1492. doi: 10.1177/1359105312465102
Lisko, S. A., and O'dell, V. (2010). Integration of theory and practice: experiential learning theory and nursing education. Nurs. Educ. Perspect. 31, 106–108. doi: 10.1043/1536-5026-31.2.106
Mandarino, K. (2014). Transitional-age youths: barriers to accessing adult mental health services and the changing definition of adolescence. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 24, 462–474. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2013.835760
Miettinen, R. (2000). The concept of experiential learning and John Dewey's theory of reflective thought and action. Int. J. Lifelong Educ. 19, 54–72. doi: 10.1080/026013700293458
Moreira, P. A., Cloninger, C. R., Dinis, L., Sá, L., Oliveira, J. T., Dias, A., et al. (2015). Personality and well-being in adolescents. Front. Psychol. 5:1494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01494
*Morton, M. H., and Montgomery, P. (2012). Empowerment-based non-formal education for Arab youth: a pilot randomized trial. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 34, 417–425. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.11.013
Moseley, A. M., Elkins, M. R., Van der Wees, P. J., and Pinheiro, M. B. (2019). Using research to guide practice: the physiotherapy evidence database (PEDro). Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 24, 384–391. doi: 10.1016/j.bjpt.2019.11.002
National Institute for Health Care Excellence (2020). Glossary. Available online at: https://www.nice.org.uk/Glossary?letter=R (accessed October 17, 2020).
Nelson, S. K., Della Porta, M. D., Jacobs Bao, K., Lee, H. C., Choi, I., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2015). ‘It's up to you': experimentally manipulated autonomy support for prosocial behavior improves well-being in two cultures over six weeks. J. Positive Psychol. 10, 463–476. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.983959
*O'Hare, L., Biggart, A., Kerr, K., and Connolly, P. (2015). A randomized controlled trial evaluation of an after-school prosocial behavior program in an area of socioeconomic disadvantage. Element. School J. 116, 1–29. doi: 10.1086/683102
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J., Bossuyt, P., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T., and Mulrow, C. D. (2020). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. MetaArXiv. (2020) 1–36. doi: 10.31222/osf.io/v7gm2
Page, M. J., and Moher, D. (2017). Evaluations of the uptake and impact of the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) Statement and extensions: a scoping review. Syst. Rev. 6, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0663-8
Physiotherapy Evidence Database (2020a). PEDro Scale. Available online at: www.pedro.org.au/english/downloads/pedro-scale/ (accessed October 17, 2020).
Physiotherapy Evidence Database (2020b). PEDro Statistics. Available online at: https://www.pedro.org.au/english/downloads/pedro-statistics/ (accessed October 17, 2020).
*Samuels, W. E., Meers, L. L., and Normando, S. (2016). Improving upper elementary students' humane attitudes and prosocial behaviors through an in-class humane education program. Anthrozoös 29, 597–610. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2016.1228751
Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D., and Patton, G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2, 223–228. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1
Schein, E. H. (1996). Kurt Lewin's change theory in the field and in the classroom: notes toward a model of managed learning. Syst. Pract. 9, 27–47. doi: 10.1007/BF02173417
*Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., et al. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 51:52. doi: 10.1037/a0038454
Shek, D. T., Ng, C. S., and Tsui, P. F. (2010). Qualitative evaluation of the project PATHS: findings based on focus groups. Int. J. Disabil. Hum. Dev. 9, 307–313. doi: 10.1515/IJDHD.2010.046
Shek, D. T., and Yu, L. (2011). Prevention of adolescent problem behavior: longitudinal impact of the Project PATHS in Hong Kong. Sci. World J. 11:569069. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.33
Silke, C., Brady, B., Boylan, C., and Dolan, P. (2018). Factors influencing the development of empathy and pro-social behaviour among adolescents: a systematic review. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 94, 421–436. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.07.027
Spinrad, T. L., and Eisenberg, N. (2017). “Prosocial behavior and empathy-related responding: relations to children's well-being,” in The Happy Mind: Cognitive Contributions to Well-Being, eds M. Robinson and M. Eid (Cham: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-58763-9_18
*Stallard, P., Skryabina, E., Taylor, G., Phillips, R., Daniels, H., Anderson, R., et al. (2014). Classroom-based cognitive behaviour therapy (FRIENDS): a cluster randomized controlled trial to Prevent Anxiety in Children through Education in Schools (PACES). Lancet Psychiatry 1, 185–192. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70244-5
Suurmond, R., van Rhee, H., and Hak, T. (2017). Introduction, comparison, and validation of meta-essentials: a free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 8, 537–553. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1260
Tolan, P. (2016). Positive youth development interventions: advancing evaluation theory and practice. Appl. Dev. Sci. 20, 147–149. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2015.1014485
Weinstein, N., and Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps: autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 98:222. doi: 10.1037/a0016984
Weiten, W. (2013). Psychology: Themes and Variations. 9th Edn. Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis Effect Size Calculator. Available online at: https://campbellcollaboration.org/escalc/html/EffectSizeCalculator-SMD-main.php (accessed October 17, 2020).
Youth Impact Award (2020). Project Features. Available online at: https://yiaa.hk/en/content/project-features (accessed October 17, 2020).
*^References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the systematic review.
Keywords: experiential learning, prosocial behaviour, empathy, well-being, positive youth development, adolescence
Citation: Chan HH-K, Kwong HYC, Shu GLF, Ting CY and Lai FH-Y (2021) Effects of Experiential Learning Programmes on Adolescent Prosocial Behaviour, Empathy, and Subjective Well-being: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:709699. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.709699
Received: 14 May 2021; Accepted: 12 July 2021;
Published: 04 August 2021.
Edited by:
Ai Bo, University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee, United StatesReviewed by:
Wenhua Lu, City University of New York, United StatesYuerong Liu, Duke University, United States
Copyright © 2021 Chan, Kwong, Shu, Ting and Lai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Frank Ho-Yin Lai, frank.lai@northumbria.ac.uk